Context
The Zinc Finger Protein 804A gene (ZNF804A) has been implicated in schizophrenia susceptibility by several genome-wide association studies. ZNF804A is brain expressed but of unknown function.
Objective
To investigate whether the identified risk allele at the disease-associated single nucleotide polymorphism rs1344706 is associated with variation in neuropsychological performance in patients and controls.
Design
Comparison of cases and controls grouped according to ZNF804A genotype (AA vs AC vs CC) on selected measures of cognition in 2 independent samples.
Setting
Unrelated patients from general adult psychiatric inpatient and outpatient services and unrelated healthy participants from the general population were ascertained.
Participants
Patients with DSM-IV–diagnosed schizophrenia and healthy participants from independent samples of Irish (297 cases and 165 controls) and German (251 cases and 1472 controls) nationality.
Main Outcome Measures
In this 2-stage study, we tested for an association between ZNF804A rs1344706 and cognitive functions known to be impaired in schizophrenia (IQ, episodic memory, working memory, and attention) in an Irish discovery sample. We then tested significant results in a German replication sample.
Results
In the Irish samples, the ZNF804A genotype was associated with differences in episodic and working memory in patients but not in controls. These findings replicated in the same direction in the German samples. Furthermore, in both samples, when patients with a lower IQ were excluded, the association between ZNF804A and schizophrenia strengthened.
Conclusions
In a disorder characterized by heterogeneity, a risk variant at ZNF804A seems to delineate a patient subgroup characterized by relatively spared cognitive ability. Further work is required to establish whether this represents a discrete molecular pathogenesis that differs from that of other patient groups and whether this also has consequences for nosologic classification, illness course, or treatment.
Schizophrenia has a lifetime risk of approximately 1% and is a major cause of global disability.1 Despite its substantial heritability (~ 80%),2 identifying the genetic variations responsible for schizophrenia has proved challenging, as with other nonpsychiatric complex disorders.3,4 A recent genome-wide association study (GWAS)5 identified the single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs1344706 located at gene ZNF804A (OMIM 612282) as achieving genome-wide significance for psychosis (9.96 × 10−9 [odds ratio, 1.12]). Despite being relatively underpowered to replicate such a modest effect, 2 of 3 recently reported large schizophrenia GWASs6-8 supported association with the same A risk allele.
The SNP rs1344706 is located in an intron of ZNF804A that maps to chromosome 2q32.1. The human ZNF804A gene consists of 4 exons that transcribe a protein of 1210 amino acids with a predicted molecular weight of 137 kDa. The encoded protein is uncharacterized, but analysis of the protein sequence shows a zinc finger domain at the N-terminal end, suggesting that it may bind DNA and have a role in regulating gene expression. The Allen Mouse Brain Atlas indicates that the mouse orthologue Zfp804A is widely expressed in the brain.9 Lim et al10 demonstrated using a yeast 2-hybrid system that ZNF804A bound ataxin-1, which is encoded by ATXN1 (OMIM 601556). Mutations in this gene cause spinocerebellar ataxia-1, and it has been implicated, albeit inconclusively, in schizophrenia by several small studies (http://www.schizophreniaforum.org/res/sczgene/geneoverview.asp?geneid=354).
In their schizophrenia GWAS, Stefansson et al6 identified an association with genes involved in brain development and cognition. Neurocognitive deficits are core features of schizophrenia and may better represent the underlying pathogenesis than clinical diagnostic categories.11 Regardless of whether the measurement of these deficits increases power to detect an association with psychiatric risk genes,12 they have the unique advantage of enabling in vivo functional investigation of candidate genes at the level of brain behavior in large samples of patients and healthy participants. The utility of such an approach has been demonstrated by findings with existing candidate genes for schizophrenia (including DISC1, NRG1, DTNBP1, DAOA[G72], and RGS4, discussed in the study by O’Donovan et al13). No neuropsychological investigation of ZNF804A has occurred to date, to our knowledge, although evidence that ZNF804A is likely to affect brain function derives from a recent study by Esslinger et al.14 This study, investigating the same risk allele at rs1344706, identified an association with altered connectivity in healthy participants within and between regions that included the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and the hippocampus. No behavioral sequelae of this altered connectivity were associated with this finding, although the sample size (n = 115) may have been underpowered to detect such differences.
In this study, we investigated the effect on cognition of the A risk allele at rs1344706, the variant showing strongest evidence of association in the GWAS by O’Donovan et al.5 Neuropsychological assessment was designed to measure cognitive functions known to be impaired in schizophrenia: IQ, working memory, episodic memory, and attention. The neuropsychological tests examined in the different samples were selected a priori to be as equivalent as possible. To enable a clear investigation of the effects of ZNF804A variation on cognition, we focused on a single statistically significantly strong risk variant, used a large discovery sample, and implemented a study design that allowed replication of findings in an independent data set.
Irish Patient and Control Samples (Discovery Samples)
These samples consisted of 297 cases and 165 controls. Seventy-one of the case participants underwent genotyping as part of the previous GWAS,5 and the remaining 226 case participants and all the controls were independent of that study. Cases consisted of clinically stable patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia recruited from 5 sites across Ireland. The inclusion criteria required that participants were aged 18 to 65 years, had no history of substance abuse in the preceding 6 months, and had no previous head injury with loss of consciousness or a history of seizures. Diagnosis was confirmed by trained psychiatrists using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis 1 Disorders (SCID).15 Additional diagnostic details and clinical sample characteristics ascertained at the time of interview included symptom severity (Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms/Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms16) and medication dosage.
The control sample was recruited on the basis of responses to local media advertisements. Control participants were included only if they were aged 18 to 65 years and satisfied, based on clinical interview, the criteria of having no history of major mental health problems, intellectual disability, or acquired brain injury and no history of substance misuse in the preceding 6 months based on self-report. Control participants were also excluded from the study if they reported having a first-degree relative with a history of psychosis. All patient and control assessments were conducted in accordance with the relevant ethics committee approval from each participating site. All patients and controls were of Irish ancestry (ie, 4 grandparents born in Ireland), and all provided written informed consent.
German Patient and Control Samples (Replication Samples)
The German samples consisted of 251 clinically stable patients with a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia and up to 1472 controls, all of whom underwent genotyping as part of the previous study.5 Patients were ascertained from mental health services in the Munich area, and all the participants provided written informed consent. The inclusion criteria were a diagnosis of schizophrenia (>6-month symptom duration) and age 18 to 65 years. The exclusion criteria included a history of head injury or neurologic diseases. Detailed medical and psychiatric histories were collected, including data from a clinical interview using the SCID,15 to evaluate lifetime Axis I and II diagnoses. Four physicians and 1 psychologist rated the SCID interviews, and all the measurements were independently rated by a senior researcher. Participants were also rated for symptoms using the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale.17 In cases, 68% were of German descent (all 4 grandparents born in Germany), and the other 32% were German white. Of the 251 participants with schizophrenia, 241 completed the full Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale–Revised (WAIS-R) assessment and 235 completed a comprehensive neuropsychological battery.
Control participants of German descent (all 4 grandparents German) were randomly selected from the general population of Munich. Participants were contacted and invited to participate by mail. The address, sex, and age of potential participants were supplied by the municipal registration office, which enabled equal numbers of males and females and people of different ages (in decades) to be approached. Controls for this study were included only if aged 18 to 65 years. To exclude individuals with central neurologic diseases and psychotic disorders or those who had first-degree relatives with psychotic disorders, several screenings were conducted before the volunteers were enrolled in the study. First, individuals who responded were initially screened by telephone for the absence of neuropsychiatric disorders. Second, detailed medical and psychiatric histories were assessed for participants and their first-degree relatives by using a semistructured interview. Third, if no exclusion criteria were fulfilled, they were invited to a comprehensive interview including the SCID15 to validate the absence of any lifetime psychotic disorder. In addition, the Family History Assessment Module18 was conducted to exclude psychotic disorders in their first-degree relatives. A neurologic examination was also conducted to exclude individuals with current central nervous system impairment. In the case of volunteers older than 60 years, the Mini-Mental-Status-Test19 was performed to exclude individuals with possible cognitive impairment. Of the 1472 control participants, 1470 completed the full WAIS-R assessment and 367 completed tests from a further extensive neuropsychological battery.
This study was designed so that identical or near-identical tests of the cognitive domains of IQ, episodic memory, working memory, and attention were used for the Irish discovery samples and the German samples. The number of individual tests in each domain of cognition was limited to minimize multiple testing effects. The Irish discovery sample was used to test for genotypic associations with these tests. Where significant (P ≤ .05) associations were detected, these phenotypes were taken forward to the German sample for replication.
IQ was measured in the Irish sample using selected subtests (vocabulary, similarities, block design, and matrix reasoning) from the WAIS, third edition,20 yielding a full-scale verbal and performance IQ. For the German sample, IQ was indexed using the German version of the WAIS-R21 and all 11 verbal and performance subtests (vocabulary, comprehension, information, digit span, arithmetic, similarities, block design, picture completion, picture arrangement, object assembly, and digit symbol and coding). Episodic memory was assessed in the Irish samples using the logical memory immediate and delayed from the Wechsler Memory Scale, third edition (WMS-III),22 and in the German sample using the logical memory immediate and delayed from the German version of the WMS-Revised.23,24
Verbal and spatial working memory were assessed in the Irish samples using the Wechsler Letter Number Sequencing task (WMS-III)22 and the spatial working memory task from the Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery.25 In the German samples, working memory was measured using the digit span from the WAIS-R21 and the spatial span from the WMS-R.23,24 Vigilant attention was assessed in the Irish patient samples using the Continuous Performance Test (CPT), identical pairs version,26 and in the German samples using the CPT 3-7 version.27 Insufficient numbers of Irish controls completed this task, making it unavailable for this group. The memory and attention tasks used herein have been described in detail elsewhere.28
The SNP rs1344706 was genotyped in the German sample and in a proportion of the Irish sample using the Sequenom iPLEX Gold system (further details are provided by O’Donovan et al5). The call rate for the iPLEX genotyping was greater than 95% in the Irish sample and greater than 99% in the German sample. Both case and control samples were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (P > .05). The remainder of the Irish sample (n = 529) underwent genotyping using a TaqMan SNP Genotyping Assay on a 7900HT Sequence Detection System (Applied Biosystems Inc, Foster City, California). The call rate for the TaqMan genotyping was greater than 95%, and both case and control samples were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (P > .05). Along with the Irish samples, several HapMap CEU DNA samples (n = 90 [http://www.hapmap.org]) and duplicates (n = 60) were genotyped for rs1344706 for quality control purposes. All the genotypes were found to be concordant with either the available online HapMap data or each other for this SNP.
To inform appropriate adjustments in the primary cognitive analyses, the association between ZNF804A rs1344706 and demographic variables was investigated using 1-way analysis of variance. In the case of symptom severity, a PCA (principal components analysis) was undertaken separately in each of the 3 samples based on Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms/Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms scores in the Irish sample and Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale scores in the German sample (see eTable 1 and eTable 2 for factor descriptions; differences associated with genotype were analyzed using multivariate analysis of covariance in which age and sex were included as covariates.
Associations between ZNF804A rs1344706 and the phenotypes of IQ, episodic memory, working memory, and attention were tested using a general factorial design in a statistical software program (SPSS 14).29 In the original GWAS there was no difference in genotypic vs allelic model; because there was no evidence on which to test a specific dominant or recessive model and as sample sizes allowed, the analysis was based on a comparison of all 3 genotype groups. ZNF804A genotype (AA vs AC vs CC) and diagnosis (cases vs controls) were entered as fixed effects. In a series of analyses of variance, scores for each neuropsychological subtest were entered as dependent variables, with age and sex included as covariates as appropriate. Significant interaction effects were further explored by examining simple effects in cases and controls. Tests showing significant results in the Irish samples were then taken forward to the same analyses in the German samples.
ZNF804A AND DEMOGRAPHIC AND CLINICAL VARIABLES
Demographic and clinical characteristics by rs1344706 genotype appear in Table 1. In the Irish sample, no differences were observed in age, years of education, or sex between the genotype groups for either the full sample or when patients and controls were considered separately. In terms of clinical symptom severity, no difference was observed between genotype groups for PCA-derived negative or disorganized symptoms. For positive symptom severity, a trend-level difference between groups was observed (F = 2.31, P = .06); mean values for the 3 genotype groups suggested that the homozygous carriers of the A risk allele presented somewhat higher positive scores than the other genotype groups. This trend was observed in the absence of any association between genotype and medication dose.
For the German sample, no significant differences were apparent in age, sex distribution, or years of education by genotype in cases or controls (Table 1). In terms of clinical severity, a significant difference was observed for the PCA-derived depressive factor (F = 4.61, P = .01). Pairwise comparisons revealed that the CC genotype scored higher on this depressive factor than the AC (t = 2.19, P = .03) and AA (t = 3.06, P = .002) genotype groups. Genotype was not associated with any other symptom factor score, and neither were any differences in medication dosage by genotype observed.
COGNITIVE ANALYSIS OF ZNF804A
Mean scores for each of the 4 cognitive domains of IQ, working memory, episodic memory, and attention by ZNF804A genotype group for cases and controls in the Irish discovery sample are given in Table 2. As expected, patients performed significantly below controls on all cognitive tests administered in the Irish and German samples (P < .001 for all).
For the Irish samples, a significant interaction between ZNF804A genotype and case or control status revealed associations with variance on both working memory tasks in patients but not in controls (verbal working memory: F = 4.2, P = .02; spatial working memory: F = 5.04, P = −.007) (Table 2). Effect size estimates (partial η2) indicated that in cases, genotype explains 2.8% of verbal working memory and 4.4% of spatial working memory. Tukey post hoc analyses within the patient group revealed that the homozygous risk AA genotype performed significantly better than the homozygous CC genotype group (verbal working memory: H = 1.53, P = .046; spatial working memory: H = 11.38, P = .045). Finally, although a small trend for a main effect of genotype on spatial working memory was apparent, this seemed to be primarily driven by cases; separate analysis of controls revealed no such trend.
A significant association between ZNF804A genotype and verbal episodic memory was also observed in patients and not in controls for immediate and, to a lesser extent, delayed verbal memory. Effect size estimates indicated that in cases, genotype explains 2.7% of immediate logical memory scores and 1.1% of delayed verbal memory scores (Table 2). For immediate verbal episodic memory, Tukey post hoc analysis revealed that this difference was driven by homozygous (H = 1.86, P = .01) and heterozygous (H = 1.83, P = .02) risk carriers, again performing significantly better than homozygous nonrisk carriers. For delayed verbal episodic memory, the effect of genotype was smaller, and post hoc analysis did not reveal specific between-group differences.
Finally, scores for vigilant attention (the CPT task) were available for patients but not for controls. Based on analysis of covariance, again with age and sex used as covariates, we found no effect of ZNF804A genotype on CPT in terms of the 2- and 3-letter conditions (Table 2).
German Replication Sample
The significant results from the Irish samples were taken forward to the replication samples (Table 3). The results from the Irish sample replicated in the German sample cases for all the cognitive tests: verbal and spatial working memory and episodic memory. Furthermore, the allelic direction of effect was the same in that those with AA genotype performed better than AC and CC genotypes on all significant tests. For spatial and episodic memory, Tukey post hoc analysis confirmed that this was mainly driven by a significant difference between AA and CC genotypes (spatial working memory: H = 2.39, P = .047; immediate episodic memory: H = 2.81, P = .02; delayed episodic memory: H = 3.12, P = .009). In the case of verbal working memory, inter–genotype group differences were observed between homozygous risk carriers and heterozygous risk carriers only (H = 2.67, P = .02). Effect size estimates, again based on partial η2, indicated that in cases, genotype explains 3.3% of the variance in spatial working memory, 3.1% in verbal working memory, and 3% in immediate and delayed episodic memory.
ANALYSIS OF WHETHER THE ZNF804A-SCHIZOPHRENIA ASSOCIATION IS MODERATED BY COGNITION
Given the counterintuitive evidence that carriers of the ZNF804A risk genotype presented less impaired cognitive performance than did noncarriers, we tested the post hoc hypothesis that ZNF804A was delineating a subgroup of patients characterized by relatively intact cognitive performance. To do so, we used χ2 statistics to calculate the association between ZNF804A and schizophrenia in subsamples with higher cognitive ability. We selected IQ rather than memory performance for this analysis so as to provide a more general index of cognitive ability than that provided by memory function. Higher cognitive ability was based on IQ scores of at least 70, at least 80, at least 90, at least 100, at least 110, and at least 120. In the Irish and German samples, as the phenotype was narrowed to individuals with an IQ in the average range, the association between ZNF804A and schizophrenia became more statistically significant (Table 4, Table 5, and the Figure). For example, in the Irish samples, as the minimum IQ for inclusion approaches 90 (the range for normal IQ), the odds ratio of the allelic association between ZNF804A and schizophrenia increases from 1.37 (when all samples are included) to 2.29 (95% confidence interval, 1.41-3.72). In the German sample, the same trend of increasing association between ZNF804A and schizophrenia also emerges, becoming statistically significant for patients with high average IQ (IQ > 110; odds ratio, 1.63; 95% confidence interval, 1.12-2.38). Conducting this analysis based on memory scores instead of IQ revealed the same pattern of results.
Regarding its credibility as a risk variant, the SNP at ZNF804A is the first variant to have achieved genome-wide significance for psychosis, with the association replicating in multiple independent samples. Despite the modest effect size, 2 independent GWASs7,8 have provided support for an association with the same risk allele. Although this finding may have a very small effect on disease risk, it is potentially important to our understanding of the genetic mechanisms and pathways that contribute to susceptibility. Little is known about the function of ZNF804A, and this study sought to elucidate the phenotypic effects of the identified risk allele on indices of neuropsychological function. The design of this study sought to overcome weaknesses of earlier gene cognition studies (eg, small sample size and high multiple testing burden) by investigating a single GWAS significant variant and then seeking to replicate significant findings in a large independent data set using comparable cognitive tests. In this study, carrying the risk allele at the SNP rs1344706 was associated with variation in cognitive performance in patients. Specifically, schizophrenia carriers of the AA risk genotype performed relatively better on measures of episodic memory and working memory based on 2 independent Irish and German samples that had contributed to the original GWAS finding.
Although the observed association with cognition observed may seem counterintuitive, it is important to note that the risk allele at ZNF804A is not so much associated with better cognitive performance in the present study as with less impaired cognitive performance. We interpret these data to mean that ZNF804A is delineating an illness susceptibility pathway that is independent of an effect on cognition and, hence, is characterized by relatively spared cognitive ability compared with other patient subgroups whose pathway into illness is being mediated by a greater burden of more cognitively deleterious gene variants.30 Support for this view derives from the following: first, the association is seen only for cases; if ZNF804A was having a positive effect on cognitive performance, this should also be apparent in controls, but this is not the case; and second, in the association analysis between ZNF804A and schizophrenia, when cases with lower cognitive ability are excluded, the association signal strengthens, indicating that the relationship between ZNF804A and psychosis is particularly apparent in those with relatively intact cognitive function. This finding that ZNF804A may encode for a cognitively spared psychosis subtype complements earlier evidence in the original GWAS5 that inclusion of bipolar patients (a patient group typically associated with less severe cognitive impairments31) also led to a strengthening of its association with psychosis. The fact that the association is found in these 2 independently collected samples of individuals with schizophrenia also counteracts the argument that ZNF804A is, in fact, a gene for bipolar disorder.
By elucidating the role of ZNF804A in delineating a schizophrenia subtype characterized by relatively intact cognitive performance, this study counters the expectation that it is impaired cognition that will be informative about the relationship between a risk gene and a disease phenotype.32 This finding is not unique, however. Recent studies of PPP1R1B, encoding DARPP-32, and CHI3L1 found that the schizophrenia-associated risk alleles at both gene loci were associated with a relatively spared performance.30,33 Given the heterogeneity of the schizophrenia syndrome and the fact that it is possible to be schizophrenic and cognitively intact,34,35 it is perhaps to be expected that not all identified susceptibility genes will have detrimental effects on broad cognitive abilities.
In contravening the typical expectations for intermediate phenotypes (of not being associated with poorer function), an obvious concern is that the observed associations might spuriously result from 1 or more confounding demographic or clinical variables in the patient group. We examined this possibility using a variety of demographic and clinical indicators, including age, medication, sex, education, and clinical symptom severity. No differences between genotype groups on any of these variables were observed. One factor that might potentially have affected these results was the above-average IQ of the Irish controls. We presume that this bias resulted from the ascertainment strategy of advertising for participants via local media advertisements, which may inadvertently have appealed to higher IQ groups. However, IQ in the German control sample was in the average range, and post hoc analysis of this variable did not reveal significant differences associated with genotype. Furthermore, in the 1 other study28 based on exactly the same samples reported herein, the effect of genotype on cognitive ability was apparent in case and control samples in the Irish and German data sets. The impact of ZNF804A variation being apparent only in cases in the Irish and German data sets is, therefore, unlikely to be explainable purely in terms of this effect.
An important context for interpreting these data is provided by the recent imaging study of the same genetic variant reported by Esslinger et al.14 In a sample of controls, they found that the ZNF804A risk genotype was associated with altered connectivity in the DLPFC, the hippocampus, and the amygdala. Altered connectivity within and between these brain regions has been associated with schizophrenia; its association with ZNF804A provided the first evidence of the gene's functional involvement in brain activity. The fact that the 2 aspects of cognition implicated in the present study, episodic and working memory, are the aspects of cognition subserved by the brain regions identified by Esslinger et al,14 the DLPFC and the hippocampus, again implicates ZNF804A in biological processes relevant to these regions. However, there are 2 caveats to this interpretation. First, Esslinger et al14 observed an association between ZNF804A and altered brain connectivity in healthy participants but no association between the risk variant and neuropsychological performance in this population. In the present study, we similarly found no association between ZNF804A variation and neuropsychological performance in healthy participants but did find an association in patients. Second, Esslinger et al14 demonstrated enhanced functional connectivity between the DLPFC and the hippocampus. Although hypothesized to have a deleterious effect on cognition, the true functional significance of this finding for cognition and psychosis is unknown because patients were not included in that study. Speculatively, it is possible that the increased connectivity between the hippocampus and the DLPFC observed by Esslinger et al14 and previously described by Meyer-Lindenberg et al36 could represent a neural mechanism that spares episodic and working memory in patients by allowing processing of memory information in both structures. Because this is not necessary in controls (in whom these structures are unimpaired), the associated behavioral advantage would be evident in the patient group only, as seen herein. Testing this hypothesis will require imaging-based connectivity analysis in patient groups and, therefore, represents an important next step in linking these brain imaging and neuropsychological findings.
In conclusion, these findings have potential relevance for the nosologic classification of schizophrenia and related psychotic disorders, particularly given that the genetic evidence for traditional clinical dichotomies is a subject of considerable discussion.37,38 The results of the present study support the approach of subgrouping patients with schizophrenia to better understand molecular and biological processes, as has been done for other complex genetic diseases (eg, breast cancer39). These findings suggest that individuals with less compromised cognitive functioning may show a different pattern of association or may even be a genetically distinct group worthy of further study in genetic association studies. In light of the stronger association between ZNF804A in the combined schizophrenia-bipolar sample in the original GWAS,5 the present data suggest that ZNF804A is indexing a psychosis pathway defined by cognitive rather than diagnostic characteristics. If confirmed, defining the molecular etiology involved in this group may have important diagnostic, prognostic, and therapeutic implications.
Correspondence: Gary Donohoe, DClinPsych, PhD, Neuropsychiatric Genetics Research Group, Department of Psychiatry, Trinity Health Sciences Bldg, St James's Hospital, Dublin 8, Ireland (Gary.Donohoe@tcd.ie).
Submitted for Publication: July 15, 2009; final revision received October 20, 2009; accepted December 14, 2009.
Financial Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: Genotyping of the German sample was funded by grants from the Medical Research Council and Wellcome Trust. Recruitment and genotyping of the Irish sample was supported by the Wellcome Trust and Science Foundation Ireland. Dr Donohoe was supported by a Science Foundation Ireland Research Frontiers grant and a NARSAD Young Investigator Award. Dr Walters was supported by an MRC/Welsh Assembly clinical research training fellowship. Drs Jablensky and Kaladjieva were supported by grants 404046 and 513874 from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council.
Role of the Sponsor: GlaxoSmithKline had no role in the design and conduct of the study; in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; or in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.
Additional Contributions: We are grateful to the Genetics Research Centre GmbH, an initiative by GlaxoSmithKline and Ludwig-Maximilians-University. We thank all the patients who contributed to this study and all the staff who facilitated their involvement. Recruitment of the patients from Munich was partially supported by GlaxoSmithKline.
1.Prince
MPatel
VSaxena
SMaj
MMaselko
JPhillips
MRRahman
A No health without mental health.
Lancet 2007;370
(9590)
859- 877
PubMedGoogle Scholar 2.Cardno
AGGottesman
II Twin studies of schizophrenia: from bow-and-arrow concordances to Star Wars Mx and functional genomics.
Am J Med Genet 2000;97
(1)
12- 17
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Sanders
ARDuan
JLevinson
DFShi
JHe
DHou
CBurrell
GJRice
JPNertney
DAOlincy
ARozic
PVinogradov
SBuccola
NGMowry
BJFreedman
RAmin
FBlack
DWSilverman
JMByerley
WFCrowe
RRCloninger
CRMartinez
MGejman
PV No significant association of 14 candidate genes with schizophrenia in a large European ancestry sample: implications for psychiatric genetics.
Am J Psychiatry 2008;165
(4)
497- 506
PubMedGoogle Scholar 4.Todd
JA Statistical false positive or true disease pathway?
Nat Genet 2006;38
(7)
731- 733
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.O'Donovan
MCCraddock
NNorton
NWilliams
HPeirce
TMoskvina
VNikolov
IHamshere
MCarroll
LGeorgieva
LDwyer
SHolmans
PMarchini
JLSpencer
CCHowie
BLeung
HTHartmann
AMMöller
HJMorris
DWShi
YFeng
GHoffmann
PPropping
PVasilescu
CMaier
WRietschel
MZammit
SSchumacher
JQuinn
EMSchulze
TGWilliams
NMGiegling
IIwata
NIkeda
MDarvasi
AShifman
SHe
LDuan
JSanders
ARLevinson
DFGejman
PVCichon
SNöthen
MMGill
MCorvin
ARujescu
DKirov
GOwen
MJBuccola
NGMowry
BJFreedman
RAmin
FBlack
DWSilverman
JMByerley
WFCloninger
CRMolecular Genetics of Schizophrenia Collaboration, Identification of loci associated with schizophrenia by genome-wide association and follow-up.
Nat Genet 2008;40
(9)
1053- 1055
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Stefansson
HOphoff
RASteinberg
SAndreassen
OACichon
SRujescu
DWerge
TPietiläinen
OPMors
OMortensen
PBSigurdsson
EGustafsson
ONyegaard
MTuulio-Henriksson
AIngason
AHansen
TSuvisaari
JLonnqvist
JPaunio
TBørglum
ADHartmann
AFink-Jensen
ANordentoft
MHougaard
DNorgaard-Pedersen
BBöttcher
YOlesen
JBreuer
RMöller
HJGiegling
IRasmussen
HBTimm
SMattheisen
MBitter
IRéthelyi
JMMagnusdottir
BBSigmundsson
TOlason
PMasson
GGulcher
JRHaraldsson
MFossdal
RThorgeirsson
TEThorsteinsdottir
URuggeri
MTosato
SFranke
BStrengman
EKiemeney
LAMelle
IDjurovic
SAbramova
LKaleda
VSanjuan
Jde Frutos
RBramon
EVassos
EFraser
GEttinger
UPicchioni
MWalker
NToulopoulou
TNeed
ACGe
DYoon
JLShianna
KVFreimer
NBCantor
RMMurray
RKong
AGolimbet
VCarracedo
AArango
CCostas
JJönsson
EGTerenius
LAgartz
IPetursson
HNöthen
MMRietschel
MMatthews
PMMuglia
PPeltonen
LSt Clair
DGoldstein
DBStefansson
KCollier
DAGenetic Risk and Outcome in Psychosis (GROUP), Common variants conferring risk of schizophrenia.
Nature 2009;460
(7256)
744- 747
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Purcell
SMWray
NRStone
JLVisscher
PMO’Donovan
MCSullivan
PFSklar
PInternational Schizophrenia Consortium, Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
Nature 2009;460
(7256)
748- 752
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.Shi
JLevinson
DFDuan
JSanders
ARZheng
YPe’er
IDudbridge
FHolmans
PAWhittemore
ASMowry
BJOlincy
AAmin
FCloninger
CRSilverman
JMBuccola
NGByerley
WFBlack
DWCrowe
RROksenberg
JRMirel
DBKendler
KSFreedman
RGejman
PV Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia.
Nature 2009;460
(7256)
753- 757
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.Lim
JHao
TShaw
CPatel
AJSzabó
GRual
JFFisk
CJLi
NSmolyar
AHill
DEBarabási
ALVidal
MZoghbi
HY A protein-protein interaction network for human inherited ataxias and disorders of Purkinje cell degeneration.
Cell 2006;125
(4)
801- 814
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Gur
RECalkins
MEGur
RCHoran
WPNuechterlein
KHSeidman
LJStone
WS The Consortium on the Genetics of Schizophrenia: neurocognitive endophenotypes.
Schizophr Bull 2007;33
(1)
49- 68
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Gottesman
IIGould
TD The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions.
Am J Psychiatry 2003;160
(4)
636- 645
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.O'Donovan
MCCraddock
NOwen
M Schizophrenia: complex genetics, not fairy tales.
Psychol Med 2008;38
(12)
1697- 1699, discussion 1818-1820
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Esslinger
CWalter
HKirsch
PErk
SSchnell
KArnold
CHaddad
LMier
DOpitz von Boberfeld
CRaab
KWitt
SHRietschel
MCichon
SMeyer-Lindenberg
A Neural mechanisms of a genome-wide supported psychosis variant.
Science 2009;324
(5927)
605
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.First
MBSpitzer
RLGibbon
MWilliams
JB Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Patient Edition. New York: Biometrics Research Department, New York State Psychiatric Institute; 1995
16.Andreasen
NC Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms/Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (Manual). Iowa City: University of Iowa Press; 1984
17.Kay
SRFiszbein
AOpler
LA The Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull 1987;13
(2)
261- 276
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Rice
JPReich
TBucholz
KKNeuman
RJFishman
RRochberg
NHesselbrock
VMNurnberger
JI
JrSchuckit
MABegleiter
H Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1995;19
(4)
1018- 1023
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Folstein
MFFolstein
SEMcHugh
PR Mini-Mental-Status-Test: German Version. Weinheim, Germany: Beltz; 1990
20.Wechsler
D Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Third Edition (WAIS-III). New York, NY: Psychological Corp; 1997
21.Tewes
U HAWIE-R Hamburg-Wechsler Intelligenztest für Erwachsene. Goettingen, Germany: Hogrefe; 1991
22.Wechsler
D Wechsler Memory Scale, Third Edition (WMS-III). New York, NY: Psychological Corp; 1998
23.Härting
CMarkowitsch
HJNeufeld
HCalabrese
PDeisinger
KKessler
J Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised; German Version: Wechsler Gedächtnistest; Revidierte Fassung. Bern, Switzerland: Huber; 2000
24.Wechsler
D Wechsler Memory Scale–Revised. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corp; 1987
25.Cognition
C Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery, Expedio Version (CANTABexpedio). Cambridge, England: Cambridge Cognition Ltd; 2003
26.Cornblatt
BARisch
NJFaris
GFriedman
DErlenmeyerkimling
L The Continuous Performance Test, identical pairs version (CPT-IP), 1: new findings about sustained attention in normal families.
Psychiatry Res 1988;26
(2)
223- 238
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Nuechterlein
KAsarnow
R 3-7 Continuous Performance Test. Los Angeles: University of California; 2004
28.Donohoe
GWalters
JMorris
DWQuinn
EMJudge
RNorton
NGiegling
IHartmann
AMMöller
HJMuglia
PWilliams
HMoskvina
VPeel
RO'Donoghue
TOwen
MJO'Donovan
MCGill
MRujescu
DCorvin
A Influence of NOS1 on verbal intelligence and working memory in both patients with schizophrenia and healthy control subjects.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66
(10)
1045- 1054
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.SPSS Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) Version 14.0. Chicago, IL: SPSS Inc; 2005
30.Yang
MSMorris
DWDonohoe
GKenny
EO’Dushalaine
CTSchwaiger
SNangle
JMClarke
SScully
PQuinn
JMeagher
DBaldwin
PCrumlish
NO’Callaghan
EWaddington
JLGill
MCorvin
A Chitinase-3-like 1 (
CHI3L1) gene and schizophrenia: genetic association and a potential functional mechanism.
Biol Psychiatry 2008;64
(2)
98- 103
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Schretlen
DJCascella
NGMeyer
SMKingery
LRTesta
SMMunro
CAPulver
AERivkin
PRao
VADiaz-Asper
CMDickerson
FBYolken
RHPearlson
GD Neuropsychological functioning in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry 2007;62
(2)
179- 186
PubMedGoogle Scholar 32.Walters
JTOwen
MJ Endophenotypes in psychiatric genetics.
Mol Psychiatry 2007;12
(10)
886- 890
PubMedGoogle Scholar 33.Meyer-Lindenberg
AStraub
RELipska
BKVerchinski
BAGoldberg
TCallicott
JHEgan
MFHuffaker
SSMattay
VSKolachana
BKleinman
JEWeinberger
DR Genetic evidence implicating DARPP-32 in human frontostriatal structure, function, and cognition.
J Clin Invest 2007;117
(3)
672- 682
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.Palmer
BWHeaton
RKPaulsen
JSKuck
JBraff
DHarris
MJZisook
SJeste
DV Is it possible to be schizophrenic yet neuropsychologically normal?
Neuropsychology 1997;11
(3)
437- 446
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Goldstein
GShemansky
WJAllen
DN Cognitive function in schizoaffective disorder and clinical subtypes of schizophrenia.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol 2005;20
(2)
153- 159
PubMedGoogle Scholar 36.Meyer-Lindenberg
ASOlsen
RKKohn
PDBrown
TEgan
MFWeinberger
DRBerman
KF Regionally specific disturbance of dorsolateral prefrontal-hippocampal functional connectivity in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62
(4)
379- 386
PubMedGoogle Scholar 37.Owen
MJCraddock
N Diagnosis of functional psychoses: time to face the future.
Lancet 2009;373
(9659)
190- 191
PubMedGoogle Scholar 38.Craddock
NO'Donovan
MCOwen
MJ Psychosis genetics: modeling the relationship between schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and mixed (or “schizoaffective”) psychoses.
Schizophr Bull 2009;35
(3)
482- 490
PubMedGoogle Scholar 39.Garcia-Closas
MHall
PNevanlinna
HPooley
KMorrison
JRichesson
DABojesen
SENordestgaard
BGAxelsson
CKArias
JIMilne
RLRibas
GGonzález-Neira
ABenítez
JZamora
PBrauch
HJustenhoven
CHamann
UKo
YDBruening
THaas
SDörk
TSchürmann
PHillemanns
PBogdanova
NBremer
MKarstens
JHFagerholm
RAaltonen
KAittomäki
Kvon Smitten
KBlomqvist
CMannermaa
AUusitupa
MEskelinen
MTengström
MKosma
VMKataja
VChenevix-Trench
GSpurdle
ABBeesley
JChen
XDevilee
Pvan Asperen
CJJacobi
CETollenaar
RAHuijts
PEKlijn
JGChang-Claude
JKropp
SSlanger
TFlesch-Janys
DMutschelknauss
ESalazar
RWang-Gohrke
SCouch
FGoode
ELOlson
JEVachon
CFredericksen
ZSGiles
GGBaglietto
LSeveri
GHopper
JLEnglish
DRSouthey
MCHaiman
CAHenderson
BEKolonel
LNLe Marchand
LStram
DOHunter
DJHankinson
SECox
DGTamimi
RKraft
PSherman
MEChanock
SJLissowska
JBrinton
LAPeplonska
BKlijn
JGHooning
MJMeijers-Heijboer
HCollee
JMvan den Ouweland
AUitterlinden
AGLiu
JLin
LYYuqing
LHumphreys
KCzene
KCox
ABalasubramanian
SPCross
SSReed
MWBlows
FDriver
KDunning
ATyrer
JPonder
BASangrajrang
SBrennan
PMcKay
JOdefrey
FGabrieau
VSigurdson
ADoody
MStruewing
JPAlexander
BEaston
DFPharoah
PDAustralian Ovarian Cancer Management Group, Heterogeneity of breast cancer associations with five susceptibility loci by clinical and pathological characteristics.
PLoS Genet 2008;4
(4)
e1000054
PubMed10.1371/journal.pgen.1000054
Google Scholar