Context
Although genetic influences on bipolar disorder are well established, localization of genes that predispose to the illness has proven difficult. Given that genes predisposing to bipolar disorder may be transmitted without expression of the categorical clinical phenotype, a strategy for identifying risk genes is to identify and map quantitative intermediate phenotypes or endophenotypes.
Objective
To adjudicate neurocognitive endophenotypes for bipolar disorder.
Design
All participants underwent diagnostic interviews and comprehensive neurocognitive evaluations. Neurocognitive measures found to be heritable were entered into analyses designed to determine which test results are impaired in affected individuals, are sensitive to the genetic liability for the illness, and are genetically correlated with affection status.
Setting
Central valley of Costa Rica; Mexico City, Mexico; and San Antonio, Texas.
Participants
Seven hundred nine Latino individuals participated in the study. Of these, 660 were members of extended pedigrees with at least 2 siblings diagnosed as having bipolar disorder (n = 230). The remaining subjects were community control subjects drawn from each site who did not have a personal or family history of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia.
Main Outcome Measure
Neurocognitive test performance.
Results
Two of the 22 neurocognitive variables were not significantly heritable and were excluded from subsequent analyses. Patients with bipolar disorder were impaired on 6 cognitive measures compared with nonrelated healthy controls. Nonbipolar first-degree relatives were impaired on 5 of these, and the following 3 tests were genetically correlated with affection status: Digit Symbol Coding Task, Object Delayed Response Task, and immediate facial memory.
Conclusion
This large-scale extended pedigree study of cognitive functioning in bipolar disorder identifies measures of processing speed, working memory, and declarative (facial) memory as candidate endophenotypes for bipolar disorder.
Despite considerable evidence that the risk for bipolar disorder is inherited, the molecular genetic basis for this illness remains elusive. A recent genome-wide association analysis with more than 5000 cases and 6000 control subjects implicated 2 risk genes for the illness: ANK3 and CACNA1C.1 Because these genes regulate voltage-gated sodium and calcium channels, respectively, the findings suggest that ion channel dysfunction may play an important role in the pathophysiologic process of bipolar disorder. However, as noted by the authors, these genes have relatively small risk ratios, explaining little of the genetic contribution to the illness. Given the high heritability and familial relative risk of bipolar disorder, there is little doubt that additional genes are involved in the etiology of the illness. The identification of these genes is of paramount importance because they hold the potential for spurring novel treatments for this common and debilitating illness.
Given that genes predisposing to bipolar disorder may be transmitted without expression of the clinical phenotype, a strategy for identifying risk genes is the use of quantitative intermediate phenotypes or endophenotypes for the illness.2 A recent National Institute of Mental Health work group called for the widespread implementation of endophenotypic markers in the search for genes predisposing to bipolar disorder.3 However, relatively few endophenotypes have been proposed for the illness,4 and fewer still have been validated in large-scale studies.
Findings that asymptomatic individuals with bipolar disorder have neuropsychological impairments,5,6 that these deficits appear to be stable over time,7 and that unaffected first-degree relatives of bipolar probands have similar, although less pronounced, impairments8 suggest that neurocognitive measures may be candidate endophenotypes for the illness.9 The goal of the present study was to test this hypothesis.
Quantitative neurocognitive intermediate phenotypes can be assessed in affected and unaffected individuals, providing much greater statistical power to localize and identify disease-related genes than affection status alone.10,11 However, establishing a particular measure as an endophenotype requires that the trait be heritable, sensitive to affection status, and genetically correlated with the illness.2,12 Although neurocognitive endophenotypes are appropriate for linkage and association experimental designs, the establishment of a particular trait as an intermediate phenotype is most efficient with large extended pedigrees13 because these families provide a single sample in which all of the criteria for effective endophenotypes can be assessed simultaneously (eg, heritability, sensitivity to the illness, and genetic correlation).
In the present study, we conducted comprehensive neurocognitive assessments on large multigenerational pedigrees selected for sibling pairs concordant for bipolar I disorder or schizoaffective disorder, bipolar subtype, and community controls. The focus on multiplex families reduces the potential of spurious or nongenetic forms of the illness and increases the potential that identified intermediate phenotypes reflect genetic factors that influence the risk of bipolar disorder. To determine whether a neurocognitive measure is a candidate endophenotype for bipolar disorder, we document its heritability, demonstrate that patients and their unaffected first-degree relatives are impaired on the measure, and establish genetic correlation between the measure and affection status.
Seven hundred nine Latino individuals from the central valley of Costa Rica (328 individuals [46.3%]); Mexico City, Mexico (139 [19.6%]); and San Antonio, Texas (242 [34.1%]), participated in the study. Of these, 660 were members of 45 families (mean [SD] family size, 14.67 [10.56] members; range, 3-37 members) with at least 2 siblings diagnosed as having bipolar disorder. Probands were recruited through systematic screening of outpatient and inpatient facilities. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were the same across sites and required a previous diagnosis of bipolar I disorder in probands and at least 1 sibling with bipolar disorder or schizoaffective disorder, bipolar type. Affected individuals were excluded if they did not provide written consent to contact family members or had a history of mental retardation, a neurological disorder, or severe head trauma. Once an affected sibling pair provided informed consent, attempts were made to recruit all first-, second-, and third-degree relatives. Inclusion and exclusion criteria were identical for family members, with the exception of requiring a personal history of bipolar disorder. The remaining subjects were ethnically matched community controls drawn in equal numbers from each site who did not have a personal or family history (first-degree relatives) of bipolar disorder or schizophrenia. Community controls were recruited through advertisements in local newspapers and via fliers placed in medical clinics and had the same inclusion and exclusion criteria as family members.
All subjects provided written informed consent on forms in the language of their choice (Spanish or English) approved by the review boards of all participating study sites.
All participants, regardless of diagnostic or family status, received the Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies14 and the Family Interview for Genetic Studies.15 Interviews were conducted by psychiatrists with established reliability (κ = 0.85). Final diagnoses were determined through a best estimation process16 in which 2 psychiatrists review available records, arrive at independent DSM-IV diagnoses, and reach a consensus. If consensus could not be reached, a third best estimator reviewed the case independently (this occurred once in this sample).
Neurocognitive assessment
Each participant received the South Texas Assessment of Neurocognition, a 90-minute neuropsychological evaluation consisting of standard and computerized measures.13 Tests were selected on evidence of heritability, sensitivity to bipolar disorder, and minimization of the effects of language or the availability of parallel English and Spanish forms. Instructions for computerized neuropsychological tests were translated to Spanish by bilingual psychologists and translated back into English by professional translators. Other tests (eg, the California Verbal Learning Test [CVLT]) have published English17 and Spanish18 editions. All subjects received the same battery of tests in the language of their choice and in a fixed order and were allowed breaks as needed.
The South Texas Assessment of Neurocognition battery included 16 tests with 21 separate measures (Table 1). The computerized Digit Symbol Coding Task required subjects to indicate, via button press, if a centrally presented digit-symbol pair was identical to 1 of the 9 digit-symbol pairs in the reference list at the top of the screen.13 During the identical pairs version of the Continuous Performance Test, subjects viewed a series of numbers presented briefly on the screen, pressing the space bar whenever the same number appeared twice in a row.28 During the manual Stroop Test, subjects pressed arrow keys that faced in the same direction (congruent) or in the opposite direction (incongruent) as an arrow on the screen.21 The Spatial Delayed Response Task involved subjects remembering where a set of circles appeared and indicating, via button press, if a single circle was presented in the same location.23 During the Object Delayed Response Task, subjects remembered 3 abstract shapes (presented serially) and, after a delay, selected 1 of 4 shapes not previously presented. The Penn Face Memory Test involved the presentation of 20 target faces and then immediate and delayed recognition (indicated by button press) of these faces mixed with an equal number of foils.25 During the Penn Conditional Exclusion Test, subjects chose which stimulus of 4 did not belong, based on inferred rules, and were provided feedback.27 In addition, the variables derived from the South Texas Assessment of Neurocognition battery were entered into a factor analysis (varimax rotation), and the first principal component was used to provide a measure of general cognitive functioning.
Quantitative genetic analyses
All analyses were conducted with the Sequential Oligogenic Linkage Analysis Routines (SOLAR) computer program,29 which uses maximum likelihood variance decomposition methods to determine the relative importance of genetic and environmental influences on a measure by modeling the covariance among family members as a function of genetic proximity (kinship). The polygenic model assumes that a trait is the result of multiple gene loci at unidentified positions in the genome. In the case of a dichotomous trait (eg, affection status), these loci are thought to contribute to an underlying illness liability that results in the disorder if a threshold is reached. To ensure that neuropsychological traits conform to the assumptions of normality, an inverse normal transformation was applied.
Heritability (h2) represents the portion of the phenotypic variance accounted for by the total additive genetic variance (h2 = σ2g/σ2p). Genetic variance (σ2) is the portion of phenotypic variance due to pedigree relationships rather than environmental factors or error. Indices with stronger covariance between genetically more rather than between genetically less similar individuals have higher heritability. Within the SOLAR program, this is assessed by contrasting the observed covariance matrices for a trait with the covariance matrix predicted by kinship. Heritability analyses were conducted twice, without demographic covariates and again with age, sex, age × sex interaction, age2, age2 × sex interaction, education level (in years), and location of assessment included as covariates in the model. Although the second h2 estimates are critical for the current investigation, the raw estimates were included for comparability with published data. Only neuropsychological variables with significant heritability, corrected for multiple comparison at a 5% false discovery rate (FDR),30 were included in subsequent analyses.
Sensitivity to liability for bipolar disorder
Heritable neuropsychological variables were examined to determine which measures differentiate individuals with bipolar disorder from unrelated, unaffected individuals. Within the SOLAR program, these analyses were conducted by including a variable that identified individuals in specific liability groups (eg, bipolar vs unrelated) and testing whether the mean of these groups differed within the context of the known pedigree structure and covariates (χ2 test). Evidence that a particular neurocognitive measure was sensitive to bipolar disorder was provided if the grouping variable explained a significant proportion of trait variance at 5% FDR. Effect size estimates (standardized mean differences) were derived using the following equation:
Effect Size = [(4 × χ2)/(n − χ2)] − 1/2,
where n is the number of individuals included in the analyses. Given the nonindependence of participants, this effect size estimate is conservative.
Neurocognitive measures sensitive to bipolar disorder were examined in analogous analyses in unaffected (nonbipolar) first-degree relatives to confirm that the candidate endophenotype reflected underlying genetic vulnerability. An individual's liability for bipolar disorder was defined as the shortest genetic distance to an affected individual.
Although differences in neurocognitive measures between unaffected first-degree relatives and controls that rule out effects secondary to disease in the affected individuals are indicative of a genetic vulnerability, they could also be due to environmental factors shared by individuals with bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives. To determine whether neurocognitive performance and liability for bipolar disorder have common genetic or environmental influences, mixed discrete/continuous trait bivariate analyses were conducted. These analyses decompose phenotypic correlations into genetic (ρg) and environmental (ρe) correlations between the 2 traits. If the genetic correlation is significantly different from 0, then the traits are considered to be influenced by the same genetic factors (eg, ≥1 genes influence both traits, or pleiotropy31). If the environmental correlation is significantly different from 0, then the traits are considered to be influenced by the same environmental factors (eg, education level). The significance (5% FDR) of these correlations was tested by comparing the natural log (ln) likelihood for 2 restricted models (with ρg or ρe constrained to equal 0.0) against the ln likelihood for the model in which these parameters were estimated.
Finally, we performed multivariate analyses on those neurocognitive traits genetically correlated with bipolar disorder to determine whether these traits represent independent risk factors.
Of the 660 subjects from extended pedigrees, 230 had a best-estimate consensus DSM-IV diagnosis of bipolar disorder (type I, 161 subjects; type II, 51; and not otherwise specified, 6) or schizoaffective disorder, bipolar subtype (12 subjects), and are considered part of a broad bipolar phenotype (Table 2). Six individuals were diagnosed as having schizophrenia or schizoaffective depressive subtype and were excluded from all analyses. Among family members without major psychosis, 243 were unaffected (nonbipolar spectrum) first-degree, 86 were unaffected second-degree, and 42 were unaffected third-degree relatives of affected individuals. One hundred eight subjects were not biologically related to affected individuals and were used to form an unrelated control sample; 59 were subjects who had married into selected families and 49 were community controls. Liability groups differed in average age (F3,704 = 34.57 [P < .001]) and in sex distributions (χ2 = 7.15 [P = .05]) but not in education level (F3,704 = 0.89 [P = .44]) (Table 2). Individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype had significantly higher rates of anxiety disorders (χ2 = 32.34 [P < .001]) and past alcohol abuse/dependence (χ2 = 63.52 [P < .001]) than did their nonbipolar family members and unrelated participants. Among these individuals, 129 (56.1%) were prescribed psychotropic medications (some of them, multiple medications) at the time of assessment: 66 (28.7%) were prescribed antidepressants, 16 (7.0%) were prescribed lithium, 41 (17.8%) were prescribed mood stabilizers, 30 (13.0%) were prescribed anticonvulsants, 57 (24.8%) were prescribed sedatives, 37 (16.1%) were prescribed atypical antipsychotics, 23 (10.0%) were prescribed typical antipsychotics, and 11 (4.8%) were taking stimulants.
Estimated heritability for 22 neurocognitive variables are presented in Table 3. Although demographic covariates were fixed for all analyses, covariates that reached significance for a specific neurocognitive measure are included in the far right column. Two measures failed to reach significant levels of heritability: CVLT semantic clustering and CVLT delayed recall. Performance on these measures was significantly correlated with education and differed by location. When these covariates were omitted from analyses, these indices were significantly heritable.
Location of assessment significantly influenced performance on many neurocognitive measures (Table 3). Examining differences between individual sites indicated that the effect sizes for these differences were generally small. Indeed, the average effect size for neurocognitive performance differences between San Antonio and Costa Rica was 0.32; between San Antonio and Mexico City, 0.20; and between Mexico City and Costa Rica, 0.17. In contrast, the effect size for formal education was 0.61. Although performance on neurocognitive tests is influenced by myriad factors, educational attainment, which varies significantly across countries, has been shown to have a more profound effect on test performance than other factors.32,33 This pattern of results is consistent with our prior work in these locations.13
Sensitivity to liability for bipolar disorder
Results from analyses determining the sensitivity of neurocognitive measures to liability for bipolar disorder are given in Table 4. Individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype were statistically impaired on 6 of the 20 heritable cognitive measures compared with unrelated, unaffected subjects after controlling for FDR. To examine diagnostic specificity, analyses were repeated after constraining the affected group to patients with bipolar I disorder. These patients were impaired on all of the measures identified in the broad phenotype group and also on semantic fluency.
Given that individuals with bipolar disorder have increased rates of anxiety and alcohol use disorders (Table 2), it is unclear whether neurocognitive impairments are due to these co-occurring illnesses or to bipolar disorder per se. Hence, analyses were repeated with lifetime history of anxiety disorders and alcoholism included as covariates. Although anxiety disorders were associated with fewer hits on the Continuous Performance Test (P = .01) and alcoholism was linked to deficits on the Continuous Performance Test (hits, P < .001) and the Trail-Making Test (part A, P = .04; part B, P = .003), the addition of these covariates did not substantially alter the pattern of results in Table 4.
Because psychotropic medications may influence neurocognitive performance, use of psychotropic medications at the time of assessment was entered as a covariate into the model. However, individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype remained significantly impaired on measures sensitive to bipolar disorder when controlling for medication use. Furthermore, unaffected first-degree relatives, who based on the clinical interview do not use psychotropic medications, were impaired on 5 of these measures relative to unrelated controls, including the Digit Symbol Coding Task, letter-number span, Object Delayed Response Task, and immediate and delayed facial memory.
Genetic and environmental correlations performed on neurocognitive measures impaired in bipolar disorder are given in Table 5 and in the Figure. Of these measures, the Digit Symbol Coding Task, Object Delayed Response Task, and immediate facial memory were significantly and negatively correlated with affection status, indicating that poorer cognitive performance on these measures is related to an increasing genetic risk of bipolar disorder. None of the environmental correlations were significant, given the FDR. Similar correlations were observed when affection status was defined as bipolar I disorder (ρg [SE] for Digit Symbol Coding Task, −0.51 [0.21], P = .04; ρg [SE] for Object Delayed Response Task, −0.53 [0.17], P = .009; and ρg [SE] for immediate facial memory, −0.44 [0.19], P = .03).
In the full sample, performance on the Digit Symbol Coding Task was significantly genetically correlated with that of the Object Delayed Response Task (ρg [SE], 0.78 [0.09]; P = .003) and immediate facial memory (0.59 [0.16]; P = .001). Similarly, the Object Delayed Response Task performance was significantly correlated with that of immediate facial memory (0.62 [0.12]; P < .001).
Measures of processing speed, working memory, and declarative (facial) memory are candidate endophenotypes for bipolar disorder. Each of these measures was heritable, impaired in individuals with the illness and their nonbipolar relatives, and genetically correlated with affection status. Initial evidence for the specificity of these neurocognitive measures for liability to bipolar disorder was provided because the index of generalized cognitive functioning was not impaired in the illness. Although a number of investigators have demonstrated that healthy first-degree relatives of bipolar probands have memory or executive functioning impairments, this is, to our knowledge, the first large-scale family-based study to provide evidence that the neurocognitive deficits found in bipolar disorder are linked to genetic liability for the illness. Although there are various definitions for intermediate phenotypes or endophenotypes,2,12 there is universal agreement that these measures must be associated with genetic liability for the illness under investigation. Thus, an implicit assumption about endophenotypes is that the same genes that convey risk for disease also influence the endophenotype. More formally, there is an assumption of pleiotropy. Pleiotropy can be demonstrated through estimation of genetic correlation.31 Unfortunately, in nontwin designs, genetic correlation requires relatively large samples of related individuals (range, >300 to 600 persons) and, hence, is rarely applied in psychiatric genetics. However, without formally demonstrating pleiotropy, one of the primary criteria of an endophenotype is left untested. Each of the candidate endophenotypes identified in this study meet the necessary and sufficient criteria for a viable intermediate phenotype, providing testable hypotheses about brain systems implicated in the pathophysiologic process of bipolar disorder.
Performance on the Digit Symbol Coding Task is an index of processing speed or of the time needed to execute simple cognitive operations. Although processing speed has not been localized to a single brain region, cognitive neuroscience emphasizes the integration of information across spatially distinct brain regions,34,35 suggesting that cognitive slowing as indexed by the processing rate is related to neuronal efficiency.36 Several investigators have reported that euthymic individuals with bipolar disorder have moderate to severe digit-symbol coding impairments.6,37,38 Although processing speed measures such as the Digit Symbol Coding Task may be negatively influenced by psychotropic medications, a recent meta-analysis in schizophrenia suggested that medication status had little effect on patient performance.39 Furthermore, at least 2 previous studies have reported poor Digit Symbol Coding Task performance in unaffected family members of probands with bipolar disorder.40,41 Our results further these findings by showing a significant negative genetic correlation between bipolar disorder and Digit Symbol Coding Task performance.
The Object Delayed Response Task24 includes the maintenance and manipulation of memory for complex visual objects and was modeled after measures used in nonhuman primates.42 Functional magnetic resonance imaging experiments indicate that task performance engages a complex network of brain areas associated with working memory (eg, prefrontal, temporal, and parietal regions).43 Patients with bipolar disorder showed relative hyperactivation in the right prefrontal and anterior cingulate regions and relative hypoactivation in medial temporal and visual processing regions compared with healthy subjects.43 Although relatively few investigators have used delayed response tasks in bipolar disorder, working memory deficits—particularly when manipulation of information is critical—have been consistently reported in patients who are in remission.5 In contrast, there is little prior evidence of working memory dysfunction in unaffected relatives of patients with bipolar disorder.8 The present findings suggest that the application of working memory specifically designed for use in affective disorders could improve the sensitivity of these measures to liability for bipolar disorder.44 Nonetheless, the present findings should be confirmed in an independent sample.
Declarative memory impairments are among the most commonly reported cognitive deficits in bipolar disorder. Although most investigators report verbal memory impairments, visuospatial and facial memory deficits have also been observed in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.5,8 Indeed, in an early investigation of monozygotic twin-pairs discordant for bipolar disorder, Gourovitch and colleagues45 reported facial memory impairment in unaffected co-twins of bipolar probands. In the present study, verbal declarative memory was assessed with the CVLT.17,18 Although CVLT performance was heritable and impaired in individuals with bipolar disorder, nonbipolar first-degree relatives were not statistically different from healthy subjects. Our findings are similar to those reported in a recent meta-analysis of cognitive performance in unaffected first-degree relatives of bipolar probands.8 Although the present study nominates a facial declarative memory task as a candidate endophenotype for bipolar disorder, verbal declarative memory measures cannot be excluded and may represent important risk factors for the illness. More generally, although findings from the present study are encouraging, they are limited by the families studied and the neurocognitive tests used. Studies in different samples and with overlapping test batteries are warranted.
Significant heritability estimates indicate that genetic factors contribute substantially to the variation in the phenotype. However, heritability estimates do not provide information concerning the underlying genetic architecture of a phenotype and are subject to a number of assumptions.46 Indeed, phenotypes with high heritability estimates are not necessarily influenced by fewer genes or by genes with larger effects. For example, normal variation in adult height is highly heritable (h2 = 0.89-0.93),47 but current estimates suggest that up to 44 independent loci are associated with normal stature.48,49 In contrast, phenotypes with somewhat lower heritability estimates may have less complex genetic architectures. For example, the estimated heritability of the neuregulin 1 transcript was 0.50, but linkage analysis indicated a single locus (logarithm of odds = 15.8) on chromosome 8.50,51 Thus, although an intermediate phenotype should be heritable, the strength of the heritability estimate may be less critical. Heritability estimates for bipolar disorder are typically higher than those reported herein for neurocognitive traits. However, it remains to be seen whether the genetic architectures of these neurocognitive endophenotypes are less complex than for the illness itself. Furthermore, simulation studies indicate that quantitative endophenotypes should be significantly more powerful than qualitative diagnoses for identifying genes.52
Endophenotypes can inform psychiatric nosology. Although bipolar disorder and schizophrenia are considered discrete psychiatric illnesses with unique etiologies, growing evidence suggests that the illnesses may have some common genetic roots.53 Each of the neurocognitive endophenotypes nominated for bipolar disorder have also been found in schizophrenia. Indeed, the Digit Symbol Coding Task applied herein was the most sensitive cognitive measure of genetic liability for schizophrenia in our extended pedigree study.13 Similarly, the facial declarative memory measure has previously been found to be sensitive to risk of schizophrenia.54 Although our Object Delayed Response Task24 has not been used in schizophrenia family studies, similar measures have been proposed as intermediate phenotypes for the illness.54 Although the Spatial Delayed Response Task applied in the present study was previously shown to be sensitive to the genetic liability for schizophrenia,13,23 individuals with the broad bipolar phenotype were not impaired on this measure, implying some level of diagnostic specificity. However, we have shown in 2 separate samples that patients with bipolar disorder and a lifetime history of psychotic symptoms are impaired on this test, whereas patients without formal psychosis are not,44,55 suggesting that the manifestation of psychosis may be linked to spatial working memory deficits rather than to a specific diagnostic category. It is possible that by using a broad bipolar phenotype, in which few individuals manifest psychosis, we obscured deficits on this test. Indeed, when affection status was limited to bipolar I disorder alone, impairments on this test were more pronounced, though still below the FDR requirement. These findings raise questions about the specificity of endophenotypes for bipolar disorder alone rather than for psychotic illness more generally. However, it is unlikely that these questions will be fully addressed until the genetic factors that influence these neurocognitive endophenotypes and bipolar disorder are identified and found to confer risk for schizophrenia.
A number of the neurocognitive measures impaired in individuals with bipolar disorder were not impaired in nonbipolar first-degree relatives. Performance on these measures may be sensitive to the environmental factors necessary for the diathesis of the illness, and thus impairment in affected individuals is not associated with the genetic risk of bipolar disorder. Alternately, these tests may be influenced by affective or psychotic symptoms, psychotropic medication, sleep disturbances, or other sequelae of the illness. Indeed, current symptoms and psychotropic medications used to treat bipolar disorder have been shown to impair performance on specific neurocognitive tests.6,56 Although 56.1% of the patients included in the study were receiving medication at the time of assessment, inclusion of a single covariate coding use of psychotropic medication did not significantly alter the observed results. Although this analysis was coarse and did not account for potentially important factors (eg, lifetime history of medication use, specific class of medication), each of the proposed endophenotypes was also impaired in nonsymptomatic and medication-naive, unaffected first-degree relatives. Hence, it is unlikely that the candidate endophenotypes identified herein are explained by patient mood symptoms or medication use.
Although the pattern of results did not significantly differ when affection status was constrained to bipolar I disorder, it is difficult to make conclusions about diagnostic specificity from this sample. Seventy percent of the affected individuals had bipolar I disorder, and those individuals with bipolar II disorder, bipolar disorder not otherwise specified, or schizoaffective disorder were related to those with bipolar I disorder. To draw inferences about the utility of these cognitive endophenotypes for the genetic investigation of non–bipolar type I illnesses, families with these illnesses who do not have bipolar I disorder should be studied. Similar conclusions can be drawn about psychiatric comorbidities such as alcohol abuse and anxiety disorders.
A potential advantage of neurocognitive endophenotypes is that they may be less genetically complex than psychiatric diagnoses,12,57 although this potential has yet to be empirically demonstrated.58 However, because multiple neurocognitive endophenotypes can be studied simultaneously, they may afford an additional advantage over diagnostic classification, regardless of their genetic complexity. By focusing on the gene or genes common to all studied intermediate phenotypes, the number of candidate genes nominated for subsequent validation or functional genomic studies could be significantly reduced. However, such a strategy is valid only when significant genetic correlation between endophenotypes can be established (eg, pleiotropy). In the present study, the candidate endophenotypes were genetically correlated with each other and with bipolar disorder. This implies that these endophenotypes may represent a single genetic pathway for the illness and thus may facilitate this strategy prioritizing genes for additional study.
As demonstrated here and elsewhere,13,54 computerized neurocognitive measures can be efficiently and reliably administered to large numbers of individuals, a requirement for effective endophenotypes.59 Indeed, each of the tests sensitive to risk of bipolar disorder was developed specifically for large-scale studies of psychopathology. These tests are being applied in a multisite family-based linkage study of bipolar disorder and in a molecular genetics study of randomly ascertained individuals in large extended pedigrees. Together, these studies should provide clues to the genetic architecture of the neurocognitive endophenotypes identified in the present study and should facilitate the localization and identification of specific genes that contribute to these endophenotypes. These genes, in turn, should be examined for association with bipolar disorder. This iterative process should provide a window into the causal neurobiological pathways involved in the illness and the potential to develop biomarkers for psychiatric disorders.
Correspondence: David C. Glahn, PhD, Olin Neuropsychiatry Research Center, Whitehall Research Building, Institute of Living, 200 Retreat Ave, Hartford, CT 06106 (david.glahn@yale.edu).
Submitted for Publication: December 15, 2008; final revision received June 16, 2009; accepted June 19, 2009.
Author Contributions: Drs Glahn, Almasy, and Escamilla take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the statistical analyses. All authors had full access to all the data in the study.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants MH69856 (principle investigator, Dr Escamilla) and MH080912 (principle investigator, Dr Glahn) from the National Institute of Mental Health and a Young Investigator Award from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Depression (Dr Glahn). The SOLAR program is supported by grant MH59490 from the National Institute of Mental Health.
Previous Presentation: These data were presented in part at the 25th Annual Congress of the Collegium Internationale Neuro-psychopharmacologicum; July 16, 2008; Munich, Germany.
1.Ferreira
MAO’Donovan
MMeng
YJones
IRRuderfer
DMJones
LFan
JKirov
GPerlis
RHGreen
EKSmoller
JWGrozeva
DStone
JNikolov
IChambert
KHamshere
MLNimgaonkar
VLMoskvina
VThase
MECaesar
SSachs
GSFranklin
JGordon-Smith
KArdlie
KGGabriel
SBFraser
CBlumenstiel
BDefelice
MBreen
GGill
MMorris
DWElkin
AMuir
WJ McGhee
KAWilliamson
RMacIntyre
DJMacLean
AWSt
CDRobinson
MVan Beck
MPereira
ACKandaswamy
R McQuillin
ACollier
DABass
NJYoung
AHLawrence
JFerrier
INAnjorin
AFarmer
ACurtis
DScolnick
EM McGuffin
PDaly
MJCorvin
APHolmans
PABlackwood
DHGurling
HMOwen
MJPurcell
SMSklar
PCraddock
NWellcome Trust Case Control Consortium, Collaborative genome-wide association analysis supports a role for
ANK3 and
CACNA1C in bipolar disorder.
Nat Genet 2008;40
(9)
1056- 1058
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Gottesman
IIGould
TD The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions.
Am J Psychiatry 2003;160
(4)
636- 645
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Merikangas
KRChakravarti
AMoldin
SAraj
HBlangero
JCBurmeister
MCrabbe
J
JrDepaulo
JR
JrFoulks
EFreimer
NBKoretz
DSLichtenstein
WMignot
EReiss
ALRisch
NJTakahashi
JS Future of genetics of mood disorders research.
Biol Psychiatry 2002;52
(6)
457- 477
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Hasler
GDrevets
WGould
TGottesman
IManji
H Toward constructing an endophenotype strategy for bipolar disorders.
Biol Psychiatry 2006;60
(2)
93- 105
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Robinson
LJThompson
JGallagher
PGoswami
UYoung
AHFerrier
INMoore
PB A meta-analysis of cognitive deficits in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.
J Affect Disord 2006;93
(1-3)
105- 115
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Martínez-Arán
AVieta
EReinares
MColom
FTorrent
CSánchez-Moreno
JBenabarre
AGoikolea
JMComes
MSalamero
M Cognitive function across manic or hypomanic, depressed, and euthymic states in bipolar disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 2004;161
(2)
262- 270
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Burdick
KEGoldberg
JHarrow
MFaull
RMalhotra
A Neurocognition as a stable endophenotype in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.
J Nerv Ment Dis 2006;194
(4)
255- 260
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Arts
BJabben
NKrabbendam
Lvan Os
J Meta-analyses of cognitive functioning in euthymic bipolar patients and their first-degree relatives.
Psychol Med 2008;38
(6)
771- 785
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Glahn
DCBearden
CNiendam
TEscamilla
M The feasibility of neuropsychological endophenotypes in the search for genes associated with bipolar affective disorder.
Bipolar Disord 2004;6
(3)
171- 182
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Almasy
LBlangero
J Endophenotypes as quantitative risk factors for psychiatric disease: rationale and study design.
Am J Med Genet 2001;105
(1)
42- 44
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Blangero
JWilliams
JTAlmasy
L Novel family-based approaches to genetic risk in thrombosis.
J Thromb Haemost 2003;1
(7)
1391- 1397
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Glahn
DCAlmasy
LBlangero
JBurk
GMEstrada
JPeralta
JMMeyenberg
NCastro
MPBarrett
JNicolini
HRaventós
HEscamilla
MA Adjudicating neurocognitive endophenotypes for schizophrenia.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2007;144B
(2)
242- 249
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Nurnberger
JI
JrBlehar
MCKaufmann
CAYork-Cooler
CSimpson
SGHarkavy-Friedman
JSevere
JBMalaspina
DReich
T Diagnostic Interview for Genetic Studies: rationale, unique features, and training: NIMH Genetics Initiative.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51
(11)
849- 859, 863-864
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Maxwell
ME Manual for the FIGS. Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Mental Health; 1992
16.Leckman
JFSholomskas
DThompson
WDBelanger
AWeissman
MM Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: a methodological study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982;39
(8)
879- 883
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Delis
DKramer
JKaplan
EOber
B California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II) Manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment Co; 2000
18.Fortuny
LRomo
DHeaton
RPardee
R Manual De Normas y Procedimientos Para La Bateria Neuropsicologica en Espanol. Lisse, the Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger Publishers; 2001
19.Delis
DCKramer
JHKaplan
EHoldnack
J Reliability and validity of the Delis-Kaplan Executive Function System: an update.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2004;10
(2)
301- 303
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Wechsler
D WAIS-III: Administration and Scoring Manual: Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1997
21.Barch
DMBraver
TAkbudak
EConturo
TOllinger
JSnyder
A Anterior cingulate cortex and response conflict: effects of response modality and processing domain.
Cereb Cortex 2001;11
(9)
837- 848
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Gold
JMCarpenter
CRandolph
CGoldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Auditory working memory and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test performance in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54
(2)
159- 165
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Glahn
DCTherman
SManninen
MHuttunen
MKaprio
JLönnqvist
JCannon
TD Spatial working memory as an endophenotype for schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;53
(7)
624- 626
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Glahn
DCBarrett
JBearden
CEMintz
JGreen
MFSerap Monkul
ENajt
PSoares
JCVelligan
DI Dissociable mechanisms for memory impairment in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.
Psychol Med 2006;36
(8)
1085- 1095
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Gur
RCRagland
JMoberg
PTurner
THBilker
WBKohler
CSiegel
SJGur
RE Computerized neurocognitive scanning, I: methodology and validation in healthy people.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2001;25
(5)
766- 776
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Wechsler
D Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1999
27.Kurtz
MMRagland
JMoberg
PGur
R The Penn Conditional Exclusion Test: a new measure of executive-function with alternate forms of repeat administration.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2004;19
(2)
191- 201
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Cornblatt
BAErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Global attentional deviance as a marker of risk for schizophrenia: specificity and predictive validity.
J Abnorm Psychol 1985;94
(4)
470- 486
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Almasy
LBlangero
J Multipoint quantitative-trait linkage analysis in general pedigrees.
Am J Hum Genet 1998;62
(5)
1198- 1211
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Benjamini
YHochberg
Y Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing.
J R Stat Soc B 1995;57289- 300
Google Scholar 31.Almasy
LDyer
TBlangero
J Bivariate quantitative trait linkage analysis: pleiotropy versus co-incident linkages.
Genet Epidemiol 1997;14
(6)
953- 958
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Chung
JHSakong
JKang
PSKim
CYLee
KSJeon
MJSung
NJAhn
SHWon
KC Cross-cultural comparison of neurobehavioral performance in Asian workers.
Neurotoxicology 2003;24
(4-5)
533- 540
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Pontón
MOSatz
PHerrera
LOrtiz
FUrrutia
CPYoung
RD’Elia
LFFurst
CJNamerow
N Normative data stratified by age and education for the Neuropsychological Screening Battery for Hispanics (NeSBHIS): initial report.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc 1996;2
(2)
96- 104
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Fry
AFHale
S Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children.
Biol Psychol 2000;54
(1-3)
1- 34
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Hansell
NKWright
MJLuciano
MGeffen
GMGeffen
LBMartin
NG Genetic covariation between event-related potential (ERP) and behavioral non-ERP measures of working-memory, processing speed, and IQ.
Behav Genet 2005;35
(6)
695- 706
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Rypma
BBerger
JSGenova
HMRebbechi
DD’Esposito
M Dissociating age-related changes in cognitive strategy and neural efficiency using event-related fMRI.
Cortex 2005;41
(4)
582- 594
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Ferrier
INStanton
BKelly
TScott
J Neuropsychological function in euthymic patients with bipolar disorder.
Br J Psychiatry 1999;175246- 251
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Thompson
JMGallagher
PHughes
JWatson
SGray
JMFerrier
INYoung
AH Neurocognitive impairment in euthymic patients with bipolar affective disorder.
Br J Psychiatry 2005;18632- 40
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Dickinson
DRamsey
MGold
J Overlooking the obvious: a meta-analytic comparison of digit symbol coding tasks and other cognitive measures in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007;64
(5)
532- 542
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Ferrier
INChowdhury
RThompson
JWatson
SYoung
A Neurocognitive function in unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with bipolar disorder: a preliminary report.
Bipolar Disord 2004;6
(4)
319- 322
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41. McIntosh
AMHarrison
LForrester
KLawrie
SJohnstone
E Neuropsychological impairments in people with schizophrenia or bipolar disorder and their unaffected relatives.
Br J Psychiatry 2005;186378- 385
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Petrides
M Impairments on nonspatial self-ordered and externally ordered working memory tasks after lesions of the mid-dorsal part of the lateral frontal cortex in the monkey.
J Neurosci 1995;15
(1, pt 1)
359- 375
PubMedGoogle Scholar 43.Robinson
JLBearden
CEMonkul
ESTordesillas-Gutiérrez
DVelligan
DIFrangou
SGlahn
DC Fronto-temporal dysregulation in remitted bipolar patients: an fMRI delayed-non-match-to-sample (DNMS) study.
Bipolar Disord 2009;11
(4)
351- 360
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Glahn
DCBearden
CEBarguil
MBarrett
JReichenberg
ABowden
CLSoares
JCVelligan
DI The neurocognitive signature of psychotic bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2007;62
(8)
910- 916
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Gourovitch
MLTorrey
EGold
JRandolph
CWeinberger
DGoldberg
T Neuropsychological performance of monozygotic twins discordant for bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 1999;45
(5)
639- 646
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Silventoinen
KSammalisto
SPerola
MBoomsma
DICornes
BKDavis
CDunkel
LDe Lange
MHarris
JRHjelmborg
JVLuciano
MMartin
NGMortensen
JNisticò
LPedersen
NLSkytthe
ASpector
TDStazi
MAWillemsen
GKaprio
J Heritability of adult body height: a comparative study of twin cohorts in eight countries.
Twin Res 2003;6
(5)
399- 408
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Weedon
MNLango
HLindgren
CMWallace
CEvans
DMMangino
MFreathy
RMPerry
JRStevens
SHall
ASSamani
NJShields
BProkopenko
IFarrall
MDominiczak
AJohnson
TBergmann
SBeckmann
JSVollenweider
PWaterworth
DMMooser
VPalmer
CNMorris
ADOuwehand
WHZhao
JHLi
SLoos
RJBarroso
IDeloukas
PSandhu
MSWheeler
ESoranzo
NInouye
MWareham
NJCaulfield
MMunroe
PBHattersley
AT McCarthy
MIFrayling
TMDiabetes Genetics Initiative; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium; Cambridge GEM Consortium, Genome-wide association analysis identifies 20 loci that influence adult height.
Nat Genet 2008;40
(5)
575- 583
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Göring
HHCurran
JEJohnson
MPDyer
TDCharlesworth
JCole
SAJowett
JBAbraham
LJRainwater
DLComuzzie
AGMahaney
MCAlmasy
L MacCluer
JWKissebah
AHCollier
GRMoses
EKBlangero
J Discovery of expression QTLs using large-scale transcriptional profiling in human lymphocytes.
Nat Genet 2007;39
(10)
1208- 1216
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Carless
MCurran
JCharlesworth
J Transcriptional profiling for identification of Cis-regulated genes involved in complex brain-related disorders. Paper presented at: International Society of Psychiatric Genetics; October8 , 2007; New York, NY
52.Wijsman
EMAmos
C Genetic analysis of simulated oligogenic traits in nuclear and extended pedigrees: summary of GAW10 contributions.
Genet Epidemiol 1997;14
(6)
719- 735
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Craddock
NO'Donovan
MCOwen
MJ Genes for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder? implications for psychiatric nosology.
Schizophr Bull 2006;32
(1)
9- 16
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Gur
RENimgaonkar
VAlmasy
LCalkins
MERagland
JDPogue-Geile
MFKanes
SBlangero
JGur
RC Neurocognitive endophenotypes in a multiplex multigenerational family study of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry 2007;164
(5)
813- 819
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Glahn
DCBearden
CECakir
SBarrett
JANajt
PSerap Monkul
EMaples
NVelligan
DISoares
JC Differential working memory impairment in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: effects of lifetime history of psychosis.
Bipolar Disord 2006;8
(2)
117- 123
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Holmes
MKErickson
KLuckenbaugh
DDrevets
WCBain
EECannon
DMSnow
JSahakian
BJManji
HKZarate
CA
Jr A comparison of cognitive functioning in medicated and unmedicated subjects with bipolar depression.
Bipolar Disord 2008;10
(7)
806- 815
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Greenwood
TABraff
DLight
GCadenhead
KSCalkins
MEDobie
DJFreedman
RGreen
MFGur
REGur
RCMintz
JNuechterlein
KHOlincy
ARadant
ADSeidman
LJSiever
LJSilverman
JMStone
WSSwerdlow
NRTsuang
DWTsuang
MTTuretsky
BISchork
NJ Initial heritability analyses of endophenotypic measures for schizophrenia: the consortium on the genetics of schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2007;64
(11)
1242- 1250
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Schulze
TG McMahon
F Defining the phenotype in human genetic studies: forward genetics and reverse phenotyping.
Hum Hered 2004;58
(3-4)
131- 138
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref