[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Effect sizes for psychological, oculomotor, and neuropsychological domain parameters. Negative values indicate that family members performed worse compared with the control participants. *Effects were statistically significant (P < .05).

Effect sizes for psychological, oculomotor, and neuropsychological domain parameters. Negative values indicate that family members performed worse compared with the control participants. *Effects were statistically significant (P < .05).

Figure 2. 
Accuracy of saccades in family members and control participants. Data are expressed as mean (SE) saccade error (degrees of visual angle from target) during a visually guided saccade task with unpredictable targets presented at 10°, 20°, and 30° from central fixation.

Accuracy of saccades in family members and control participants. Data are expressed as mean (SE) saccade error (degrees of visual angle from target) during a visually guided saccade task with unpredictable targets presented at 10°, 20°, and 30° from central fixation.

Figure 3. 
Variability of saccade accuracy in family members and control participants. Data are expressed as mean (SE) trial-wise variability in saccade error (standard deviation of saccade error) during a visually guided saccade task with unpredictable targets presented at 10°, 20°, and 30° from central fixation.

Variability of saccade accuracy in family members and control participants. Data are expressed as mean (SE) trial-wise variability in saccade error (standard deviation of saccade error) during a visually guided saccade task with unpredictable targets presented at 10°, 20°, and 30° from central fixation.

Figure 4. 
Mean (SE) pursuit gain across target speeds during the closed-loop stage of the foveofugal ramp task in family members and control participants. Three family members and 3 controls were not included in analyses because they either failed to complete the ramp task (n = 1) or had excessive blink artifacts (n = 5).

Mean (SE) pursuit gain across target speeds during the closed-loop stage of the foveofugal ramp task in family members and control participants. Three family members and 3 controls were not included in analyses because they either failed to complete the ramp task (n = 1) or had excessive blink artifacts (n = 5).

Figure 5. 
Mean (SE) pursuit gain across target speeds during the open-loop stage of the foveofugal step-ramp task in family members and control participants. Three family members and 3 controls were not included in analyses because they either failed to complete the step-ramp task (n = 1) or had excessive blink artifacts (n = 5).

Mean (SE) pursuit gain across target speeds during the open-loop stage of the foveofugal step-ramp task in family members and control participants. Three family members and 3 controls were not included in analyses because they either failed to complete the step-ramp task (n = 1) or had excessive blink artifacts (n = 5).

Figure 6. 
Average latency of onset for rightward and leftward saccades in family members and control participants during the predictive saccade task. Data points reflect the average values of consecutive pairs of same-direction trials (eg, trials 1 and 3 and trials 2 and 4).

Average latency of onset for rightward and leftward saccades in family members and control participants during the predictive saccade task. Data points reflect the average values of consecutive pairs of same-direction trials (eg, trials 1 and 3 and trials 2 and 4).

Figure 7. 
Percentage of prosaccade errors during the gap and overlap conditions of an antisaccade task. Data are expressed as mean (SE) rate of prosaccade errors across various target-step amplitudes from center fixation in degrees of visual angle during the gap (A) and overlap (B) conditions.

Percentage of prosaccade errors during the gap and overlap conditions of an antisaccade task. Data are expressed as mean (SE) rate of prosaccade errors across various target-step amplitudes from center fixation in degrees of visual angle during the gap (A) and overlap (B) conditions.

Table 1. 
Demographic Characteristics of Family Members of Individuals With Autism and of Healthy Control Subjectsa
Demographic Characteristics of Family Members of Individuals With Autism and of Healthy Control Subjectsa
Table 2. 
Performance on Neuropsychological Domains and Individual Neuropsychological Tests for Family Members of Individuals With Autism and for Healthy Control Subjectsa
Performance on Neuropsychological Domains and Individual Neuropsychological Tests for Family Members of Individuals With Autism and for Healthy Control Subjectsa
1.
Pickles  ABolton  PMacdonald  HBailey  ALe Couteur  ASim  CHRutter  M Latent-class analysis of recurrence risks for complex phenotypes with selection and measurement error: a twin and family history study of autism.  Am J Hum Genet 1995;57 (3) 717- 726PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Szatmari  P Heterogeneity and the genetics of autism.  J Psychiatry Neurosci 1999;24 (2) 159- 165PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Bolton  PMacdonald  HPickles  ARios  PGoode  SCrowson  MBailey  ARutter  M A case-control family history study of autism.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994;35 (5) 877- 900PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Landa  RPiven  JWzorek  MMGayle  JOChase  GAFolstein  SE Social language use in parents of autistic individuals.  Psychol Med 1992;22 (1) 245- 254PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Losh  MPiven  J Social-cognition and the broad autism phenotype: identifying genetically meaningful phenotypes.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007;48 (1) 105- 112PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Hughes  CLeboyer  MBouvard  M Executive function in parents of children with autism.  Psychol Med 1997;27 (1) 209- 220PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Constantino  JNLajonchere  CLutz  MGray  TAbbacchi  A McKenna  KSingh  DTodd  RD Autistic social impairment in the siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders.  Am J Psychiatry 2006;163 (2) 294- 296PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Delorme  RGousse  VRoy  ITrandafir  AMathieu  FMouren-Siméoni  MCBetancur  CLeboyer  M Shared executive dysfunctions in unaffected relatives of patients with autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Eur Psychiatry 2007;22 (1) 32- 38PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Pickles  AStarr  EKazak  SBolton  PPapanikolaou  KBailey  AGoodman  RRutter  M Variable expression of the autism broader phenotype: findings from extended pedigrees.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41 (4) 491- 502PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Losh  MAdolphs  RPoe  MDCouture  SPenn  DBaranek  GTPiven  J Neuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotype.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66 (5) 518- 526PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Fidler  DJBailey  JNSmalley  SL Macrocephaly in autism and other pervasive developmental disorders.  Dev Med Child Neurol 2000;42 (11) 737- 740PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Lainhart  JEBigler  EDBocian  MCoon  HDinh  EDawson  GDeutsch  CKDunn  MEstes  ATager-Flusberg  HFolstein  SHepburn  SHyman  S McMahon  WMinshew  NMunson  JOsann  KOzonoff  SRodier  PRogers  SSigman  MSpence  MAStodgell  CJVolkmar  F Head circumference and height in autism: a study by the Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism.  Am J Med Genet A 2006;140 (21) 2257- 2274PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Goldberg  JAnderson  GMZwaigenbaum  LHall  GBNahmias  CThompson  ASzatmari  P Cortical serotonin type-2 receptor density in parents of children with autism spectrum disorders.  J Autism Dev Disord 2009;39 (1) 97- 104PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Piven  JTsai  GCNehme  ECoyle  JTChase  GAFolstein  SE Platelet serotonin: a possible marker for familial autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 1991;21 (1) 51- 59PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Dalton  KMNacewicz  BMAlexander  ALDavidson  RJ Gaze-fixation, brain activation, and amygdala volume in unaffected siblings of individuals with autism.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;61 (4) 512- 520PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Dawson  GWebb  SJWijsman  ESchellenberg  GEstes  AMunson  JFaja  S Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism: implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism.  Dev Psychopathol 2005;17 (3) 679- 697PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Adolphs  RSpezio  MLParlier  MPiven  J Distinct face-processing strategies in parents of autistic children.  Curr Biol 2008;18 (14) 1090- 1093PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Manning  JTBaron-Cohen  SWheelwright  SSanders  G The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism.  Dev Med Child Neurol 2001;43 (3) 160- 164PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Bell  BBAbel  LALi  WChristian  JCYee  RD Concordance of smooth pursuit and saccadic measures in normal monozygotic twin pairs.  Biol Psychiatry 1994;36 (8) 522- 526PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Harris  MSReilly  JLKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Longitudinal studies of antisaccades in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia.  Psychol Med 2006;36 (4) 485- 494PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Lencer  RSprenger  AHarris  MSReilly  JLKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Effects of second-generation antipsychotic medication on smooth pursuit performance in antipsychotic-naive schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008;65 (10) 1146- 1154PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Reilly  JLHarris  MSKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Abnormalities in visually guided saccades suggest corticofugal dysregulation in never-treated schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2005;57 (2) 145- 154PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Yee  CMNuechterlein  KHDawson  ME A longitudinal analysis of eye tracking dysfunction and attention in recent-onset schizophrenia.  Psychophysiology 1998;35 (4) 443- 451PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Luna  BDoll  SKHegedus  SJMinshew  NJSweeney  JA Maturation of executive function in autism.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;61 (4) 474- 481PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Rosenhall  UJohansson  EGillberg  C Oculomotor findings in autistic children.  J Laryngol Otol 1988;102 (5) 435- 439PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Takarae  YMinshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Oculomotor abnormalities parallel cerebellar histopathology in autism.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004;75 (9) 1359- 1361PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Minshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism.  Neurology 1999;52 (5) 917- 922PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Takarae  YMinshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Atypical involvement of frontostriatal systems during sensorimotor control in autism.  Psychiatry Res 2007;156 (2) 117- 127PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Bailey  ALuthert  PDean  AHarding  BJanota  IMontgomery  MRutter  MLantos  P A clinicopathological study of autism.  Brain 1998;121 (pt 5) 889- 905PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Bauman  MLKemper  TL Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism: a review and future directions.  Int J Dev Neurosci 2005;23 (2-3) 183- 187PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Fatemi  SHHalt  ARRealmuto  GEarle  JKist  DAThuras  PMerz  A Purkinje cell size is reduced in cerebellum of patients with autism.  Cell Mol Neurobiol 2002;22 (2) 171- 175PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Ritvo  ERFreeman  BJScheibel  ABDuong  TRobinson  HGuthrie  DRitvo  A Lower Purkinje cell counts in the cerebella of four autistic subjects: initial findings of the UCLA-NSAC Autopsy Research Report.  Am J Psychiatry 1986;143 (7) 862- 866PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Whitney  ERKemper  TLBauman  MLRosene  DLBlatt  GJ Cerebellar Purkinje cells are reduced in a subpopulation of autistic brains: a stereological experiment using calbindin-D28k.  Cerebellum 2008;7 (3) 406- 416PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Fatemi  SHStary  JMHalt  ARRealmuto  GR Dysregulation of reelin and Bcl-2 proteins in autistic cerebellum.  J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31 (6) 529- 535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Benayed  RGharani  NRossman  IMancuso  VLazar  GKamdar  SBruse  SE Tischfield  SSmith  BJZimmerman  RADicicco-Bloom  EBrzustowicz  LMMillonig  JH Support for the homeobox transcription factor gene ENGRAILED 2 as an autism spectrum disorder susceptibility locus.  Am J Hum Genet 2005;77 (5) 851- 868PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Gharani  NBenayed  RMancuso  VBrzustowicz  LMMillonig  JH Association of the homeobox transcription factor, ENGRAILED 2, 3, with autism spectrum disorder.  Mol Psychiatry 2004;9 (5) 474- 484PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Takarae  YMinshew  NJLuna  BKrisky  CMSweeney  JA Pursuit eye movement deficits in autism.  Brain 2004;127 (pt 12) 2584- 2594PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Takarae  YLuna  BMinshew  NJSweeney  JA Patterns of visual sensory and sensorimotor abnormalities in autism vary in relation to history of early language delay.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14 (6) 980- 989PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Meck  WH Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception.  Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1996;3 (3-4) 227- 242PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Simó  LSKrisky  CMSweeney  JA Functional neuroanatomy of anticipatory behavior: dissociation between sensory-driven and memory-driven systems.  Cereb Cortex 2005;15 (12) 1982- 1991PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Goldberg  MCLasker  AGZee  DSGarth  ETien  ALanda  RJ Deficits in the initiation of eye movements in the absence of a visual target in adolescents with high functioning autism.  Neuropsychologia 2002;40 (12) 2039- 2049PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
D’Cruz  AMMosconi  MWSteele  SRubin  LHLuna  BMinshew  NSweeney  JA Lateralized response timing deficits in autism.  Biol Psychiatry 2009;66 (4) 393- 397PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Mosconi  MWKay  MD’Cruz  AMSeidenfeld  AGuter  SStanford  LDSweeney  JA Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders.  Psychol Med 2009;39 (9) 1559- 1566PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Sweeney  JAMintun  MAKwee  SWiseman  MBBrown  DLRosenberg  DRCarl  JR Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory.  J Neurophysiol 1996;75 (1) 454- 468PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Koczat  DLRogers  SJPennington  BFRoss  RG Eye movement abnormality suggestive of a spatial working memory deficit is present in parents of autistic probands.  J Autism Dev Disord 2002;32 (6) 513- 518PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Lord  CRutter  MLe Couteur  A Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders.  J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24 (5) 659- 685PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Lord  CRisi  SLambrecht  LCook  EH  JrLeventhal  BLDiLavore  PCPickles  ARutter  M The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2000;30 (3) 205- 223PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Rutter  MBailey  ALord  C The Social Communication Questionnaire Manual.  Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2003
49.
Bolton  PMacdonald  HPickles  ARios  PGoode  SCrowson  MBailey  ARutter  M A case-control family history study of autism.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994;35 (5) 877- 900PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Luna  BGarver  KEUrban  TALazar  NASweeney  JA Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood.  Child Dev 2004;75 (5) 1357- 1372PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Saslow  MG Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement.  J Opt Soc Am 1967;57 (8) 1024- 1029PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Lisberger  SGWestbrook  LE Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys.  J Neurosci 1985;5 (6) 1662- 1673PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Harris  MSWiseman  CLReilly  JLKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Effects of risperidone on procedural learning in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia.  Neuropsychopharmacology 2009;34 (2) 468- 476PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Goodman  WKPrice  LHRasmussen  SAMazure  CFleischmann  RLHill  CLHeninger  GRCharney  DS The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, I: development, use, and reliability.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46 (11) 1006- 1011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Scahill  LRiddle  MA McSwiggin-Hardin  MOrt  SIKing  RAGoodman  WKCicchetti  DLeckman  JF Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: reliability and validity.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36 (6) 844- 852PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Baron-Cohen  SWheelwright  SSkinner  RMartin  JClubley  E The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians.  J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31 (1) 5- 17PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Erwin  RJGur  RCGur  RESkolnick  BMawhinney-Hee  MSmailis  J Facial emotion discrimination, I: task construction and behavioral findings in normal subjects.  Psychiatry Res 1992;42 (3) 231- 240PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Roche  AFMukherjee  DGuo  SMMoore  WM Head circumference reference data: birth to 18 years.  Pediatrics 1987;79 (5) 706- 712PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Keller  ELSlakey  DPCrandall  WF Microstimulation of the primate cerebellar vermis during saccadic eye movements.  Brain Res 1983;288 (1-2) 131- 143PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Iwamoto  YYoshida  K Saccadic dysmetria following inactivation of the primate fastigial oculomotor region.  Neurosci Lett 2002;325 (3) 211- 215PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Noda  HMurakami  SWarabi  T Effects of fastigial stimulation upon visually-directed saccades in macaque monkeys.  Neurosci Res 1991;10 (3) 188- 199PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Golla  HTziridis  KHaarmeier  TCatz  NBarash  SThier  P Reduced saccadic resilience and impaired saccadic adaptation due to cerebellar disease.  Eur J Neurosci 2008;27 (1) 132- 144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Catani  MJones  DKDaly  EEmbiricos  NDeeley  QPugliese  LCurran  SRobertson  DMurphy  DG Altered cerebellar feedback projections in Asperger syndrome.  Neuroimage 2008;41 (4) 1184- 1191PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Courchesne  EYeung-Courchesne  RPress  GAHesselink  JRJernigan  TL Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism.  N Engl J Med 1988;318 (21) 1349- 1354PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Webb  SJSparks  BFFriedman  SDShaw  DWGiedd  JDawson  GDager  SR Cerebellar vermal volumes and behavioral correlates in children with autism spectrum disorder.  Psychiatry Res 2009;172 (1) 61- 67PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Cleavinger  HBBigler  EDJohnson  JLLu  J McMahon  WLainhart  JE Quantitative magnetic resonance image analysis of the cerebellum in macrocephalic and normocephalic children and adults with autism.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14 (3) 401- 413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Zeegers  MPol  HHDurston  SNederveen  HSchnack  Hvan Daalen  EDietz  Cvan Engeland  HBuitelaar  J No differences in MR-based volumetry between 2- and 7-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder and developmental delay.  Brain Dev 2009;31 (10) 725- 730PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Sharpe  JA Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit: lesion studies.  Brain Cogn 2008;68 (3) 241- 254PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Clementz  BAReid  SA McDowell  JECadenhead  KS Abnormality of smooth pursuit eye movement initiation: specificity to the schizophrenia spectrum?  Psychophysiology 1995;32 (2) 130- 134PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Levy  DLHolzman  PSMatthysse  SMendell  NR Eye tracking and schizophrenia: a selective review.  Schizophr Bull 1994;20 (1) 47- 62PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Hong  LEMitchell  BDAvila  MTAdami  H McMahon  RPThaker  GK Familial aggregation of eye-tracking endophenotypes in families of schizophrenic patients.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (3) 259- 264PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Holzman  PSKringlen  ELevy  DLProctor  LRHaberman  SJYasillo  NJ Abnormal-pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia: evidence for a genetic indicator.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1977;34 (7) 802- 805PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Lencer  RReilly  JLHarris  MSSprenger  AKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Sensorimotor transformation deficits for smooth pursuit in first-episode affective psychoses and schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2010;67 (3) 217- 223PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Sweeney  JALuna  BHaas  GLKeshavan  MSMann  JJThase  ME Pursuit tracking impairments in schizophrenia and mood disorders: step-ramp studies with unmedicated patients.  Biol Psychiatry 1999;46 (5) 671- 680PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
75.
Dieterich  MMuller-Schunk  SStephan  TBense  SSeelos  KYousry  TA Functional magnetic resonance imaging activations of cortical eye fields during saccades, smooth pursuit, and optokinetic nystagmus.  Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1164282- 292PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Mustari  MJOno  SDas  VE Signal processing and distribution in cortical-brainstem pathways for smooth pursuit eye movements.  Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1164147- 154PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
77.
Meck  WHPenney  TBPouthas  V Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans.  Curr Opin Neurobiol 2008;18 (2) 145- 152PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
78.
Herbert  MRHarris  GJAdrien  KTZiegler  DAMakris  NKennedy  DNLange  NTChabris  CFBakardjiev  AHodgson  JTakeoka  MTager-Flusberg  HCaviness  VS  Jr Abnormal asymmetry in language association cortex in autism.  Ann Neurol 2002;52 (5) 588- 596PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
79.
Müller  RABehen  MERothermel  RDChugani  DCMuzik  OMangner  TJChugani  HT Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: a PET study.  J Autism Dev Disord 1999;29 (1) 19- 31PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Cornish  KM McManus  IC Hand preference and hand skill in children with autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 1996;26 (6) 597- 609PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Escalante-Mead  PRMinshew  NJSweeney  JA Abnormal brain lateralization in high-functioning autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2003;33 (5) 539- 543PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
82.
Soper  HVSatz  POrsini  DLHenry  RRZvi  JCSchulman  M Handedness patterns in autism suggest subtypes.  J Autism Dev Disord 1986;16 (2) 155- 167PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
McAlonan  GMDaly  EKumari  VCritchley  HDvan Amelsvoort  TSuckling  JSimmons  ASigmundsson  TGreenwood  KRussell  ASchmitz  NHappe  FHowlin  PMurphy  DG Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger's syndrome.  Brain 2002;125 (pt 7) 1594- 1606PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
84.
McAlonan  GMCheung  CCheung  VWong  NSuckling  JChua  SE Differential effects on white-matter systems in high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome.  Psychol Med 2009;39 (11) 1885- 1893PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
85.
Waiter  GDWilliams  JHMurray  ADGilchrist  APerrett  DIWhiten  A Structural white matter deficits in high-functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorder: a voxel-based investigation.  Neuroimage 2005;24 (2) 455- 461PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
86.
Ghaziuddin  MMountain-Kimchi  K Defining the intellectual profile of Asperger syndrome: comparison with high-functioning autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2004;34 (3) 279- 284PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Koyama  TTachimori  HOsada  HTakeda  TKurita  H Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007;61 (1) 99- 104PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
Klin  AVolkmar  FRSparrow  SSCicchetti  DVRourke  BP Validity and neuropsychological characterization of Asperger syndrome: convergence with nonverbal learning disabilities syndrome.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1995;36 (7) 1127- 1140PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
89.
Hughes  CPlumet  MHLeboyer  M Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40 (5) 705- 718PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
90.
Thakkar  KNPolli  FEJoseph  RMTuch  DSHadjikhani  NBarton  JJManoach  DS Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD).  Brain 2008;131 (pt 9) 2464- 2478PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
91.
Schmitz  NRubia  KDaly  ESmith  AWilliams  SMurphy  DG Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (1) 7- 16PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
Kana  RKKeller  TAMinshew  NJJust  MA Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;62 (3) 198- 206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Goldberg  MCMostofsky  SHCutting  LEMahone  EMAstor  BCDenckla  MBLanda  RJ Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD.  J Autism Dev Disord 2005;35 (3) 279- 293PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
94.
Steele  SDMinshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Spatial working memory deficits in autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2007;37 (4) 605- 612PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Williams  DLGoldstein  GCarpenter  PAMinshew  NJ Verbal and spatial working memory in autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2005;35 (6) 747- 756PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
96.
Luna  BMinshew  NJGarver  KELazar  NAThulborn  KREddy  WFSweeney  JA Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: an fMRI study of spatial working memory.  Neurology 2002;59 (6) 834- 840PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
97.
Le Couteur  ABailey  AGoode  SPickles  ARobertson  SGottesman  IRutter  M A broader phenotype of autism: the clinical spectrum in twins.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37 (7) 785- 801PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
98.
Losh  MChildress  DLam  KPiven  J Defining key features of the broad autism phenotype: a comparison across parents of multiple- and single-incidence autism families.  Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008;147B (4) 424- 433PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
August 2010

Neurobehavioral Abnormalities in First-Degree Relatives of Individuals With Autism

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Center for Cognitive Medicine, University of Illinois at Chicago.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(8):830-840. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.87
Abstract

Context  Studying sensorimotor and neurocognitive impairments in unaffected family members of individuals with autism may help identify familial pathophysiological mechanisms associated with the disorder.

Objective  To determine whether atypical sensorimotor or neurocognitive characteristics associated with autism are present in first-degree relatives of individuals with autism.

Design  Case-control comparison of neurobehavioral functions.

Setting  University medical center.

Participants  Fifty-seven first-degree relatives of individuals with autism and 40 age-, sex-, and IQ-matched healthy control participants (aged 8-54 years).

Main Outcome Measures  Oculomotor tests of sensorimotor responses (saccades and smooth pursuit); procedural learning and response inhibition; neuropsychological tests of motor, memory, and executive functions; and psychological measures of social behavior, communication skills, and obsessive-compulsive behaviors.

Results  On eye movement testing, family members demonstrated saccadic hypometria, reduced steady-state pursuit gain, and a higher rate of voluntary response inhibition errors relative to controls. They also showed lateralized deficits in procedural learning and open-loop pursuit gain (initial 100 milliseconds of pursuit) and increased variability in the accuracy of large-amplitude saccades that were confined to rightward movements. In neuropsychological studies, only executive functions were impaired relative to those of controls. Family members reported more communication abnormalities and obsessive-compulsive behaviors than controls. Deficits across oculomotor, neuropsychological, and psychological domains were relatively independent from one another.

Conclusions  Family members of individuals with autism demonstrate oculomotor abnormalities implicating pontocerebellar and frontostriatal circuits and left-lateralized alterations of frontotemporal circuitry and striatum. The left-lateralized alterations have not been identified in other neuropsychiatric disorders and are of interest given atypical brain lateralization and language development associated with the disorder. Similar oculomotor deficits have been reported in individuals with autism, suggesting that they may be familial and useful for studies of neurophysiological and genetic mechanisms in autism.

Autism is a highly heritable neurodevelopmental disorder with considerable genetic and phenotypic heterogeneity.1,2 Its core behavioral features include social and communication impairments, behavioral inflexibility, and executive dysfunction. Disturbances in these domains also have been identified in unaffected family members,3-10 suggesting that they are familial traits. Neurophysiological and cognitive phenotypes are difficult to study in individuals with severe cognitive impairment, but family studies can include individuals without regard to proband disability and therefore provide potentially more generalizable findings about autism and familial mechanisms. Studying variation in neurobehavioral phenotypes may help resolve pathophysiological mechanisms in autism and provide quantitative traits for family genetic research.

Studies of unaffected family members of individuals with autism have reported multiple informative findings, including increased rates of macrocephaly,11,12 decreased cortical serotonin receptor density,13 elevated whole-blood serotonin levels,14 neurophysiological15,16 and neuroanatomic abnormalities15 associated with face processing deficits,17 and minor physical anomalies.18 Sensorimotor functions, which have been shown to be abnormal in individuals with autism, have not been extensively examined in familial studies.

Saccadic and smooth-pursuit eye movement impairments in autism represent potential intermediate phenotypes owing to their well-defined neurophysiological features, quantitative nature, heritability,19 and stability over time.20-23 Saccades are rapid eye movements that shift gaze between objects in the visual field. Saccade dysmetria (reduced saccade accuracy) and increased variability of saccade accuracy implicating variability-reducing functions of the cerebellum have been reported in some24-26 but not all studies of autism,27,28 suggesting pontocerebellar deficits. Saccade abnormalities are of interest in the context of abnormal Purkinje cell size and number,29-33 reductions in levels of the signaling molecule reelin in the cerebellum,34 and association or altered expression of genes related to cerebellar development in autism.35,36

Smooth-pursuit eye movements stabilize gaze on slowly moving objects. Deficits in sustained and open-loop (approximately the first 100 milliseconds) pursuit have been documented in autism.37,38 Sustained pursuit impairments have been associated with dysfunction within the cerebellar and frontoparietal sensorimotor systems.28,38 Although sustained pursuit deficits in autism are not lateralized, individuals with autism show slower rightward open-loop pursuit than healthy control subjects.37 Because there is no lateralized alteration in motion perception during psychophysical testing38 or in V5 activation during sustained pursuit tracking,28 the absence of a rightward open-loop advantage in individuals with autism suggests an alteration in the transfer of motion information from the left extrastriate cortex to sensorimotor systems.

Procedural learning of motor sequences with interresponse intervals on the order of 1 second depends on striatal learning and response timing systems.39,40 Reduced temporal precision of responses during an oculomotor serial reaction time task has been described in autism.41 This abnormality was greater for rightward predictive saccades,42 which, like the lateralized open-loop pursuit deficits, indicates a left hemisphere dysfunction. Deficits in the executive control of saccades assessed with antisaccade and memory-guided saccade tasks have been reported in autism,24,27,41,43 implicating bilateral alterations in prefrontal systems.44 Koczat et al45 reported reduced accuracy of memory-guided saccades in 11 unaffected parents of patients with autism.

Oculomotor studies in individuals with autism thus document distinct deficits that implicate pontocerebellar circuitry, left frontotemporal circuitry, left striatum, and prefrontal systems. In the present study, we sought to determine whether oculomotor deficits evident in individuals with autism are present in unaffected family members and whether they are related to other putative familial traits, including neuropsychological, psychological, and head circumference measures.

Methods
Sample

Study participants consisted of 42 parents and 15 siblings of individuals diagnosed as having autistic disorder and 40 healthy matched controls (Table 1). To recruit a relatively representative sample of family members, first-degree relatives of consecutive individuals with autistic disorder seen in our clinics were identified regardless of the age or illness severity of the affected proband. Thirty probands with autistic disorder were recruited through clinics at the University of Illinois at Chicago. Proband diagnoses were confirmed by the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised46 and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic47 using DSM-IV criteria. Family members of probands who met criteria for Asperger disorder or pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified were not included. Individuals with a known genetic or metabolic disorder associated with autism were excluded. All but 9 probands were too young (<8 years of age) or too cognitively/behaviorally impaired to complete eye movement and neuropsychological testing; thus, proband testing data are not reported. Proband clinical characteristics are included in the supplementary materials available on the authors' Web site (http://ccm.psych.uic.edu/PublishedSupplementaryData/Mosconi_et_al._Autism_family_study_Supplementary_tables.html).

Only family members who did not meet cutoffs on the Social Communication Questionnaire48 for autism were included in the analyses. History of psychiatric disorders was assessed using the Autism Family History Interview Revised–Modified.49 Seven family members had psychiatric diagnoses, including attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, depression, and anxiety; none met current symptom criteria. Four of these individuals reported a history of taking antidepressants, psychostimulants, or benzodiazepines. At the time of testing, one was taking clonazepam (Klonopin) (16 hours before testing) and another had received sertraline hydrochloride (Zoloft) for more than 1 month.

Controls were recruited through local newsletters and had no history of mood or psychotic disorder, neurological disorder, family history of autism in first- or second-degree relatives, or first-degree relatives with a major mental illness or developmental disorder. Family members and controls had no history of head injury, birth injury, drug dependence or recent abuse, or seizure disorder and had a full-scale IQ of more than 80. Groups were matched by IQ, age, and sex. Family member and control groups were matched in the proportion of individuals (49 of 57 [86%] for the family member group and 35 of 40 [88%] for the control group) who had reached the age (14 years) at which performance on cognitive tasks of interest approaches adult levels.50 Far visual acuity was normal or corrected to at least 20/40. Informed consent was obtained from all adult participants; minors provided assent, and their parents provided consent. Study procedures were approved by the institutional review board of the University of Illinois at Chicago.

Eye movement studies

Participants were tested in a darkened black room, positioned in a chin rest 140 cm from a display screen. Visual stimuli subtending 0.5° to 1° of visual angle were presented in the horizontal plane at eye level on a 183 × 244-cm seamless rear-projection screen (TechPlex 150; Stewart Filmscreen Corporation, Torrance, California) with the use of a high-resolution projector (Marquee 8500 Ultra projector; Christie Digital Systems, Cypress, California) with 2500 × 2000 resolution and a 120-Hz refresh rate. Direct current electro-oculography was used to record eye movements during saccade tasks to assess large-amplitude saccades (Grass Neurodata 12 Acquisition System; Astro-Med, Inc, West Warwick, Rhode Island). Smooth pursuit was monitored using infrared sensors (Model 310; Applied Science Laboratories, Inc, Bedford, Massachusetts) that detected catch-up saccades with amplitudes on the order of 0.20° to 0.25°. Fixation of static targets across the horizontal plane was used to calibrate eye movement recordings. Blinks were monitored using electrodes placed above and below the left eye linked to an AC-coupled bioamplifier. Eye movement data were digitized at 500 Hz with a 12-bit A/D converter (DI-720; DATAQ Instruments, Akron, Ohio). Digital finite impulse-response filters with nonlinear transition bands were applied. Data from each trial were visually inspected and scored without knowledge of subject characteristics. For pursuit tasks, saccades and blinks were excluded before calculating pursuit gain.

Visually guided saccade task

The visually guided saccade task was used to evaluate saccade latencies (time from the appearance of the peripheral target to the response initiation), saccade error (distance in degrees of the angle between eye location at the end of the saccade and the target location), and variability of saccade error (standard deviation of saccade error for all of the trials). Participants fixated a central target for 1500 to 2500 milliseconds before a peripheral target appeared unpredictably with equal probability at 10°, 20°, or 30° to the left or right of center. In the gap condition, the central fixation target was extinguished 200 milliseconds before the peripheral target appeared. In the overlap condition, the central fixation remained illuminated for 200 milliseconds after the appearance of the peripheral target. The overlap condition assesses the inhibitory modulation of saccade initiation by the visual fixation system, whereas the gap condition assesses response latencies without the delaying influence of ongoing central fixation.51 Thirty-six trials of each condition were interleaved and presented in a fixed pseudorandom order.

Foveofugal ramp task

To assess pursuit latency and pursuit gain (ratio of average pursuit speed to target speed), targets were presented at center fixation for 2 to 4 seconds and then swept 15° to the left or right at a constant speed of 4°/s, 8°/s, 16°/s, 24°/s, or 32°/s. The onset, speed, and direction of target movement were pseudorandomly assigned for 40 trials (4 trials × 5 velocities × 2 directions). The latency to initiate pursuit was computed only when pursuit initiation preceded the first catch-up saccade.

Foveofugal step-ramp task

To assess open- and closed-loop gains separately, a step-ramp task was used in which targets stepped 3° to the left or right and immediately continued moving in the same direction at a constant speed of 4°/s, 8°/s, 16°/s, or 24°/s until reaching 15° of visual angle. In this paradigm, participants typically make a catch-up saccade approximately 200 milliseconds after the onset of target motion followed immediately by smooth pursuit. The first 100 milliseconds of pursuit is referred to as open-loop pursuit because it is driven by sensory motion information, and it occurs too early for visual feedback to affect performance.52 The subsequent sustained pursuit is referred to as closed-loop pursuit because performance feedback can be used to reduce tracking error. The latency and gain of initial catch-up saccades and pursuit gain for open- and closed-loop phases were examined. Thirty-two trials were presented (4 trials × 4 velocities × 2 directions).

Predictive saccade task

This serial reaction time task assesses procedural learning of a motor response. Individuals shifted gaze between targets alternating at 6° to the left and right of center every 750 milliseconds (1.33 Hz) 40 times. Participants quickly learn to anticipate the timing and location of target appearance, leading to a marked reduction in saccade latencies because saccades are based on internally generated predictions rather than on a response to target appearance. The latency and gain (response amplitude over the target distance) of primary saccades were measured, as was the proportion of trials with anticipatory saccades (latency <90 milliseconds).40,42,53

Antisaccade task

This task assesses the ability to inhibit saccades toward peripheral targets and instead to look immediately to the target's mirror location in the opposite hemifield. Stimulus parameters were identical to those used in the visually guided saccade task. Participants were reminded of task instructions if consecutive errors were made. Performance on 10 practice trials verified task comprehension. The percentage of trials in which participants looked toward rather than away from targets and the latency of those responses were measured.

Neuropsychological and psychological evaluation

Tests of motor, memory, and executive functions were selected on the basis of findings from previous studies of individuals with autism and their family members and an interest in covering a broad range of cognitive abilities to characterize areas of relative strength and deficit. The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale54 and the Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale55 were administered to individuals 15 years or older and to those younger than 15 years, respectively. The Autism-Spectrum Quotient was administered to individuals 16 years or older to assess communication, socialization, attention switching, attention to detail, and imagination.56 The Penn Emotion Discrimination Task57 was administered to examine participants' ability to recognize facial affect (Table 2).

Head circumference measurement

Head circumference was measured by placing a tape around the head covering the glabella and opisthocranion. Two measurements were recorded and averaged.

Data analysis

Repeated-measures analyses of variance were used to compare groups on visually guided saccade, antisaccade, and pursuit tasks. Some subjects' data were not included for some tasks owing to excessive blink artifacts or failures to complete tasks. For predictive saccades, latency and gain data were each modeled over the logarithm of trial number (to linearize exponential learning curves). Then, mixed-effects regression was used to model differential learning effects. No significant interactions were observed for diagnostic group with sex or age for any task. No obvious bimodality was present for any measure.

Neuropsychological test scores were converted to z scores using age-normed standard scores and then combined on an a priori basis into motor, memory, and executive function domains. Psychological symptom ratings were converted to z scores relative to the healthy control group and averaged to assess social-emotional, communication, and obsessive-compulsive domains. Multivariate analyses of variance were performed to assess group differences on neuropsychological and psychological symptom domains. For these analyses, age and sex were entered as random factors, and subject group was entered as the fixed factor. No interactions of age or sex with group effects were observed. Tables with means and standard deviations of all parameters for each eye movement task are available in the supplementary material.

Results

Performance on eye movement tasks generally did not vary as a function of age (P > .30). The exception was antisaccade performance, in which younger subjects made more inhibitory errors (r97 = −0.39; P < .001); however, this effect did not differ across groups (F2,94 = 0.79; P = .66). Thus, all relatives were combined without regard to age except for exploratory correlational analyses in which age effects were statistically controlled.

Visually guided saccades

Saccade error was greater for family members than controls (F1,95 = 7.06; P = .009) (Figure 1 and Figure 2), especially for targets further from center (F2,94 = 7.13; P = .001). For variability in saccade error, the group × hemifield (F1,95 = 4.14; P = .04) and group × hemifield × target-step amplitude interactions (F2,94 = 5.86; P = .004) were significant (Figure 3). Variability in saccade accuracy was greater for family members relative to controls for rightward saccades, primarily for larger saccades. For saccade latency and velocity, the main effect of group and the interactions with group were not significant (P > .10).

Foveofugal ramp task

Family members had lower pursuit gain than controls (F1,89 = 10.50; P = .002), especially when tracking faster target speeds (F4,86 = 4.90; P = .001) (Figure 4). Group × direction effects were not significant, and no group differences were observed for pursuit or saccade latency (P > .10). However, the proportion of trials in which pursuit initiation preceded the first catch-up saccade was higher in family members (53%) than controls (43%) for leftward but not rightward (42% vs 49%) trials (F1,88 = 12.82; P = .001).

Foveofugal step-ramp task

For open-loop pursuit gain, the main effect of group and the group × target speed interaction was not significant (P > .10), but the group × direction interaction was significant (F1,87 = 5.72; P = .02) (Figure 5). Mean (SD) rightward and leftward open-loop gains of 0.71 (0.04) and 0.76 (0.04), respectively, seen in family members, did not show the rightward advantage (t53 = 1.61; P = .11) that was seen in healthy controls (t36 = 2.05; P = .05), who had mean (SD) rightward and leftward gains of 0.86 (0.05) and 0.75 (0.06), respectively. For closed-loop pursuit gain, group differences were significant (F1,87 = 4.17; P = .04). The group × target speed interaction was significant (F3,85 = 3.96; P = .001), with family members showing lower closed-loop gain than controls for faster target speeds. No group differences in the accuracy or latency of initial catch-up saccades were observed (P > .10).

Predictive saccade task

The group × trial interaction was significant, indicating that reductions in saccade latencies across trials were less robust in family members than controls (β = −45.56; SE = 16.24; P = .005). Also, a group × direction effect was observed, with family members showing similar response latencies for leftward and rightward responses, not the increasingly faster rightward relative to leftward responses across trials seen in controls (β = 19.88; SE = 8.28; P = .02) (Figure 6). Consistent with these observations, family members showed a lower percentage of anticipatory saccades than controls for rightward (55% vs 63%) but not leftward responses (57% vs 54%; group × direction, F1,88 = 4.72; P = .03). Saccade gain did not differ between groups for rightward or leftward responses (P > .10).

Antisaccade task

Family members failed to voluntarily suppress saccades to peripheral targets more often than controls (F1,95 = 4.26; P = .04) (Figure 7). This effect was greater for gap than overlap trials (F1,95 = 7.25; P = .008) and when targets were presented closer to center fixation (F2,94 = 6.10; P = .02). There were no group differences in relation to target direction or in response latencies for correctly performed trials (P > .10).

Neuropsychological and psychological measures

The multivariate test for overall group differences across neuropsychological domains was significant (F3,94 = 3.62; P = .02). Univariate comparisons indicated that family members showed poorer executive function performance but no impairments in motor or memory abilities (Figure 1). The greatest group differences in executive functions were in visual-spatial working memory. Groups also differed on psychological symptom ratings (F3,74 = 4.49; P = .006). Family members reported deficits in pragmatic and nonverbal communication but not reduced vocabulary knowledge. Obsessive-compulsive behaviors were more common in family members (Table 2).

Head circumference

Normative data considering differences related to age and sex58 were used to calculate z scores for each subject's head circumference.12 Head circumference did not differ between groups, nor did proportions of macrocephalic (≥97th percentile; 5 of 57 [9%] for family members; 3 of 40 [8%] for controls) or microcephalic (≤3rd percentile; 1 of 57 [2%] for family members; 1 of 40 [2%] for controls) subjects (P > .20; Table 1).

Correlational analyses

In analyses controlling for effects of age, we examined relationships among performance parameters and compared the strength of these correlations between groups using the Fisher z conversion. In the eye movement data, sensorimotor disturbances were more strongly related to alterations in executive abilities in family members than controls. For family members, visually guided saccade error was associated with error rate on the antisaccade task (R254 = 0.08; P = .02). This relationship was not significant for controls (P = .18), and it was stronger for family members than controls (z = 2.51; P = .01). Decreased pursuit gain during the ramp task (R251 = 0.08; P = .02) and decreased open-loop pursuit gain for rightward targets during the step-ramp task (R251 = 0.10; P = .01) were associated with antisaccade error rate for family members but not controls (P = .29 and .21, respectively). These relationships were significantly stronger for family members than controls (pursuit gain and antisaccade error rate: z = 2.52, P = .006; rightward open-loop pursuit gain and antisaccade error rate: z = 2.61, P = .01). Manual motor performance was positively correlated with executive function in neuropsychological testing for family members (R254 = 0.12; P = .006) but not controls (P = .23); however, the strength of this relationship did not differ between groups.

Response latencies on different tasks were more highly intercorrelated in controls than family members. Visually guided saccade latencies were associated with correct antisaccade latencies in controls (R237 = 0.14; P = .03) but not family members (P = .50). This relationship was stronger in controls than in family members (z = 2.15; P = .03). Saccade accuracy (saccade error [R237 = 0.29; P < .001] and variability in saccade error [R237 = .34; P < .001]) was associated with visuospatial search (time to complete Trail Making Test A) for controls but not family members (P = .34 and .40, respectively). Both relationships were stronger for controls than family members (saccade error and visuospatial search: z = 2.99, P = .02; variability in saccade error and visuospatial search: z = 3.18, P = .01). No other associates of eye movement performance, neuropsychological performance, psychological measures, or head circumference were significant in family members or controls (P > .20).

Comment

Autism is a heritable disorder with behavioral, neurobiological, and genetic heterogeneity. Identifying functional alterations in specific neural pathways is a crucial step in understanding pathophysiological mechanisms in this disorder. The present findings document that first-degree relatives of individuals with autism demonstrate a unique pattern of oculomotor impairments similar to that previously reported in independent samples of individuals with autism, suggesting that these alterations within sensorimotor and cognitive brain circuitry may be familial traits. Family members also demonstrated executive dysfunction on neuropsychological tests, communication abnormalities, and increased rates of obsessive and compulsive behaviors, but these were independent from one another and from oculomotor impairments.

The different oculomotor abnormalities demonstrated by family members implicate distinct neural circuits. Saccadic dysmetria and variable saccade accuracy implicate pontocerebellar circuitry. Given available functional magnetic resonance imaging data in individuals with autism,28 decreased closed-loop pursuit gain may in part also result from alteration in this circuitry. Open-loop pursuit and predictive saccades of family members lacked the rightward advantage evident in controls, suggesting reduced left hemispheric specialization in frontotemporal and striatal systems, respectively. Increased rates of antisaccade errors suggest that prefrontal systems are affected bilaterally. These different alterations were moderately intercorrelated (R2 range, 0.08-0.12) in family members, suggesting a general profile of deficit but also significant variability in the extent to which the different alterations are expressed in different family members. These discrete laboratory measures thus may provide useful tools for studying specific neural circuit–level dysfunctions across the autism spectrum and for parsing more homogeneous phenotypes in family genetic research.

Oculomotor impairments in family members of individuals with autism

Saccadic dysmetria and increased variability of saccade accuracy observed in family members parallel findings we reported previously in some24-26 but not all27,28 independent samples of individuals with autism. Alterations in saccade accuracy indicate that the modulatory role of the cerebellum on pontine output is attenuated in some individuals with autism. Cells in the vermis and fastigial nuclei of the cerebellum encode motor error signals during saccades and use this information to alter saccade dynamics to reduce systematic inaccuracy and the variability in responses over time.59-62 Altered saccade metrics in family members is consistent with multiple reports of cerebellar histopathological features29-33 and functional and anatomic abnormalities in some63-65 but not all66,67 magnetic resonance imaging studies of autism. The functional integrity of the cerebellum has not been studied systematically in unaffected family members. Thus, our findings provide novel evidence that altered cerebellar function evident in individuals with autism also is evident in unaffected family members.

Decreased accuracy of sustained smooth pursuit in family members may also be a manifestation of cerebellar dysfunction. Projections from the cerebellum to frontal eye fields modulate the velocity and trajectory of sustained pursuit based on sensory feedback regarding position and velocity errors.68 Reduced activation in the cerebellum and frontal eye fields during pursuit in individuals with autism has been reported in functional magnetic resonance imaging studies.28 Preliminary evidence exists for the familiality of saccade and smooth-pursuit function,19 consistent with the possibility that these deficits may reflect familial neural system alterations in autism.28

On the foveofugal step-ramp task, family members exhibited lower open-loop pursuit gain toward rightward moving targets relative to controls. This observation parallels our previous findings in an independent sample of individuals with autism.37 Alterations of closed-loop pursuit have also been described in patients with schizophrenia21,69,70 and their unaffected family members,71,72 as well as in other disorders.73,74 Therefore, this abnormality lacks diagnostic specificity. In contrast, the atypical lateralized open-loop pursuit deficit in individuals with autism, which we now show in unaffected relatives, has not been reported in any neuropsychiatric illness. Initial cortical processing of visual motion signals is performed by contralateral extrastriate area V5, which sends motion-related signals to sensorimotor systems in ipsilateral frontal eye fields and cerebellum to regulate open-loop pursuit.75,76 Available psychophysical evidence suggests no lateralized deficit in motion processing in autism.38 Therefore, the lateralized deficit in pursuit is not likely to involve sensorimotor systems but rather circuitry supporting the feed-forward input of motion information to sensorimotor systems.

Atypical procedural learning for rightward saccades also was observed in family members. This alteration in procedural learning implicates left frontostriatal systems, especially the basal ganglia, where chronometric mechanisms regulate the precise timing of learned response intervals.77 Similar to the lateralized alteration in open-loop pursuit, lateralized procedural learning effects have not been identified in other neuropsychiatric disorders. In both cases, the lateralized effects appeared to reflect a lack of typical hemispheric advantage for rightward movements because controls showed a rightward advantage, whereas leftward and rightward responses were similar in autism. This might result from an alteration in the maturation of hemispheric specialization or from an abnormality that differentially affects the left hemisphere.

Alterations in the specialization of left hemisphere functions in autism suggested by the present findings align with evidence of atypical language development,78,79 reduced left hemisphere motor dominance,80-82 and left lateralized white matter abnormalities in some83,84 but not all85 previous studies. Our observations also are consistent with reports of greater impairments in verbal than in nonverbal cognitive abilities in many individuals with autism.86-88 Thus, the left lateralized neurophysiological alterations in our unaffected family members indicate that this atypical lateralization associated with autism may extend to unaffected family members in ways that can be detected with sensitive neurophysiological measurements.

Family members made more inhibitory errors on the antisaccade task and scored lower on neuropsychological measures of executive function than did controls. Deficits on the antisaccade task were more pronounced on gap trials, suggesting diminished inhibitory control of saccade generation by fixation neurons in rostral superior colliculus that receive significant modulatory input from the prefrontal cortex. These findings parallel previous neuropsychological studies of family members,6,8,10,89 demonstrations of antisaccade deficits in individuals with autism,24,27,41,43,90 and functional magnetic resonance imaging findings of prefrontal and anterior cingulate alterations during tasks requiring response monitoring and planning in individuals with autism.90-92

Family members' reduced executive functioning performance was most evident for spatial working memory. This effect is consistent with previous reports of spatial working memory impairments in individuals with autism93-95 and their unaffected family members45,89 and with prefrontal cortical abnormalities during an oculomotor working memory task.96 Thus, our findings indicate that alterations in prefrontal systems important for planning volitional behaviors and voluntarily suppressing context-inappropriate behavior occur in unaffected family members.

Psychological characteristics of family members of individuals with autism

Consistent with previous work, we found evidence of atypical social communication and increased rates of obsessive-compulsive behaviors in family members, but these were not related to deficits on oculomotor tasks.4,5,7-10,97,98 Family members did not show impairments on a standardized test of receptive vocabulary, suggesting deficits in pragmatic aspects of communication rather than in vocabulary knowledge per se. Although eye movement abnormalities were observed on several paradigms, family members did not show manual motor impairments on neuropsychological testing. This apparent disparity between oculomotor and manual motor measures suggests that standard neuropsychological tests of manual skill do not provide equivalent sensitivity to deficits in manual motor control or that sensorimotor impairment within family members is relatively confined to oculomotor systems. Further studies are needed to resolve this inconsistency.

Clues to the pathophysiological mechanisms of autism

The findings of the present study show that alterations in cerebellar, frontotemporal, striatal, and prefrontal circuitry can be detected with laboratory tests in unaffected family members of individuals with autism. These results represent a significant contribution to the ongoing development of etiopathophysiological models of autism. The demonstration of cerebellar dysfunction is relevant given findings of cerebellar histopathology in autism, which our results suggest may be familial. The deficit in left frontotemporal circuitry is important because the relevant long-fiber tracts directly overlay pathways crucial for language skills. Impairments in striatal learning systems may affect procedural learning and the development of coordinated motor control. The deficits in executive functions observed in oculomotor and neuropsychological testing are likely a manifestation of alterations in prefrontal systems that are associated with autism. Further work is needed by way of replication of our findings, quantitative evaluation of the familiality of these traits in family trios, and efforts to demonstrate association of oculomotor and other phenotypes with genetic mechanisms.

The distinct pattern of oculomotor abnormalities observed in autism and the specific paradigms evaluating the functional integrity of different neural circuits may provide promising intermediate phenotypes for family research. Furthermore, because the neurophysiological measures were unrelated to atypical social, communication, and cognitive flexibility and head circumference measurements, they may provide independent phenotyping information beyond that yielded by psychological and morphometric evaluations. By providing a direct and quantitative evaluation of the integrity of functional brain systems, oculomotor measures may provide useful tools for localizing neural circuitry alterations in individuals with autism and their unaffected family members.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: John A. Sweeney, PhD, Center for Cognitive Medicine, University of Illinois at Chicago, 912 S Wood St, Mail Code 913, Chicago, IL 60612 (jsweeney@psych.uic.edu).

Submitted for Publication: August 21, 2009; final revision received January 20, 2010; accepted February 8, 2010.

Author Contributions: All statistical analyses were performed by Drs Mosconi and Sweeney.

Financial Disclosure: Dr Sweeney reported receiving funding support through a Janssen Pharmaceutica research grant and as a consultant to Pfizer Inc.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by grant HD35469 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism, grant P50HD055751 from the National Institute of Mental Health Autism Center of Excellence, the National Alliance for Autism Research, and Autism Speaks fellowship 4853.

Role of the Sponsors: The funding agencies had no role in the design, analysis, or reporting of results.

Previous Presentation: Portions of this study were presented at the 2009 Neural Control of Movement conference; May 1, 2009; Kona, Hawaii, and the International Meeting for Autism Research; May 8, 2009; Chicago, Illinois.

Online-Only Material: Supplementary material available at http://ccm.psych.uic.edu/PublishedSupplementaryData/Mosconi_et_al._Autism_family_study_Supplementary_tables.html.

Additional Contributions: We gratefully acknowledge the participation of the individual subjects and their families in these studies.

This article was corrected on November 17, 2010.

References
1.
Pickles  ABolton  PMacdonald  HBailey  ALe Couteur  ASim  CHRutter  M Latent-class analysis of recurrence risks for complex phenotypes with selection and measurement error: a twin and family history study of autism.  Am J Hum Genet 1995;57 (3) 717- 726PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Szatmari  P Heterogeneity and the genetics of autism.  J Psychiatry Neurosci 1999;24 (2) 159- 165PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Bolton  PMacdonald  HPickles  ARios  PGoode  SCrowson  MBailey  ARutter  M A case-control family history study of autism.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994;35 (5) 877- 900PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Landa  RPiven  JWzorek  MMGayle  JOChase  GAFolstein  SE Social language use in parents of autistic individuals.  Psychol Med 1992;22 (1) 245- 254PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Losh  MPiven  J Social-cognition and the broad autism phenotype: identifying genetically meaningful phenotypes.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007;48 (1) 105- 112PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Hughes  CLeboyer  MBouvard  M Executive function in parents of children with autism.  Psychol Med 1997;27 (1) 209- 220PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Constantino  JNLajonchere  CLutz  MGray  TAbbacchi  A McKenna  KSingh  DTodd  RD Autistic social impairment in the siblings of children with pervasive developmental disorders.  Am J Psychiatry 2006;163 (2) 294- 296PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Delorme  RGousse  VRoy  ITrandafir  AMathieu  FMouren-Siméoni  MCBetancur  CLeboyer  M Shared executive dysfunctions in unaffected relatives of patients with autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Eur Psychiatry 2007;22 (1) 32- 38PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Pickles  AStarr  EKazak  SBolton  PPapanikolaou  KBailey  AGoodman  RRutter  M Variable expression of the autism broader phenotype: findings from extended pedigrees.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41 (4) 491- 502PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Losh  MAdolphs  RPoe  MDCouture  SPenn  DBaranek  GTPiven  J Neuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotype.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2009;66 (5) 518- 526PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Fidler  DJBailey  JNSmalley  SL Macrocephaly in autism and other pervasive developmental disorders.  Dev Med Child Neurol 2000;42 (11) 737- 740PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Lainhart  JEBigler  EDBocian  MCoon  HDinh  EDawson  GDeutsch  CKDunn  MEstes  ATager-Flusberg  HFolstein  SHepburn  SHyman  S McMahon  WMinshew  NMunson  JOsann  KOzonoff  SRodier  PRogers  SSigman  MSpence  MAStodgell  CJVolkmar  F Head circumference and height in autism: a study by the Collaborative Program of Excellence in Autism.  Am J Med Genet A 2006;140 (21) 2257- 2274PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Goldberg  JAnderson  GMZwaigenbaum  LHall  GBNahmias  CThompson  ASzatmari  P Cortical serotonin type-2 receptor density in parents of children with autism spectrum disorders.  J Autism Dev Disord 2009;39 (1) 97- 104PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Piven  JTsai  GCNehme  ECoyle  JTChase  GAFolstein  SE Platelet serotonin: a possible marker for familial autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 1991;21 (1) 51- 59PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Dalton  KMNacewicz  BMAlexander  ALDavidson  RJ Gaze-fixation, brain activation, and amygdala volume in unaffected siblings of individuals with autism.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;61 (4) 512- 520PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Dawson  GWebb  SJWijsman  ESchellenberg  GEstes  AMunson  JFaja  S Neurocognitive and electrophysiological evidence of altered face processing in parents of children with autism: implications for a model of abnormal development of social brain circuitry in autism.  Dev Psychopathol 2005;17 (3) 679- 697PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Adolphs  RSpezio  MLParlier  MPiven  J Distinct face-processing strategies in parents of autistic children.  Curr Biol 2008;18 (14) 1090- 1093PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Manning  JTBaron-Cohen  SWheelwright  SSanders  G The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and autism.  Dev Med Child Neurol 2001;43 (3) 160- 164PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Bell  BBAbel  LALi  WChristian  JCYee  RD Concordance of smooth pursuit and saccadic measures in normal monozygotic twin pairs.  Biol Psychiatry 1994;36 (8) 522- 526PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Harris  MSReilly  JLKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Longitudinal studies of antisaccades in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia.  Psychol Med 2006;36 (4) 485- 494PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Lencer  RSprenger  AHarris  MSReilly  JLKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Effects of second-generation antipsychotic medication on smooth pursuit performance in antipsychotic-naive schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008;65 (10) 1146- 1154PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Reilly  JLHarris  MSKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Abnormalities in visually guided saccades suggest corticofugal dysregulation in never-treated schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2005;57 (2) 145- 154PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Yee  CMNuechterlein  KHDawson  ME A longitudinal analysis of eye tracking dysfunction and attention in recent-onset schizophrenia.  Psychophysiology 1998;35 (4) 443- 451PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Luna  BDoll  SKHegedus  SJMinshew  NJSweeney  JA Maturation of executive function in autism.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;61 (4) 474- 481PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Rosenhall  UJohansson  EGillberg  C Oculomotor findings in autistic children.  J Laryngol Otol 1988;102 (5) 435- 439PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Takarae  YMinshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Oculomotor abnormalities parallel cerebellar histopathology in autism.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2004;75 (9) 1359- 1361PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Minshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Oculomotor evidence for neocortical systems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism.  Neurology 1999;52 (5) 917- 922PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Takarae  YMinshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Atypical involvement of frontostriatal systems during sensorimotor control in autism.  Psychiatry Res 2007;156 (2) 117- 127PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Bailey  ALuthert  PDean  AHarding  BJanota  IMontgomery  MRutter  MLantos  P A clinicopathological study of autism.  Brain 1998;121 (pt 5) 889- 905PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Bauman  MLKemper  TL Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism: a review and future directions.  Int J Dev Neurosci 2005;23 (2-3) 183- 187PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Fatemi  SHHalt  ARRealmuto  GEarle  JKist  DAThuras  PMerz  A Purkinje cell size is reduced in cerebellum of patients with autism.  Cell Mol Neurobiol 2002;22 (2) 171- 175PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Ritvo  ERFreeman  BJScheibel  ABDuong  TRobinson  HGuthrie  DRitvo  A Lower Purkinje cell counts in the cerebella of four autistic subjects: initial findings of the UCLA-NSAC Autopsy Research Report.  Am J Psychiatry 1986;143 (7) 862- 866PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Whitney  ERKemper  TLBauman  MLRosene  DLBlatt  GJ Cerebellar Purkinje cells are reduced in a subpopulation of autistic brains: a stereological experiment using calbindin-D28k.  Cerebellum 2008;7 (3) 406- 416PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Fatemi  SHStary  JMHalt  ARRealmuto  GR Dysregulation of reelin and Bcl-2 proteins in autistic cerebellum.  J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31 (6) 529- 535PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Benayed  RGharani  NRossman  IMancuso  VLazar  GKamdar  SBruse  SE Tischfield  SSmith  BJZimmerman  RADicicco-Bloom  EBrzustowicz  LMMillonig  JH Support for the homeobox transcription factor gene ENGRAILED 2 as an autism spectrum disorder susceptibility locus.  Am J Hum Genet 2005;77 (5) 851- 868PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Gharani  NBenayed  RMancuso  VBrzustowicz  LMMillonig  JH Association of the homeobox transcription factor, ENGRAILED 2, 3, with autism spectrum disorder.  Mol Psychiatry 2004;9 (5) 474- 484PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Takarae  YMinshew  NJLuna  BKrisky  CMSweeney  JA Pursuit eye movement deficits in autism.  Brain 2004;127 (pt 12) 2584- 2594PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Takarae  YLuna  BMinshew  NJSweeney  JA Patterns of visual sensory and sensorimotor abnormalities in autism vary in relation to history of early language delay.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14 (6) 980- 989PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Meck  WH Neuropharmacology of timing and time perception.  Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1996;3 (3-4) 227- 242PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Simó  LSKrisky  CMSweeney  JA Functional neuroanatomy of anticipatory behavior: dissociation between sensory-driven and memory-driven systems.  Cereb Cortex 2005;15 (12) 1982- 1991PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Goldberg  MCLasker  AGZee  DSGarth  ETien  ALanda  RJ Deficits in the initiation of eye movements in the absence of a visual target in adolescents with high functioning autism.  Neuropsychologia 2002;40 (12) 2039- 2049PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
D’Cruz  AMMosconi  MWSteele  SRubin  LHLuna  BMinshew  NSweeney  JA Lateralized response timing deficits in autism.  Biol Psychiatry 2009;66 (4) 393- 397PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Mosconi  MWKay  MD’Cruz  AMSeidenfeld  AGuter  SStanford  LDSweeney  JA Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders.  Psychol Med 2009;39 (9) 1559- 1566PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Sweeney  JAMintun  MAKwee  SWiseman  MBBrown  DLRosenberg  DRCarl  JR Positron emission tomography study of voluntary saccadic eye movements and spatial working memory.  J Neurophysiol 1996;75 (1) 454- 468PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Koczat  DLRogers  SJPennington  BFRoss  RG Eye movement abnormality suggestive of a spatial working memory deficit is present in parents of autistic probands.  J Autism Dev Disord 2002;32 (6) 513- 518PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Lord  CRutter  MLe Couteur  A Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders.  J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24 (5) 659- 685PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Lord  CRisi  SLambrecht  LCook  EH  JrLeventhal  BLDiLavore  PCPickles  ARutter  M The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2000;30 (3) 205- 223PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Rutter  MBailey  ALord  C The Social Communication Questionnaire Manual.  Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2003
49.
Bolton  PMacdonald  HPickles  ARios  PGoode  SCrowson  MBailey  ARutter  M A case-control family history study of autism.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1994;35 (5) 877- 900PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Luna  BGarver  KEUrban  TALazar  NASweeney  JA Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood.  Child Dev 2004;75 (5) 1357- 1372PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Saslow  MG Effects of components of displacement-step stimuli upon latency for saccadic eye movement.  J Opt Soc Am 1967;57 (8) 1024- 1029PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Lisberger  SGWestbrook  LE Properties of visual inputs that initiate horizontal smooth pursuit eye movements in monkeys.  J Neurosci 1985;5 (6) 1662- 1673PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Harris  MSWiseman  CLReilly  JLKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Effects of risperidone on procedural learning in antipsychotic-naive first-episode schizophrenia.  Neuropsychopharmacology 2009;34 (2) 468- 476PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Goodman  WKPrice  LHRasmussen  SAMazure  CFleischmann  RLHill  CLHeninger  GRCharney  DS The Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale, I: development, use, and reliability.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1989;46 (11) 1006- 1011PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Scahill  LRiddle  MA McSwiggin-Hardin  MOrt  SIKing  RAGoodman  WKCicchetti  DLeckman  JF Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale: reliability and validity.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36 (6) 844- 852PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Baron-Cohen  SWheelwright  SSkinner  RMartin  JClubley  E The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians.  J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31 (1) 5- 17PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Erwin  RJGur  RCGur  RESkolnick  BMawhinney-Hee  MSmailis  J Facial emotion discrimination, I: task construction and behavioral findings in normal subjects.  Psychiatry Res 1992;42 (3) 231- 240PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Roche  AFMukherjee  DGuo  SMMoore  WM Head circumference reference data: birth to 18 years.  Pediatrics 1987;79 (5) 706- 712PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Keller  ELSlakey  DPCrandall  WF Microstimulation of the primate cerebellar vermis during saccadic eye movements.  Brain Res 1983;288 (1-2) 131- 143PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Iwamoto  YYoshida  K Saccadic dysmetria following inactivation of the primate fastigial oculomotor region.  Neurosci Lett 2002;325 (3) 211- 215PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Noda  HMurakami  SWarabi  T Effects of fastigial stimulation upon visually-directed saccades in macaque monkeys.  Neurosci Res 1991;10 (3) 188- 199PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Golla  HTziridis  KHaarmeier  TCatz  NBarash  SThier  P Reduced saccadic resilience and impaired saccadic adaptation due to cerebellar disease.  Eur J Neurosci 2008;27 (1) 132- 144PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Catani  MJones  DKDaly  EEmbiricos  NDeeley  QPugliese  LCurran  SRobertson  DMurphy  DG Altered cerebellar feedback projections in Asperger syndrome.  Neuroimage 2008;41 (4) 1184- 1191PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Courchesne  EYeung-Courchesne  RPress  GAHesselink  JRJernigan  TL Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in autism.  N Engl J Med 1988;318 (21) 1349- 1354PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Webb  SJSparks  BFFriedman  SDShaw  DWGiedd  JDawson  GDager  SR Cerebellar vermal volumes and behavioral correlates in children with autism spectrum disorder.  Psychiatry Res 2009;172 (1) 61- 67PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
66.
Cleavinger  HBBigler  EDJohnson  JLLu  J McMahon  WLainhart  JE Quantitative magnetic resonance image analysis of the cerebellum in macrocephalic and normocephalic children and adults with autism.  J Int Neuropsychol Soc 2008;14 (3) 401- 413PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
67.
Zeegers  MPol  HHDurston  SNederveen  HSchnack  Hvan Daalen  EDietz  Cvan Engeland  HBuitelaar  J No differences in MR-based volumetry between 2- and 7-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder and developmental delay.  Brain Dev 2009;31 (10) 725- 730PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
68.
Sharpe  JA Neurophysiology and neuroanatomy of smooth pursuit: lesion studies.  Brain Cogn 2008;68 (3) 241- 254PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
69.
Clementz  BAReid  SA McDowell  JECadenhead  KS Abnormality of smooth pursuit eye movement initiation: specificity to the schizophrenia spectrum?  Psychophysiology 1995;32 (2) 130- 134PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
70.
Levy  DLHolzman  PSMatthysse  SMendell  NR Eye tracking and schizophrenia: a selective review.  Schizophr Bull 1994;20 (1) 47- 62PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
71.
Hong  LEMitchell  BDAvila  MTAdami  H McMahon  RPThaker  GK Familial aggregation of eye-tracking endophenotypes in families of schizophrenic patients.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (3) 259- 264PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
72.
Holzman  PSKringlen  ELevy  DLProctor  LRHaberman  SJYasillo  NJ Abnormal-pursuit eye movements in schizophrenia: evidence for a genetic indicator.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 1977;34 (7) 802- 805PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
73.
Lencer  RReilly  JLHarris  MSSprenger  AKeshavan  MSSweeney  JA Sensorimotor transformation deficits for smooth pursuit in first-episode affective psychoses and schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry 2010;67 (3) 217- 223PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
74.
Sweeney  JALuna  BHaas  GLKeshavan  MSMann  JJThase  ME Pursuit tracking impairments in schizophrenia and mood disorders: step-ramp studies with unmedicated patients.  Biol Psychiatry 1999;46 (5) 671- 680PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
75.
Dieterich  MMuller-Schunk  SStephan  TBense  SSeelos  KYousry  TA Functional magnetic resonance imaging activations of cortical eye fields during saccades, smooth pursuit, and optokinetic nystagmus.  Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1164282- 292PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
76.
Mustari  MJOno  SDas  VE Signal processing and distribution in cortical-brainstem pathways for smooth pursuit eye movements.  Ann N Y Acad Sci 2009;1164147- 154PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
77.
Meck  WHPenney  TBPouthas  V Cortico-striatal representation of time in animals and humans.  Curr Opin Neurobiol 2008;18 (2) 145- 152PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
78.
Herbert  MRHarris  GJAdrien  KTZiegler  DAMakris  NKennedy  DNLange  NTChabris  CFBakardjiev  AHodgson  JTakeoka  MTager-Flusberg  HCaviness  VS  Jr Abnormal asymmetry in language association cortex in autism.  Ann Neurol 2002;52 (5) 588- 596PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
79.
Müller  RABehen  MERothermel  RDChugani  DCMuzik  OMangner  TJChugani  HT Brain mapping of language and auditory perception in high-functioning autistic adults: a PET study.  J Autism Dev Disord 1999;29 (1) 19- 31PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
80.
Cornish  KM McManus  IC Hand preference and hand skill in children with autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 1996;26 (6) 597- 609PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
81.
Escalante-Mead  PRMinshew  NJSweeney  JA Abnormal brain lateralization in high-functioning autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2003;33 (5) 539- 543PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
82.
Soper  HVSatz  POrsini  DLHenry  RRZvi  JCSchulman  M Handedness patterns in autism suggest subtypes.  J Autism Dev Disord 1986;16 (2) 155- 167PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
83.
McAlonan  GMDaly  EKumari  VCritchley  HDvan Amelsvoort  TSuckling  JSimmons  ASigmundsson  TGreenwood  KRussell  ASchmitz  NHappe  FHowlin  PMurphy  DG Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger's syndrome.  Brain 2002;125 (pt 7) 1594- 1606PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
84.
McAlonan  GMCheung  CCheung  VWong  NSuckling  JChua  SE Differential effects on white-matter systems in high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome.  Psychol Med 2009;39 (11) 1885- 1893PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
85.
Waiter  GDWilliams  JHMurray  ADGilchrist  APerrett  DIWhiten  A Structural white matter deficits in high-functioning individuals with autistic spectrum disorder: a voxel-based investigation.  Neuroimage 2005;24 (2) 455- 461PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
86.
Ghaziuddin  MMountain-Kimchi  K Defining the intellectual profile of Asperger syndrome: comparison with high-functioning autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2004;34 (3) 279- 284PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
87.
Koyama  TTachimori  HOsada  HTakeda  TKurita  H Cognitive and symptom profiles in Asperger's syndrome and high-functioning autism.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 2007;61 (1) 99- 104PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
88.
Klin  AVolkmar  FRSparrow  SSCicchetti  DVRourke  BP Validity and neuropsychological characterization of Asperger syndrome: convergence with nonverbal learning disabilities syndrome.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1995;36 (7) 1127- 1140PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
89.
Hughes  CPlumet  MHLeboyer  M Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40 (5) 705- 718PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
90.
Thakkar  KNPolli  FEJoseph  RMTuch  DSHadjikhani  NBarton  JJManoach  DS Response monitoring, repetitive behaviour and anterior cingulate abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders (ASD).  Brain 2008;131 (pt 9) 2464- 2478PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
91.
Schmitz  NRubia  KDaly  ESmith  AWilliams  SMurphy  DG Neural correlates of executive function in autistic spectrum disorders.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (1) 7- 16PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
92.
Kana  RKKeller  TAMinshew  NJJust  MA Inhibitory control in high-functioning autism: decreased activation and underconnectivity in inhibition networks.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;62 (3) 198- 206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
93.
Goldberg  MCMostofsky  SHCutting  LEMahone  EMAstor  BCDenckla  MBLanda  RJ Subtle executive impairment in children with autism and children with ADHD.  J Autism Dev Disord 2005;35 (3) 279- 293PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
94.
Steele  SDMinshew  NJLuna  BSweeney  JA Spatial working memory deficits in autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2007;37 (4) 605- 612PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
95.
Williams  DLGoldstein  GCarpenter  PAMinshew  NJ Verbal and spatial working memory in autism.  J Autism Dev Disord 2005;35 (6) 747- 756PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
96.
Luna  BMinshew  NJGarver  KELazar  NAThulborn  KREddy  WFSweeney  JA Neocortical system abnormalities in autism: an fMRI study of spatial working memory.  Neurology 2002;59 (6) 834- 840PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
97.
Le Couteur  ABailey  AGoode  SPickles  ARobertson  SGottesman  IRutter  M A broader phenotype of autism: the clinical spectrum in twins.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37 (7) 785- 801PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
98.
Losh  MChildress  DLam  KPiven  J Defining key features of the broad autism phenotype: a comparison across parents of multiple- and single-incidence autism families.  Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2008;147B (4) 424- 433PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×