Context
There are no effective pharmacotherapies for stimulant dependence but there are many plausible targets for development of novel therapeutics. We hypothesized that dopamine-related targets are relevant for treatment of stimulant dependence, and there will likely be individual differences in response to dopaminergic challenges.
Objective
To measure behavioral and brain functional markers of drug-related attentional bias in stimulant-dependent individuals studied repeatedly after short-term dosing with dopamine D2/D3 receptor antagonist and agonist challenges.
Design
Randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, parallel-groups, crossover design using pharmacological functional magnetic resonance imaging.
Setting
Clinical research unit (GlaxoSmithKline) and local community in Cambridge, England.
Participants
Stimulant-dependent individuals (n = 18) and healthy volunteers (n = 18).
Interventions
Amisulpride (400 mg), pramipexole dihydrochloride (0.5 mg), or placebo were administered in counterbalanced order at each of 3 repeated testing sessions.
Main Outcome Measures
Attentional bias for stimulant-related words was measured during functional magnetic resonance imaging by a drug-word Stroop paradigm; trait impulsivity and compulsivity of dependence were assessed at baseline by questionnaire.
Results
Drug users demonstrated significant attentional bias for drug-related words, which was correlated with greater activation of the left prefrontal and right cerebellar cortex. Attentional bias was greater in people with highly compulsive patterns of stimulant abuse; the effects of dopaminergic challenges on attentional interference and related frontocerebellar activation were different between high- and low-compulsivity subgroups.
Conclusions
Greater attentional bias for and greater prefrontal activation by stimulant-related words constitute a candidate neurocognitive marker for dependence. Individual differences in compulsivity of stimulant dependence had significant effects on attentional bias, its brain functional representation, and its short-term modulation by dopaminergic challenges.
Chronic stimulant drug abuse is associated with major adverse health, social, legal, and economic consequences.1-3 Yet the pharmacological and psychological treatments available for stimulant dependence are characterized by high dropout and relapse rates.4-6 There is a clear need to improve treatment efficacy for the growing number of stimulant-dependent individuals.3
Advances in neuroimaging research have contributed to a better understanding of the neurobiological basis of stimulant dependence and have indicated some plausible targets for pharmacological intervention. On the hypothesis that stimulant dependence may be associated with a loss of “top-down” inhibitory control over lower-level reward processing,7,8 several studies have investigated brain and behavioral responses to tasks, such as the color-word Stroop paradigm, that entail attentional control to override an automatic but incorrect response tendency.9-18 In studies of stimulant dependence, a drug-related version of the Stroop test has been used to measure the degree of involuntary attention, or attentional bias, toward drug-related words compared with neutral words.19-23 The increased salience of drug-related cues, as measured by attentional bias in drug-word Stroop paradigms, is thought to contribute to the maintenance of drug-taking behavior7,24 and has demonstrated predictive value for the risk of relapse.12,21,25-27
The exact neurochemical mechanisms underpinning attentional bias to drug-related stimuli in stimulant-dependent individuals (SDIs) are unknown, but attentional bias on the Stroop paradigm is responsive to dopaminergic modulation.28-30 Dopamine is generally released in response to reward-predicting or salient events31-33 and also in response to drug-related cues in drug-dependent individuals.34,35 Accumulating evidence indicates that striatal dopamine systems are disrupted in stimulant dependence.36-38 For example, studies using raclopride positron emission tomography have shown that SDIs have a significantly lower binding potential of striatal dopamine D2 receptors,39-41 which has usually been interpreted as a marker of reduced postsynaptic receptor density.42 Reduced striatal dopamine receptor density appears to be directly related to decreased metabolic activity in the orbitofrontal cortex,39,40 a brain area implicated in compulsive behavior and craving.43
Collectively, these studies suggest that attentional bias predisposing to stimulant dependence could be related to abnormal dopamine transmission and could therefore be treated by drugs targeting dopamine receptors. There are rationales for both dopamine agonist and antagonist drugs, and both have been tried, but the findings are inconsistent,5,44-47 in most cases providing no more than weak evidence for therapeutic efficacy. One possible explanation for this discrepancy between the theoretical strength of the target and the apparent lack of clinical benefit is that stimulant dependence is a heterogeneous syndrome. There are important psychological dimensions of difference between individuals, which may modulate their response to treatment and attenuate the power of clinical trials involving small numbers of psychologically undifferentiated SDIs. Two psychological dimensions of particular interest in this respect are impulsivity and compulsivity: both are known to be abnormal in SDIs compared with nondependent people,24,48-50 and both have been linked to dopaminergic mechanisms.51-54Impulsivity can be defined as loss of inhibitory control over the response to rewarding or distracting stimuli and may play a role as both a cause and a consequence of substance abuse55,56; compulsivity is defined as persistence or perseverance of behavior in the absence of reward or despite punishment and may be particularly important in the maintenance of drug-taking habits.54 Both impulsivity and drug-related compulsivity have been linked with alterations in the dopamine system52,57,58 and are thought to account for the variability of dopaminergic drug effects in SDIs.51
Herein, we tested 2 hypothetical predictions: (1) attentional bias for drug-related cues, and related brain activation, would be abnormal in people with stimulant dependence and (2) the effects of dopaminergic challenge drugs on attentional bias and related brain systems would be influenced by individual differences in impulsivity and compulsivity among a group of SDIs. We conducted a double-blind, placebo-controlled, pharmacological functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study to evaluate the effects on drug-word Stroop task performance of single doses of a dopamine D2/D3 receptor agonist (pramipexole dihydrochloride, 0.5 mg) and a D2/D3 antagonist (amisulpride, 400 mg) administered in a within-subjects crossover design to SDIs and healthy comparison volunteers.
Thirty-six right-handed volunteers participated in this study: 18 individuals with a chronic history of stimulant drug abuse, satisfying the DSM-IV-TR59 criteria for dependence on either crack cocaine (n = 5), cocaine (n = 5), or amphetamines (n = 8) and 18 matched healthy controls. Stimulant-dependent individuals were non–treatment seeking and recruited from the local community. Prior to study enrolment, all participants had a satisfactory medical review and were screened for any other current Axis I psychiatric disorder using the Structured Clinical Interview for the DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders.60 Participants were excluded on the basis of any current Axis I psychiatric disorder, other than substance dependence in SDIs. Participants were also screened for normal appearance of structural MRI scans (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220). All volunteers provided written informed consent and received monetary compensation for participation. The study was approved by the Cambridge Research Ethics Committee (REC06/Q0108/130; principal investigator, T.W.R.).
Short-term dopaminergic drug treatments
Amisulpride is a selective antagonist at the dopamine D2/D3 receptors. We administered a single oral dose of 400 mg of amisulpride; no serious adverse events were reported. Pramipexole is a selective agonist at the dopamine D2/D3 receptors61; we administered a single oral dose of 1.5 mg of pramipexole dihydrochloride to the first 6 participants enrolled in the study (3 SDIs, 3 nondependent volunteers). However, this dose of pramipexole was poorly tolerated in the 3 nondependent volunteers, who were unable to perform the tasks at this treatment session because of nausea, vomiting, sweating, and tiredness. Subsequently, the dose of pramipexole dihydrochloride was reduced to 0.5 mg orally for all participants (ie, for 18 controls [including the 3 volunteers who did not tolerate the higher dose] and 15 SDIs). All participants were also administered 30 mg of domperidone orally at each treatment session to prevent emetic effects of dopamine receptor agonism (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220).
All participants completed the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale as a measure of self-reported impulsivity. In SDIs, drug-related compulsivity was assessed using the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale,62,63 an equivalent to the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale64 of symptom severity in obsessive-compulsive disorder. The Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale measures the amount of time, interference, and distress caused by stimulant-related thoughts or urges and the efforts to resist them. Obsessive-compulsive tendencies in controls were assessed by the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220).
Attentional control tasks
We used 2 versions of the Stroop task as tests of attentional control, based on the paradigm used by Compton et al,65 in which participants were asked to indicate the font color of a word displayed on the screen using a 4-button box. Each button corresponded to 1 of 4 possible font colors: red, blue, yellow, or green. The drug-word Stroop task involved 2 lists of target words: 16 amphetamine-related and 16 cocaine-related words. Both lists were matched with a list of 16 neutral words with regard to length and frequency. Equivalent to the drug-word version, the color-word Stroop task also involved 2 lists of color words, each including 8 distinct color words that were presented twice per block. Each color word was always displayed in a font color incongruent with its meaning. Both lists of color words were matched with neutral words for length and frequency (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220).
Both the drug-word and the color-word Stroop tasks were presented as a blocked periodic paradigm during fMRI (Figure 1). This design was chosen because interference effects in emotional Stroop paradigms are thought to rely on a slow disengagement process66 that generalizes to drug-related words,67 and the block design has therefore been the method of choice in many prior studies investigating attentional interference in an emotional context.65,68-70 For each word trial, the interstimulus interval was 2.2 seconds, including the presentation of a fixation cross for 0.3 second followed by the presentation of a word for 1.9 seconds, during which time the participants had to register a response by button press. Each block of words lasted 35.2 seconds, and 2 blocks of each word type were followed by a 38-second fixation block (Figure 1). The order of the words within each block was randomized and the order of the blocks was counterbalanced across participants and testing sessions. The allocation of the font colors to the words was randomized; each of the 4 colors occurred equally often in each word category. All participants were trained on the task prior to each scanning session with a practice run of at least 1 color-word block and 1 block of different neutral words to familiarize themselves with the task and to minimize practice effects. Acute Stroop task–induced stimulant cravings were measured immediately before and after the Stroop task by asking the following question: “How would you rate your current state of craving on a scale from 0 to 100?”
Task performance was measured in terms of accuracy and latency of response to each trial. Attentional bias for drug-related words was quantified by an interference score, calculated for each individual as the median latency of correct responses to the drug-related words minus the median latency of correct responses to the neutral words. Likewise, attentional interference in the color-word Stroop task was calculated as the median latency of correct responses to color words minus the median latency of correct responses to neutral words. The median is generally more robust than the mean as a measure of central location in small samples; however, the mean interference scores were also calculated for comparative purposes (Table 1 and Table 2).
Whole-brain fMRI data were acquired at the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, England, using a Siemens Magnetom Tim Trio whole-body scanner operating at 3 T(Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany). During the performance of each Stroop task, 32 transaxial sections of gradient echo, echoplanar imaging data depicting blood oxygen level–dependent contrast were acquired parallel to the intercommissural line with the following parameters: repetition time = 2000 milliseconds, echo time = 30 milliseconds, flip angle = 78°, slice thickness = 3 mm plus 0.75-mm interslice gap, image matrix size = 64 × 64, and within-plane voxel dimensions = 3.0 mm × 3.0 mm. Prior to data analysis, the first 5 echoplanar images were discarded to account for T1 equilibration effects.
Analysis of demographic, psychometric, and behavioral data
Using data from both groups, attentional interference scores, error, and craving data were each analyzed separately using a repeated-measures analysis of covariance (ANCOVA) model with drug treatment (3 levels: placebo, amisulpride, and pramipexole) as the within-subject factor and group (2 levels: nondependent volunteers and SDIs) as the between-subject factor. Using data from the patient group only, attentional interference was also analyzed using a repeated-measures ANCOVA model with drug treatment (3 levels: placebo, amisulpride, and pramipexole) as the within-subject factor and compulsivity subgroup (2 levels: high and low) as the between-subject factor. In all analyses of attentional interference scores, only data on correctly performed trials were included. To control for group differences in depressive mood, education, and dose of pramipexole, we included the Beck Depression Inventory scores, years of education, and plasma levels of pramipexole as covariates in all analyses involving SDIs and controls; the subgroup analyses (involving SDIs only) included plasma levels of pramipexole as a covariate. Statistical tests were conducted using SPSS (version 13; SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois) and were reported as significant if P < .05.
Details of the fMRI data analysis procedures are briefly summarized herein (full details, http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220). We first identified by whole-brain analysis those brain regions where functional activation was specifically related to the contrast of drug-related vs neutral words and strongly correlated with the behavioral measure of attentional interference (as defined earlier). At each voxel of these behaviorally relevant frontal and cerebellar regions, we tested the factorial effects of diagnostic group and dopaminergic drug challenge by ANCOVA using all data on both groups. Finally, we also tested each voxel of this frontocerebellar system to investigate factorial effects of compulsivity (high or low) and dopaminergic drug challenge by ANCOVA in the patient group only. These anatomically focused factorial analyses of the fMRI data are formally analogous to parallel factorial analyses of behavioral data on attentional interference (described earlier).
All data sets from both Stroop tasks for SDIs and controls under all drug conditions were initially preprocessed to correct for effects of subject motion, differential slice timing, and differences in global means.72-74 At the first single-subject level of analysis, a time series regression model specified 2 key contrasts between correctly performed trials: (1) drug-related words vs neutral words in the drug-word blocks and (2) color words vs neutral words in the color-word blocks. These contrasts are equivalent to those used to generate attentional interference scores in the behavioral data and are therefore most specific to the cognitive control processes of interest. This design matrix, convolved with a hemodynamic response function,75 was regressed at each voxel onto the corrected time series. The resulting maps of the model coefficients normalized by their standard errors for each individual were mapped into the standard space of the Montreal Neurological Institute echo-planar imaging template by linear affine transformation to obtain a group activation map. These activation maps included all participants under all drug treatments to identify brain regions that were significantly activated by contrast 1 (drug-word Stroop task) and by contrast 2 (color-word Stroop task) using a permutation test on spatial statistics (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220).
We used the activation map of the drug-word Stroop task contrast to define a mask (shown as red voxels in Figure 2) and regressed the median attentional interference score for each individual on the individual activation statistics at each voxel in this mask. This procedure identified a set of voxels that were both generically activated by the task and significantly associated with variability in attentional interference (shown as yellow voxels in Figure 2). The mean activation statistic over all voxels associated with attentional interference was calculated for each participant and used as a dependent variable in ANCOVA models that tested the effects of group, drug, and drug × group interaction on brain activation in these regions. The same regional activation statistics were also used as dependent variables in ANCOVA models that tested the effects of subgroup (eg, high or low compulsivity), drug, and drug × subgroup interaction on brain activation.
Baseline impulsivity, compulsivity, and mood
As expected from previous research,49 SDIs reported significantly higher levels of impulsivity on the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale than controls. Stimulant-dependent individuals also generally reported high levels of compulsivity of stimulant abuse on the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale, whereas healthy controls did not show strong obsessive-compulsive traits, as assessed by the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (Table 3). Although participants with clinical depression were excluded from the study, SDIs reported higher scores on depressive mood, as assessed at baseline by the Montgomery-Asberg Depression Rating Scale77 and corroborated by serial scores on the Beck Depression Inventory–II71 before each treatment session (F2,34 = 21.50; P < .001).
Subjective effects of drug treatment
Dopaminergic challenge drugs had no significant effects on subjective alertness, contentedness, or calmness scales. There were also no significant group differences or drug × group interactions on any of these measures (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220). There was no evidence for induction of craving by performance of the drug-word Stroop task and no effect of dopaminergic challenge drugs on self-reported craving.
Behavioral data for the stroop tasks
In the drug-word Stroop paradigm, SDIs showed a marked attentional bias toward drug-related cues compared with controls (Figure 2A). Thus, across all 3 short-term treatment conditions, SDIs demonstrated significantly longer latency than controls in correctly identifying the font colors of drug-related words compared with their responses to neutral words (Table 1). Drug users did not make more errors on the drug-word Stroop task than controls. The significant drug × group interaction for accuracy was driven by controls who showed a tendency to make fewer errors while taking amisulpride relative to placebo (F1,14 = 3.48; P = .08). On the color-word Stroop paradigm, no main effect of group and no drug × group interaction were identified (Table 1).
fMRI DATA FOR THE DRUG-WORD AND COLOR-WORD STROOP TASKS
The brain regions activated by drug words (relative to neutral words) included the left ventral and dorsal prefrontal cortex and precentral cortex; anterior cingulate, medial prefrontal, and premotor cortex; posterior cingulate and medial posterior parietal cortex; bilateral middle and inferior temporal cortex; left thalamus and caudate nucleus; and right cerebellum (Table 4 and http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220). Within this pattern of brain activation, attentional bias for drug words was most strongly associated with greater activation of the left ventral prefrontal cortex (Montreal Neurological Institute coordinates [x, y, z] in millimeters: −46, 26, 12; −44, 22, −8; and −40, 6, 30) and right cerebellum (x, y, z: 22, −80, −40) (Figure 2B and C). We fitted an ANCOVA model to test for significant main and interactive effects of group and dopaminergic drug challenge on these regions of activation. Only the effect of group was significant: SDIs had greater task-related activation of the left ventral prefrontal cortex and right cerebellum compared with controls (F1,31 = 6.40; P < .05) (Figure 2D and http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220).
In the color-word Stroop task, the only brain area activated in all participants was the left cerebellum (x, y, z: −4, −46, −40) (Table 5). We did not find a significant relationship with attentional interference scores in this locus of activation and therefore did not investigate it any further.
Effects of impulsivity and compulsivity on stimulant dependence
To investigate possible effects of individual differences in baseline measurements of impulsivity on drug-related attentional bias, we divided the SDIs into low- and high-impulsivity subgroups by a median split on their total Barratt Impulsiveness Scale scores. The high- and low-impulsivity subgroups were not significantly different on any other demographic or clinical variables (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220). There were no significant differences between impulsivity subgroups in latency or accuracy of response to the drug-word Stroop test and there were no significant drug × impulsivity subgroup interactions (Table 2).
Likewise, we also divided the SDIs into high- and low-compulsivity subgroups by a median split on their total Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale scores; the compulsivity subgroups were not significantly different on any other baseline variables (http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?ke220). However, there was a significant subgroup effect on attentional interference during the drug-word Stroop test; the high-compulsivity subgroup demonstrated significantly greater attentional bias to drug-related related words (Figure 3A) (Table 2). There was also a significant drug × compulsivity subgroup interaction whereby the dopaminergic agents had markedly different effects on attentional bias in high- and low-compulsivity subgroups. Post hoc analysis indicated that this interaction was due to different effects of pramipexole in the 2 subgroups: low-compulsivity SDIs showed no attentional bias, whereas in high-compulsivity SDIs, response latencies for drug-related words were increased (F1,15 = 9.80; P < .05).
The effects of compulsivity on attentional bias for drug-related words were followed up by an analysis of subgroup effects on brain activation in the frontocerebellar regions previously defined. We found a significant drug × compulsivity subgroup interaction on brain activation in the left ventral prefrontal cortex and right cerebellum (F2,30 = 3.36; P < .05) (Figure 3B). Post hoc analysis showed that this interaction also was mainly driven by pramipexole, which caused a significant reduction in frontocerebellar activation compared with placebo in the low-compulsivity subgroup (t8 = 2.34; P < .05) and the opposite effect of increased prefrontal activation, in the high-compulsivity subgroup (t8 = −0.77; P > .05).
Using 2 versions of the Stroop paradigm for testing attentional control, we confirmed that SDIs had significantly greater attentional bias in favor of drug-related cues,20,21,23,63,80 reflected in greater attentional interference scores on the drug-word Stroop test (Figure 2A). Moreover, this impairment of attentional control in people with stimulant dependence was specific to drug-related cues and did not generalize to their performance on the color-word Stroop task. Thus, it seems that there is a clinically relevant impairment of attentional control in stimulant dependence21 that does not simply reflect the global impairment in attentional function that has been associated with stimulant dependence.81,82
We were also able to show, using fMRI, that greater attentional bias for drug-related words was associated with greater activation of the right cerebellum, a brain area that is implicated in Stroop task interference control,83-86 and the left ventral prefrontal cortex or inferior frontal gyrus, a brain area that is implicated in the retrieval of semantic knowledge87-89 and the processing of word meaning.90-92 This neurocognitive association was demonstrated by the contrast between SDIs and controls: SDIs had significantly greater frontocerebellar activation (Figure 2D) as well as significantly greater attentional interference scores (Figure 2A) and attentional interference was positively correlated with frontocerebellar activation over all subjects in both groups (Figure 2C). Although abnormal inferior frontal gyrus activation during the disorder-related Stroop paradigm has been identified in various groups of psychiatric patients,68,69,93 overactivation of left inferior frontal gyrus when identifying font colors of drug-related words might be specific for stimulant dependence. Left inferior frontal gyrus overactivation in stimulant users has also been reported in response to cocaine-related videos94 or imagery95 relative to neutral cues or when a stimulant drug was administered unexpectedly.96
Compulsivity of drug abuse and frontocerebellar systems
We also found that SDIs with a highly compulsive pattern of stimulant drug abuse generally had greater attentional bias for drug-related words (Figure 3A). Moreover, the high- and low-compulsivity subgroups differed markedly in their response to single-dose challenges of pramipexole and amisulpride: most saliently, pramipexole increased attentional interference and frontocerebellar activation in highly compulsive SDIs, whereas in low-compulsivity SDIs, pramipexole had the opposite effect, tending to reduce or “normalize” attentional interference and frontocerebellar activation. How can we explain the different effects of these drugs on attentional interference and related brain activation in high- and low-compulsivity subgroups of SDIs?
At a mechanistic level, the normalizing effect of pramipexole on attentional bias and related frontocerebellar systems in low-compulsive SDIs could be achieved by 1 of 2 main actions. If there is relatively deficient striatal dopamine neurotransmission in this subgroup, consistent with findings recently reviewed by Narendran and Martinez,97 that impairs the function of the associated corticostriatal “loops,” the dopamine D2/D3 agonist could restore normal function via its postsynaptic action.98 Alternatively, given that the SDIs were not in a withdrawal state, it is conceivable that they were exhibiting upregulation of striatal dopamine neurotransmission at the synapse and that the relatively low dose of the dopamine agonist restored normal functioning by its presynaptic action at dopamine D2 autoreceptors.99,100 However, the D2/D3 receptor antagonist, amisulpride, generally had opposite effects to those of pramipexole, perhaps favoring the former hypothesis.
Clearly, neither of these hypotheses could account for the opposite effects of pramipexole in the high-compulsivity subgroup. One hypothesis that might explain this is that highly compulsive individuals have developed a dopamine receptor sensitivity that could lead to an exaggerated and pathological response to pramipexole. Similar observations of postsynaptic supersensitivity have been described in patients with Parkinson disease who compulsively overdose their dopamine agonist medication.58 The consistent finding of reduced D2/D3 receptor binding in stimulant dependence36,38,97,101 argues against this interpretation. However, recent preclinical studies have demonstrated that a long-term regimen of amphetamine exposure does lead to an increase in activation of the high-affinity state of striatal D2 receptors.102 Regardless of these considerations, it is evident that heterogeneity in response to dopaminergic agents clearly has to be taken into account when interpreting the studies of dopamine neurotransmission in stimulant abusers and indeed may help to resolve some of the inconsistencies in the field.97
One general therapeutic implication of these results is that dopaminergic drugs might have value in controlling the attentional bias toward stimulant-related cues that has previously been shown to predict relapse following a period of abstinence.21 For example, dopaminergic agents could reduce the risk of attentional capture by relapse-provoking cues and thereby promote maintenance of abstinence. The observation that we did not find a relationship between attentional bias and self-reported craving may not be unusual given that many aspects of compulsive drug taking occur through automatic or habitual processes.54,103,104 Our data further suggest that the therapeutic effects of dopaminergic drugs could be strongly influenced by the baseline compulsivity of stimulant drug use (which can vary considerably within a group homogenously defined as satisfying DSM-IV criteria for stimulant drug dependence). This finding also has potential implications for early clinical trial design in the development of new candidate therapeutics for stimulant dependence: greater statistical power for tests of therapeutic efficacy might be achieved by sampling psychologically stratified, eg, high- or low-compulsivity, groups of patients.
Strengths of the study include the balanced, parallel-groups, crossover design; the use of psychological test paradigms theoretically focused on cognitive mechanisms of special relevance to drug dependence; the choice of pharmacologically contrasting dopaminergic challenge drugs; and the well-characterized group of stimulant users. Weaknesses of the study include modest sample size, which has probably constrained the statistical power of the experiment, although the repeated-measures design generated 108 (36 × 3) behavioral and neuroimaging observations to assess factorial effects of interest. This is a large number of observations by comparison with other neuroimaging studies of stimulant dependence. The dose of pramipexole dihydrochloride was reduced during the course of the experiment, from 1.5 mg to 0.5 mg, because of the occurrence of adverse effects at the starting dose. This difference in dose was associated with proportional changes in pharmacokinetic measures of exposure, which were subsequently included as covariates in ANCOVA models to control for any effects of dose on behavioral or brain functional differences between groups.
Although SDIs were carefully selected according to eligibility criteria that excluded comorbid Axis I disorders and alcohol and opiate dependence, there was still a degree of heterogeneity within the SDI group in terms of their stimulant drug of choice (cocaine or amphetamine) and its source (3 amphetamine users were prescribed amphetamine for harm reduction). This clinical heterogeneity motivates our analysis of individual differences in compulsivity and impulsivity of drug use; however, it also presents a constraint on the generalizability of our results. The observation that high- and low-compulsivity SDIs did not differ in terms of drug-taking histories or clinical symptoms may seem surprising but similar observations have been made with regard to craving in the presence of drug-related cues, which seems not to be reflected in clinical or demographic variables but in biological differences of cue-induced dopamine release in the striatum.35
In relation to the cognitive tests, one may argue that a lack of familiarity of drug words in controls may have affected the results. This seems unlikely since previous research has shown that the interference effect for the drug-word Stroop task is specific to drug-dependent individuals and independent from the familiarity of words.105,106 It is still not clear whether drug-related compulsivity is similar to compulsive behavior observed in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder, a disorder that has also been associated with dopamine dysfunction.107-109 In our healthy controls, however, the Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale has been shown to be insensitive for obsessive-compulsive behaviors at a subclinical level. Further studies may consider using other measures such as the Padua Inventory,110 which assesses the spectrum rather than the severity of obsessive-compulsive symptoms. Moreover, behavioral measures of compulsivity in stimulant dependence need to be developed to validate the individual differences in compulsivity of drug use measured herein by a self-report instrument (Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale). For example, future studies of SDIs might usefully also include more general cognitive measures of attentional set shifting or reversal learning as more objective markers of compulsive perseveration.
Correspondence: Karen D. Ersche, PhD, CPsychol, University of Cambridge, Behavioural and Clinical Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychiatry, Herchel Smith Building, Cambridge CB2 0SZ, England (ke220@cam.ac.uk).
Submitted for Publication: March 19, 2009; final revision received September 16, 2009; accepted September 17, 2009.
Author Contributions: Drs Ersche and Bullmore contributed equally to this work.
Financial Disclosure: This work was funded and sponsored by GlaxoSmithKline grant RG45422 and conducted within the GlaxoSmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge, Cambridge, England. Dr Bullmore is employed part time by the University of Cambridge and part time by GlaxoSmithKline plc and is a shareholder of GlaxoSmithKline. Dr Craig is full-time employed by P1vital Ltd and Ms Shabbir and Dr Merlo-Pich are full-time employed by GlaxoSmithKline plc. Dr Abbott has provided computing services for GlaxoSmithKline. Dr Suckling has received grant support from GlaxoSmithKline. Dr Müller has received research grant support from Janssen-Cilag and honoraria or travel expenses from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly, Janssen-Cilag, Pharmacia-Upjohn, and UCB Pharma. Dr Sahakian has provided consulting services to Cambridge Cognition Ltd, GlaxoSmithKline, Novartis, AstraZeneca, and Eli Lilly; she holds shares of CeNeS and has received honoraria for grand rounds in psychiatry at Massachusetts General Hospital (continuing medical education credits) and for speaking at the International Conference on Cognitive Dysfunction in Schizophrenia and Mood Disorders (2007). Dr Sahakian is also on the Medical Research Council Neurosciences and Mental Health Board and the Science Coordination Team for the Foresight Project on Mental Capital and Wellbeing and receives an honorarium as an associate editor of Psychological Medicine. Dr Robbins has provided consulting services for Cambridge Cognition Ltd and the pharmaceutical industry, including Eli Lilly and GlaxoSmithKline, and has also received honoraria from Roche, Merck Sharp and Dohme, Lundbeck, and GlaxoSmithKline. He is a past recipient of research grants from GlaxoSmithKline and Pfizer and holds shares from CeNeS and share options from Cambridge Cognition Ltd and Allon Therapeutics. He also receives an editorial honorarium from Springer-Verlag (Psychopharmacology).
Funding/Support: Software development was supported by a Human Brain Project grant from the National Institute of Mental Health and the National Institute of Biomedical Imaging and Bioengineering. Dr Ersche is a recipient of the Betty Behrens Research Fellowship at Clare Hall, University of Cambridge. Dr Sahakian is supported by a Wellcome Trust programme grant. Dr Robbins is supported by a Wellcome Trust programme grant and is a recipient of the James S. McDonnell Foundation Collaborative Award.
Previous Presentations: Data in preliminary form were presented at the International Symposium on Attention and Performance XXII; July 17, 2008; Stow, Vermont; the 21st European College for Neuropsychopharmacology Annual Conference; September 3, 2008; Barcelona, Spain; and the Collegium Internationale Neuro-Psychopharmacologicum Regional Meeting on Addiction and Psychosis; April 26, 2009; Edinburgh, Scotland.
Additional Contributions: We thank all volunteers for their participation in this study and staff at the GlaxoSmithKline Clinical Unit Cambridge and the Wolfson Brain Imaging Centre for their assistance. We are particularly grateful to Rowena Sheehan, Judy Gilbert, Rona Weston-Arnold, Sharon Crosby, and Laura Griffiths for their dedicated help with volunteer recruitment and medical support. Thanks also go to Deanna Barch, PhD, for advice on the Stroop task paradigms and to Graham Murray, MD, PhD, for providing medical cover.
1.United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime World Drug Report 2007. New York, NY: United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime; 2007
2.Nutt
DKing
LASaulsbury
WBlakemore
C Development of a rational scale to assess the harm of drugs of potential misuse.
Lancet 2007;369
(9566)
1047- 1053
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction Cocaine and Crack Cocaine: A Growing Public Health Problem. Lisbon, Portugal: European Monitoring Centre for Drugs and Drug Addiction; 2007
4.Sayre
SLSchmitz
JMStotts
ALAverill
PMRhoades
HMGrabowski
JJ Determining predictors of attrition in an outpatient substance abuse program.
Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2002;28
(1)
55- 72
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.de Lima
MSde Oliveira Soares
BGReisser
AAFarrell
M Pharmacological treatment of cocaine dependence: a systematic review.
Addiction 2002;97
(8)
931- 949
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Kang
SYKleinman
PHWoody
GEMillman
RBTodd
TCKemp
JLipton
DS Outcomes for cocaine abusers after once-a-week psychosocial therapy.
Am J Psychiatry 1991;148
(5)
630- 635
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Goldstein
RZVolkow
ND Drug addiction and its underlying neurobiological basis: neuroimaging evidence for the involvement of the frontal cortex.
Am J Psychiatry 2002;159
(10)
1642- 1652
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Volkow
NDFowler
JSWang
GJ The addicted human brain viewed in the light of imaging studies: brain circuits and treatment strategies.
Neuropharmacology 2004;47(suppl 1)3- 13
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Bolla
KErnst
MKiehl
KMouratidis
MEldreth
DContoreggi
CMatochik
JKurian
VCadet
JKimes
AFunderburk
FLondon
E Prefrontal cortical dysfunction in abstinent cocaine abusers.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci 2004;16
(4)
456- 464
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Verdejo-García
APerez-Garcia
M Profile of executive deficits in cocaine and heroin polysubstance users: common and differential effects on separate executive components.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;190
(4)
517- 530
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Verdejo-García
AJPerales
JCPerez-Garcia
M Cognitive impulsivity in cocaine and heroin polysubstance abusers.
Addict Behav 2007;32
(5)
950- 966
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Streeter
CCTerhune
DBWhitfield
THGruber
SSarid-Segal
OSilveri
MMTzilos
GAfshar
MRouse
EDTian
HRenshaw
PFCiraulo
DAYurgelun-Todd
DA Performance on the Stroop predicts treatment compliance in cocaine-dependent individuals.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2008;33
(4)
827- 836
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Brewer
JAWorhunsky
PDCarroll
KMRounsaville
BJPotenza
MN Pretreatment brain activation during Stroop task is associated with outcomes in cocaine-dependent patients.
Biol Psychiatry 2008;64
(11)
998- 1004
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Goldstein
RZVolkow
NDWang
GJFowler
JSRajaram
S Addiction changes orbitofrontal gyrus function: involvement in response inhibition.
Neuroreport 2001;12
(11)
2595- 2599
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Toomey
RLyons
MJEisen
SAXian
HChantarujikapong
SSeidman
LJFaraone
SVTsuang
MT A twin study of the neuropsychological consequences of stimulant abuse.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60
(3)
303- 310
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Hoffman
WFMoore
MTemplin
RMcFarland
BHitzemann
RJMitchell
SH Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188
(2)
162- 170
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Salo
RNordahl
TEPossin
KLeamon
MGibson
DRGalloway
GPFlynn
NMHenik
APfefferbaum
ASullivan
EV Preliminary evidence of reduced cognitive inhibition in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.
Psychiatry Res 2002;111
(1)
65- 74
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Goldstein
RZTomasi
DRajaram
SCottone
LAZhang
LMaloney
TTelang
FAlia-Klein
NVolkow
ND Role of the anterior cingulate and medial orbitofrontal cortex in processing drug cues in cocaine addiction.
Neuroscience 2007;144
(4)
1153- 1159
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Hester
RDixon
VGaravan
H A consistent attentional bias for drug-related material in active cocaine users across word and picture versions of the emotional Stroop task.
Drug Alcohol Depend 2006;81
(3)
251- 257
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Carpenter
KMSchreiber
EChurch
SMcDowell
D Drug Stroop performance: relationships with primary substance of use and treatment outcome in a drug-dependent outpatient sample.
Addict Behav 2006;31
(1)
174- 181
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Vadhan
NPCarpenter
KMCopersino
MLHart
CLFoltin
RWNunes
EV Attentional bias towards cocaine-related stimuli: relationship to treatment-seeking for cocaine dependence.
Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2007;33
(5)
727- 736
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Copersino
MLSerper
MRVadhan
NGoldberg
BRRicharme
DChou
JCStitzer
MCancro
R Cocaine craving and attentional bias in cocaine-dependent schizophrenic patients.
Psychiatry Res 2004;128
(3)
209- 218
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Lubman
DIYucel
MPantelis
C Addiction, a condition of compulsive behaviour? neuroimaging and neuropsychological evidence of inhibitory dysregulation.
Addiction 2004;99
(12)
1491- 1502
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Cox
WMHogan
LMKristian
MRRace
JH Alcohol attentional bias as a predictor of alcohol abusers' treatment outcome.
Drug Alcohol Depend 2002;68
(3)
237- 243
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Marissen
MAEFranken
IHAWaters
AJBlanken
Pvan den Brink
WHendriks
VM Attentional bias predicts heroin relapse following treatment.
Addiction 2006;101
(9)
1306- 1312
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Waters
AJShiffman
SSayette
MAPaty
JAGwaltney
CJBalabanis
MH Attentional bias predicts outcome in smoking cessation.
Health Psychol 2003;22
(4)
378- 387
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Franken
IHAHendriks
VMStam
CJVan den Brink
W A role for dopamine in the processing of drug cues in heroin dependent patients.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol 2004;14
(6)
503- 508
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Hitsman
BMacKillop
JLingford-Hughes
AWilliams
TMAhmad
FAdams
SNutt
DJMunafò
MR Effects of acute tyrosine/phenylalanine depletion on the selective processing of smoking-related cues and the relative value of cigarettes in smokers.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;196
(4)
611- 621
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Munafò
MRMannie
ZNCowen
PJHarmer
CJMcTavish
SB Effects of acute tyrosine depletion on subjective craving and selective processing of smoking-related cues in abstinent cigarette smokers.
J Psychopharmacol 2007;21
(8)
805- 814
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Horvitz
JC Mesolimbocortical and nigrostriatal dopamine responses to salient non-reward events.
Neuroscience 2000;96
(4)
651- 656
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Volkow
NDWang
GJTelang
FFowler
JSLogan
JChildress
ARJayne
MMa
YWong
C Cocaine cues and dopamine in dorsal striatum: mechanism of craving in cocaine addiction.
J Neurosci 2006;26
(24)
6583- 6588
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Wong
DFKuwabara
HSchretlen
DJBonson
KRZhou
YNandi
ABrasić
JRKimes
ASMaris
MAKumar
AContoreggi
CLinks
JErnst
MRousset
OZukin
SGrace
AALee
JSRohde
CJasinski
DRGjedde
ALondon
ED Increased occupancy of dopamine receptors in human striatum during cue-elicited cocaine craving.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31
(12)
2716- 2727
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Martinez
DKim
JHKrystal
JAbi-Dargham
A Imaging the neurochemistry of alcohol and substance abuse.
Neuroimaging Clin N Am 2007;17
(4)
539- 555
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Volkow
NDFowler
JSWang
GJSwanson
JM Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results from imaging studies and treatment implications.
Mol Psychiatry 2004;9
(6)
557- 569
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Volkow
NDFowler
JSWang
GJSwanson
JMTelang
F Dopamine in drug abuse and addiction: results of imaging studies and treatment implications.
Arch Neurol 2007;64
(11)
1575- 1579
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Volkow
NDFowler
JSWang
GJHitzemann
RLogan
JSchlyer
DJDewey
SLWolf
AP Decreased dopamine-D(2) receptor availability is associated with reduced frontal metabolism in cocaine abusers.
Synapse 1993;14
(2)
169- 177
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Volkow
NDChang
LWang
GJFowler
JSDing
YSSedler
MLogan
JFranceschi
DGatley
JHitzemann
RGifford
AWong
CPappas
N Low level of brain dopamine D-2 receptors in methamphetamine abusers: association with metabolism in the orbitofrontal cortex.
Am J Psychiatry 2001;158
(12)
2015- 2021
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Martinez
DBroft
AFoltin
RWSlifstein
MHwang
DRHuang
YPerez
AFrankle
WGCooper
TKleber
HDFischman
MWLaruelle
MFrankel
WG Cocaine dependence and D-2 receptor availability in the functional subdivisions of the striatum: relationship with cocaine-seeking behavior.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2004;29
(6)
1190- 1202
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Volkow
NDFowler
JSWolf
APSchlyer
DShiue
CYAlpert
RDewey
SLLogan
JBendriem
BChristman
D
et al. Effects of chronic cocaine abuse on postsynaptic dopamine receptors.
Am J Psychiatry 1990;147
(6)
719- 724
PubMedGoogle Scholar 43.Everitt
BJHutcheson
DMErsche
KDPelloux
YDalley
JWRobbins
TW The orbital prefrontal cortex and drug addiction in laboratory animals and humans.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2007;1121576- 597
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Amato
LMinozzi
SPani
PPDavoli
M Antipsychotic medications for cocaine dependence.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2007;
(3)
CD006306
PubMedGoogle Scholar 45.Soares
BGLima
MSReisser
AAFarrell
M Dopamine agonists for cocaine dependence.
Cochrane Database Syst Rev 2003;
(2)
CD003352
PubMedGoogle Scholar 46.Grabowski
JShearer
JMerrill
JNegus
SS Agonist-like, replacement pharmacotherapy for stimulant abuse and dependence.
Addict Behav 2004;29
(7)
1439- 1464
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Elkashef
AVocci
FHanson
GWhite
JWickes
WTiihonen
J Pharmacotherapy of methamphetamine addiction: an update.
Subst Abus 2008;29
(3)
31- 49
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Moeller
FGBarratt
ESDougherty
DMSchmitz
JMSwann
AC Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity.
Am J Psychiatry 2001;158
(11)
1783- 1793
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Moeller
FGDougherty
DMBarratt
ESOderinde
VMathias
CWHarper
RASwann
AC Increased impulsivity in cocaine dependent subjects independent of antisocial personality disorder and aggression.
Drug Alcohol Depend 2002;68
(1)
105- 111
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Volkow
NDFowler
JS Addiction, a disease of compulsion and drive: involvement of the orbitofrontal cortex.
Cereb Cortex 2000;10
(3)
318- 325
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Cools
RSheridan
MJacobs
ED'Esposito
M Impulsive personality predicts dopamine-dependent changes in frontostriatal activity during component processes of working memory.
J Neurosci 2007;27
(20)
5506- 5514
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Dalley
JWFryer
TDBrichard
LRobinson
ESTheobald
DELääne
KPeña
YMurphy
ERShah
YProbst
KAbakumova
IAigbirhio
FIRichards
HKHong
YBaron
JCEveritt
BJRobbins
TW Nucleus accumbens D2/3 receptors predict trait impulsivity and cocaine reinforcement.
Science 2007;315
(5816)
1267- 1270
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Robbins
TWEveritt
BJ A role for mesencephalic dopamine in activation: commentary on Berridge (2006).
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2007;191
(3)
433- 437
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Everitt
BJRobbins
TW Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion.
Nat Neurosci 2005;8
(11)
1481- 1489
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.de Wit
H Impulsivity as a determinant and consequence of drug use: a review of underlying processes.
Addict Biol 2009;14
(1)
22- 31
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Dawe
SLoxton
NJ The role of impulsivity in the development of substance use and eating disorders.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004;28
(3)
343- 351
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Kalivas
PWVolkow
ND The neural basis of addiction: a pathology of motivation and choice.
Am J Psychiatry 2005;162
(8)
1403- 1413
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Evans
AHPavese
NLawrence
ADTai
YFAppel
SDoder
MBrooks
DJLees
AJPiccini
P Compulsive drug use linked to sensitized ventral striatal dopamine transmission.
Ann Neurol 2006;59
(5)
852- 858
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 2000
60.First
MBSpitzer
RLGibbon
MWilliams
JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition (SCID-I/NP). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 2002
61.Mierau
JSchneider
FJEnsinger
HAChio
CLLajiness
MEHuff
RM Pramipexole binding and activation of cloned and expressed dopamine D-2, D-3 and D-4 receptors.
Eur J Pharmacol 1995;290
(1)
29- 36
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Franken
IHHendriksa
VMvan den Brink
W Initial validation of two opiate craving questionnaires: the Obsessive Compulsive Drug Use Scale and the Desires for Drug Questionnaire.
Addict Behav 2002;27
(5)
675- 685
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Franken
IHAKroon
LYHendriks
VM Influence of individual differences in craving and obsessive cocaine thoughts on attentional processes in cocaine abuse patients.
Addict Behav 2000;25
(1)
99- 102
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Goodman
WKPrice
LHRasmussen
SAMazure
CFleischmann
RLHill
CLHeninger
GRCharney
DS The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, 1: development, use, and reliability.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46
(11)
1006- 1011
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Compton
RJBanich
MTMohanty
AMilham
MPHerrington
JMiller
GAScalf
PEWebb
AHeller
W Paying attention to emotion: an fMRI investigation of cognitive and emotional Stroop tasks.
Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2003;3
(2)
81- 96
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 67.Waters
AJSayette
MAFranken
IHASchwartz
JE Generalizability of carry-over effects in the emotional Stroop task.
Behav Res Ther 2005;43
(6)
715- 732
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.van den Heuvel
OAVeltman
DJGroenewegen
HJWitter
MPMerkelbach
JCath
DCvan Balkom
AJvan Oppen
Pvan Dyck
R Disorder-specific neuroanatomical correlates of attentional bias in obsessive-compulsive disorder, panic disorder, and hypochondriasis.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62
(8)
922- 933
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 69.Malhi
GSLagopoulos
JSachdev
PSIvanovski
BShnier
R An emotional Stroop functional MRI study of euthymic bipolar disorder.
Bipolar Disord 2005;7(suppl 5)58- 69
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Mitterschiffthaler
MTWilliams
SCRWalsh
NDCleare
AJDonaldson
CScott
JFu
CH Neural basis of the emotional Stroop interference effect in major depression.
Psychol Med 2008;38
(2)
247- 256
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Beck
ATSteer
RABrown
GK Manual for Beck Depression Inventory–II. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1996
72.Bullmore
ELong
CSuckling
JFadili
JCalvert
GZelaya
FCarpenter
TABrammer
M Colored noise and computational inference in neurophysiological (fMRI) time series analysis: resampling methods in time and wavelet domains.
Hum Brain Mapp 2001;12
(2)
61- 78
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 73.Suckling
JDavis
MHOoi
OWink
AMFadili
JSalvador
RWelchew
DSendur
LMaxim
VBullmore
ET Permutation testing of orthogonal factorial effects in a language-processing experiment using fMRI.
Hum Brain Mapp 2006;27
(5)
425- 433
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 74.Bullmore
ETBrammer
MJRabe-Hesketh
SCurtis
VAMorris
RGWilliams
SCSharma
TMcGuire
PK Methods for diagnosis and treatment of stimulus-correlated motion in generic brain activation studies using fMRI.
Hum Brain Mapp 1999;7
(1)
38- 48
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 78.Nelson
HE National Adult Reading Test Manual. Windsor, England: NFER-Nelson; 1982
79.Tzourio-Mazoyer
NLandeau
BPapathanassiou
DCrivello
FEtard
ODelcroix
NMazoyer
BJoliot
M Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain.
Neuroimage 2002;15
(1)
273- 289
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.Hester
RGaravan
H Neural mechanisms underlying drug-related cue distraction in active cocaine users.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2009;93
(3)
270- 277
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 81.Fadardi
JSCox
WM Alcohol attentional bias: drinking salience or cognitive impairment?
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;185
(2)
169- 178
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 82.Stetter
FAckermann
KBizer
AStraube
ERMann
K Effects of disease-related cues in alcoholic inpatients—results of a controlled alcohol Stroop study.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995;19
(3)
593- 599
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 83.Bremner
JDVermetten
EVythilingam
MAfzal
NSchmahl
CElzinga
BCharney
DS Neural correlates of the classic color and emotional Stroop in women with abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2004;55
(6)
612- 620
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 84.Sim
MELyoo
IKStreeter
CCCovell
JSarid-Segal
OCiraulo
DAKim
MJKaufman
MJYurgelun-Todd
DARenshaw
PF Cerebellar gray matter volume correlates with duration of cocaine use in cocaine-dependent subjects.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2007;32
(10)
2229- 2237
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 85.Egner
THirsch
J The neural correlates and functional integration of cognitive control in a Stroop task.
Neuroimage 2005;24
(2)
539- 547
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 86.Rocca
MAValsasina
PCeccarelli
AAbsinta
MGhezzi
ARiccitelli
GPagani
EFalini
AComi
GScotti
GFilippi
M Structural and functional MRI correlates of Stroop control in benign MS.
Hum Brain Mapp 2009;30
(1)
276- 290
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 87.Demb
JBDesmond
JEWagner
ADVaidya
CJGlover
GHGabrieli
JD Semantic encoding and retrieval in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a functional MRI study of task difficulty and process specificity.
J Neurosci 1995;15
(9)
5870- 5878
PubMedGoogle Scholar 88.Thompson-Schill
SLD'Esposito
MAguirre
GKFarah
MJ Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a re-evaluation.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;94
(26)
14792- 14797
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 89.Badre
DWagner
AD Left ventrolateral prefrontal cortex and the cognitive control of memory.
Neuropsychologia 2007;45
(13)
2883- 2901
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 90.Devlin
JTMatthews
PMRushworth
MFS Semantic processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation study.
J Cogn Neurosci 2003;15
(1)
71- 84
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 91.Gabrieli
JDEPoldrack
RADesmond
JE The role of left prefrontal cortex in language and memory.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1998;95
(3)
906- 913
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 92.Poldrack
RAWagner
ADPrull
MWDesmond
JEGlover
GHGabrieli
JDE Functional specialization for semantic and phonological processing in the left inferior prefrontal cortex.
Neuroimage 1999;10
(1)
15- 35
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 93.Shin
LMWhalen
PJPitman
RKBush
GMacklin
MLLasko
NBOrr
SPMcInerney
SCRauch
SL An fMRI study of anterior cingulate function in posttraumatic stress disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2001;50
(12)
932- 942
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 94.Garavan
HPankiewicz
JBloom
ACho
JKSperry
LRoss
TJSalmeron
BJRisinger
RKelley
DStein
EA Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli.
Am J Psychiatry 2000;157
(11)
1789- 1798
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 95.Kilts
CDGross
REEly
TDDrexler
KPG The neural correlates of cue-induced craving in cocaine-dependent women.
Am J Psychiatry 2004;161
(2)
233- 241
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 96.Volkow
NDWang
GJMa
YMFowler
JSZhu
WMaynard
LTelang
FVaska
PDing
YSWong
CSwanson
JM Expectation enhances the regional brain metabolic and the reinforcing effects of stimulants in cocaine abusers.
J Neurosci 2003;23
(36)
11461- 11468
PubMedGoogle Scholar 97.Narendran
RMartinez
D Cocaine abuse and sensitization of striatal dopamine transmission: a critical review of the preclinical and clinical imaging literature.
Synapse 2008;62
(11)
851- 869
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 98.Maj
JRogoz
ZSkuza
GKolodziejczyk
K The behavioural effects of pramipexole, a novel dopamine receptor agonist.
Eur J Pharmacol 1997;324
(1)
31- 37
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 99.Cannon
WB The emergency function of the adrenal medulla in pain and major emotions.
Am J Physiol 1914;33356- 372
Google Scholar 100.Mierau
JSchingnitz
G Biochemical and pharmacological studies on pramipexole, a potent and selective dopamine-D2 receptor agonist.
Eur J Pharmacol 1992;215
(2-3)
161- 170
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 101.Volkow
NDFowler
JSWang
GJBaler
RTelang
F Imaging dopamine's role in drug abuse and addiction.
Neuropharmacology 2009;56(suppl 1)3- 8
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 102.Seeman
P Dopamine D2High receptors measured ex vivo are elevated in amphetamine-sensitized animals.
Synapse 2009;63
(3)
186- 192
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 103.Hyman
SEMalenka
RC Addiction and the brain: the neurobiology of compulsion and its persistence.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2001;2
(10)
695- 703
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 104.Tiffany
ST A cognitive model of drug urges and drug-use behavior—role of automatic and nonautomatic processes.
Psychol Rev 1990;97
(2)
147- 168
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 105.Munafò
MMogg
KRoberts
SBradley
BPMurphy
M Selective processing of smoking-related cues in current smokers, ex-smokers and never-smokers on the modified Stroop task.
J Psychopharmacol 2003;17
(3)
310- 316
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 107.Perani
DGaribotto
VGorini
AMoresco
RMHenin
MPanzacchi
AMatarrese
MCarpinelli
ABellodi
LFazio
F In vivo PET study of 5HT2A serotonin and D2 dopamine dysfunction in drug-naive obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Neuroimage 2008;42
(1)
306- 314
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 108.Olver
JSO'Keefe
GJones
GRBurrows
GDTochon-Danguy
HJAckermann
UScott
ANorman
TR Dopamine D1 receptor binding in the striatum of patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder.
J Affect Disord 2009;114
(1-3)
321- 326
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 109.Denys
Dvan der Wee
NJanssen
JDe Geus
FWestenberg
HGM Low level of dopaminergic D2 receptor binding in obsessive-compulsive disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2004;55
(10)
1041- 1045
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 110.Burns
GLKeortge
SGFormea
GMSternberger
LG Revision of the Padua Inventory of obsessive compulsive disorder symptoms: distinctions between worry, obsessions, and compulsions.
Behav Res Ther 1996;34
(2)
163- 173
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref