Context
Schizophrenia is the collective term for a heterogeneous group of mental disorders with a still obscure biological basis. In particular, the specific contribution of risk or candidate gene variants to the complex schizophrenic phenotype is largely unknown.
Objective
To prepare the ground for a novel “phenomics” approach, a unique schizophrenia patient database was established by GRAS (Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia), designed to allow association of genetic information with quantifiable phenotypes. Because synaptic dysfunction plays a key role in schizophrenia, the complexin 2 gene (CPLX2) was examined in the first phenotype-based genetic association study (PGAS) of GRAS.
Design
Subsequent to a classic case-control approach, we analyzed the contribution of CPLX2 polymorphisms to discrete cognitive domains within the schizophrenic population. To gain mechanistic insight into how certain CPLX2 variants influence gene expression and function, peripheral blood mononuclear cells of patients, Cplx -null mutant mice, and transfected cells were investigated.
Setting
Coordinating research center (Max Planck Institute of Experimental Medicine) and 23 collaborating psychiatric centers all over Germany.
Participants
One thousand seventy-one patients with schizophrenia (DSM-IV) examined by an invariant investigator team, resulting in the GRAS database with more than 3000 phenotypic data points per patient, and 1079 healthy control subjects of comparable ethnicity.
Main Outcome Measure
Cognitive performance including executive functioning, reasoning, and verbal learning/memory.
Results
Six single-nucleotide polymorphisms, distributed over the whole CPLX2 gene, were found to be highly associated with current cognition of schizophrenic subjects but only marginally with premorbid intelligence. Correspondingly, in Cplx2 -null mutant mice, prominent cognitive loss of function was obtained only in combination with a minor brain lesion applied during puberty, modeling a clinically relevant environmental risk (“second hit”) for schizophrenia. In the human CPLX2 gene, 1 of the identified 6 cognition-relevant single-nucleotide polymorphisms, rs3822674 in the 3′ untranslated region, was detected to influence microRNA-498 binding and gene expression. The same marker was associated with differential expression of CPLX2 in peripheral blood mononuclear cells.
Conclusions
The PGAS allows identification of marker-associated clinical/biological traits. Current cognitive performance in schizophrenic patients is modified by CPLX2 variants modulating posttranscriptional gene expression.
Detailed knowledge of the complex gene-environment interactions underlying the etiology and pathogenesis of the schizophrenias1 is required to understand the causes of the disease and to establish causal therapies. However, the search for genetic markers of vulnerability for schizophrenia has been hampered by extensive interfering effects of patient heterogeneity and environmental factors.2 Indeed, many of the numerous genetic association studies have yielded poorly reproducible data, likely because corresponding patient cohorts were too small and/or too heterogeneous with regard to clinical characteristics. One way to circumvent the problems that are posed by the lack of exact phenotyping in the available schizophrenia samples, and thus confound genetic studies, is to analyze large patient cohorts. Three recent studies of this type3-5 showed that several common genetic variations contribute to the risk of schizophrenia, but the functional consequences of these variations are still completely unknown.
In the present study, we opted for an alternative approach to study genetic causes of the schizophrenic phenotype: a “phenotype”-based genetic association study (PGAS). This approach is different from and complementary to the genome-wide association studies on schizophrenia as a disease. With the PGAS, we are not looking for major disease genes in schizophrenia because such genes may not exist. Rather than searching for schizophrenia genes, we seek to learn more about the contribution of genetic variants of certain candidate genes to the schizophrenic phenotype. Obviously, traits of interest in schizophrenia can never be explained only by a single modifier gene. However, a particular gene may codetermine (with other trait-relevant genes) the outcome of an individual with schizophrenia.
Based on the assumption that valuable information about relevant genetic disease mechanisms can be obtained by association studies on patient cohorts of at least 1000 patients, if performed on very detailed clinical data sets and quantifiable biological readouts of schizophrenia rather than the end point diagnosis in comparison with healthy controls, we generated a new schizophrenia patient database, the GRAS (Göttingen Research Association for Schizophrenia) data collection. For this purpose, 1071 patients with schizophrenia were recruited between July 21, 2005, and July 7, 2008, by one and the same team of traveling investigators in a cross-sectional field study, consisting of 23 German psychiatric hospitals (listed in the supplementary Appendix). The corresponding data set includes biographical and family information, disease history, environmental risk factors, comorbidities, treatments, and the results of cross-sectional psychopathological, neuropsychological, and neurological examinations. With more than 3000 data points per subject, this unique database of living patients, who are accessible for follow-up studies, provides a comprehensive and standardized phenotype characterization of as yet unprecedented detail.
Neurocognitive impairments, including deficits in executive functions, attention, and memory, are core symptoms of schizophrenia and the main cause of disease-related disability.6 With regard to these and other schizophrenia symptoms, synaptic dysfunctions and disruptions of functional synaptic connectivity have been proposed to play a key role, and some authors even call schizophrenia a disease of the synapse.7-9 Numerous reports have implicated genes encoding synaptic proteins in the etiology of schizophrenia.10,11 Postmortem studies on the brain tissue of schizophrenic patients have consistently observed alterations in the expression of several synaptic proteins. In this context, the complexin (CPLX) family of presynaptic regulatory proteins is particularly interesting.12-18 This family consists of 4 members (CPLX1-CPLX4), of which only Cplx1 and Cplx2 are strongly expressed in rodent forebrain.19 They play an essential role in the regulation of synaptic transmitter release by controlling assembly and stability of exocytotic soluble N -ethylmaleimide-sensitive factor attachment protein receptor (SNARE) complexes and thereby influence synaptic signaling,20,21 synaptic plasticity,22,23 and neuronal network function.24
Whereas previous genetic association studies exploring CPLX1 (OMIM 605032) or CPLX2 (OMIM 605033) as classic genetic risk factors for schizophrenia have yielded inconclusive results,25,26 interesting findings were described regarding CPLX1 and CPLX2 messenger RNA (mRNA) and protein analysis in postmortem brain tissue. Although no systematic overall analysis of CPLX1 and CPLX2 expression patterns in normal human brain has been published, a number of reports are available on CPLX mRNA and protein changes in discrete brain areas of schizophrenic patients and control subjects, providing evidence of a decreased CPLX2 mRNA expression in regions of high relevance for schizophrenia (eg, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, superior temporal cortex, and certain regions of the hippocampus). A correspondence to decreased CPLX2 protein levels in these regions is difficult to establish because the literature is scarce.27
In addition to schizophrenia, altered CPLX /CPLX levels were measured in different brain areas of patients with diverse other neuropsychiatric disorders and implicated in the disease process, perhaps as determinants of a final common pathological pathway. Among these neuropsychiatric disorders are Huntington disease,28,29 Alzheimer disease,30 and bipolar disorder,14,17 all of them characterized, among others, by cognitive impairments. Interestingly, CPLX1 and CPLX2 expression changes in the hippocampus were found to be associated with cognitive deficits in schizophrenia,31 still leaving unanswered whether the altered tissue levels reflect the cause or the consequence of disease-related cognitive dysfunction.
In this regard, behavioral consequences observed in null mutant mice have to be considered. Although Cplx1 -null mutant mice show severe ataxia, they have no clear cognitive phenotype and only subtle alterations in social behavior.32 The Cplx2 -null mutant mice have an even milder phenotype with slight motor abnormalities and equivocal results regarding cognition under unchallenged conditions.33-35 However, when maternal deprivation stress presents a second hit, cognitive dysfunction and decreased induction of hippocampal long-term potentiation in Cplx2 -null mutant mice become evident.34 A similar second hit effect may be involved in the disease-related function of another presynaptic protein, 25-kDa synaptosome-associated protein (SNAP-25).36
Taking all these observations together, we hypothesized that common genetic variants of CPLX2 may play an important role as modifiers of cognition in situations of additional challenge. Such a second hit to the brain, which ultimately leads to the disease, may cause a schizophrenic phenotype of variable severity depending on the genetic variant present. Searching for mechanistic insight, we hypothesized that the respective variants, if located in the noncoding region of the gene, may influence quantitative gene expression, for example, by modulating binding of microRNAs (miRs), which are increasingly recognized as significant contributors to the adaptive fine-tuning of synaptic functions in the brain.37,38 To lend further support to our second hit concept, we investigated the effect of a mild peripubertal neurotrauma on cognition in Cplx2 -null mutant mice.
A more comprehensive description of the patient population and all methods and materials applied is provided in the supplementary Methods section (see also eTable 1).
The GRAS data collection was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Georg-August-University (master committee) and the local internal review boards of the collaborating centers. The project complied with the Helsinki declaration.39 Patients fulfilling DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia40,41 (all types, eg, paranoid, disorganized, catatonic, and undifferentiated,42 proven or suspected) or schizoaffective disorder were included (N = 1071), regardless of the stage of the disorder (acute, chronic, residual, or remitted). A total of 792 patients (73.9%) were diagnosed as having schizophrenia; 159 (14.8%), schizoaffective disorder; and 120 (11.2%), suspected schizophrenic psychosis (other psychotic disorder or yet to be confirmed). The mean (SD) age was 39.6 (12.8) years, with a range from 18 to 83 years. Seven hundred fourteen (66.7%) were men (age, 37.6 [12.0] years) and 357 (33.3%) were women (age, 43.7 [13.3] years). Subjects (all older than 18 years) and, if applicable, their legal representatives, gave written informed consent.
Patients were recruited in the 23 German psychiatric hospitals listed in the supplementary Appendix, and almost all of them were of European Caucasian descent (Caucasian, 95.3%; other, 1.6%; unknown, 3.1%). European Caucasians are a genetically homogeneous group with low average levels of genetic differentiation when compared with other human populations (no strong influence on association results to be expected).43-45 Specifically, the German population is very homogeneous, with a low genetic differentiation along a north-south gradient within Germany. In fact, population substructure within Germany is too low to be detectable without prior information on subpopulation membership.46
The control subjects were voluntary blood donors recruited by the Department of Transfusion Medicine at the Georg-August-University according to national guidelines for blood donation. As such, they widely fulfill health criteria, ensured by a broad predonation screening process containing standardized questionnaires, interviews, hemoglobin levels, and blood pressure, pulse, and body temperature determinations. Of the 1079 controls, 635 (58.9%) were men and 444 (41.1%) were women. The mean (SD) age was 34.7 (12.3) years, with a range from 18 to 69 years. Comparable to the patient population, almost all controls were of European Caucasian descent (Caucasian, 97.8%; other, 2.0%; unknown, 0.2%). All donors gave written informed consent.
Comprehensive interviews and testing were performed by the same traveling team of trained examiners (psychiatrists and psychologists) using the GRAS Manual described in the supplementary Methods. Briefly, structured interviews were conducted to explore biographical and family information, level of education, quality-of-life indicators, disease history, and exposure to prenatal, perinatal, and/or postnatal environmental risk factors. Likewise, the psychopathological profile, psychiatric comorbidities, and current/former treatments were assessed. Psychometric rating, neuropsychological tests, and neurological examinations were also performed.
Standard methods were used for DNA extraction from peripheral blood cells (Genomed GmbH, Löhne, Germany). Sequencing was performed using the dideoxy chain termination method (BigDye Terminator version 3.1 cycle sequencing kit on a 3730XL DNA analyzer; Applied Biosystems, Foster City, California). Genotyping was performed with simple probes (TIB Molbiol, Berlin, Germany) on a real-time polymerase chain reaction instrument (LightCycler 480; Roche Diagnostics GmbH, Mannheim, Germany).
Expression analysis was conducted in Neuro-2a (N2a) cells (LGC Standards GmbH, Wesel, Germany) with the dual-luciferase reporter system (Promega, Mannheim, Germany). Briefly, the first 274 base pairs (bp) of the 3′ untranslated region (3′UTR) of CPLX2, containing single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) rs3822674 with a C or a T allele, were amplified from respective human samples and cloned into Renilla luciferase vector phRL-SV40 (Promega). The N2a cells were plated in 96-well plates, cultured for 16 to 18 hours, and transfected using a commercially available reagent (Lipofectamine 2000; Invitrogen, Karlsruhe, Germany). A total of 1 ng of phRL-SV40 (T or C construct or the vector without the insert) and 1 pg of pCMV-FFluc control vector (Promega) were cotransfected with/without 1 pmol of hsa-miR-498 (Ambion, Foster City, California). The dual-luciferase reporter assay was performed 24 hours after transfection according to the manufacturer's protocol (Promega). Measurements were conducted with a commercially available microplate reader (Mitras LB 940; Berthold Technologies GmbH, Regensdorf, Switzerland). Renilla values were divided by the corresponding firefly readings producing values expressed as relative luciferase units.
Isolation of peripheral blood mononuclear cells and quantitative real-time reverse transcriptase–polymerase chain reaction
Blood was collected in citrate phosphate dextrose adenine tubes from schizophrenic patients with different genotypes (CC, CT, and TT) at SNP rs3822674 in the 3′UTR. Six patients had the CC genotype (mean [SD] age, 48.5 [5.3] years); of these, 3 were men (age, 52.3 [4.7] years) and 3 were women; (age, 44.6 [10.1] years). Four had the CT genotype (age, 41.8 [3.4] years), and all 4 were men. Six had the TT genotype (age, 47.5 [5.8] years); of these, 5 were men (age, 45.6 [6.7] years) and 1 was a woman (age, 57 years). Peripheral blood mononuclear cells (PBMCs) were isolated applying a standard isolation procedure (Ficoll-Paque Plus; GE Healthcare, München, Germany). The RNA was prepared using a commercially available kit (miRNeasy Mini Kit; Qiagen GmbH, Hilden, Germany). The RNA samples were used to synthesize complementary DNAs (SuperScriptIII; Invitrogen). The quantitative real-time polymerase chain reaction analysis was performed using the fluorescent dye SYBR Green (LightCycler 480; Roche Diagnostics GmbH). The cycle threshold values were standardized to the cycle threshold values of glyceraldehyde-3-phosphate dehydrogenase. Primers are listed in the supplementary Methods section.
Male Cplx2 -null mutant mice vs wild-type littermates with or without juvenile parietal cortical cryolesion47 underwent behavioral testing, including the Morris water maze, at 10 months of age.
Statistical analyses were performed with commercially available software (GraphPad Prism, version 5.01; GraphPad Software Inc, La Jolla, California) for experimental data (animal study and expression experiments). For transfection studies and for PBMC analysis, 2-tailed pairwise Mann-Whitney test was applied; for mouse behavior studies, 2-way analysis of variance for repeated measures was used. For human data, haplotype association analyses of binary categorical variables was performed with UNPHASED (version 3.0.13; Frank Dudbridge, http://www.mrc-bsu.cam.ac.uk/personal/frank/software/unphased/), performing likelihood ratio tests in a log-linear model through unconditional logistic regression adjusting for age and sex.48 Correlations of the 3 quantitative modifier variables (executive functioning, reasoning, and verbal learning/memory) and the quantitative constitutive variable (premorbid intelligence) were assessed using Pearson product-moment correlation,49 determining internal consistency of the 3 target variables by the Cronbach α coefficient.50 All metric phenotypic variables were standardized to be normally distributed with zero mean and variance 1 (by Blom Transformation51 [described in the supplementary Methods section]). Subsequently, linear models with covariate adjustment were used for single-marker association analyses (R, version 2.8.1; http://cran.r-project.org) and for haplotype association analyses of the standardized quantitative traits (SAS, version 9.152), with PLINK (version 1.06)53 for estimation and tabulation of all possible individuals' haplotype phases and their posterior probabilities (given the genotype data). Multivariate analysis modeled a target phenotype vector, accounting for individual correlation between vector entries. For single-marker association analyses, a permutation test was used to estimate multiple testing adjusted P values to obtain by chance no less than the observed amount of significances for a given set of tests (Table). All other P values are nominal, with P less than .05 indicating significance. Details for quantitative trait analyses and for the permutation test are given in the supplementary Methods section.
Information on CPLX2 sequences was obtained from the National Center for Biotechnology Information (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/) and the University of California–Santa Cruz (http://genome.ucsc.edu/) (GeneID, 10814). Accession numbers for the 2 CPLX2 transcripts are NM_006650.3 (T1) and NM_001008220.1 (T2).
CPLX2 GENOTYPING AND ASSOCIATION STUDIES
We analyzed first the genetic variability of the CPLX2 gene54 in 1071 schizophrenic patients of the GRAS sample vs 1079 healthy control subjects with comparable ethnicity. The CPLX2 coding region (exons 4-6), adjacent introns, and part of the 3′UTR were sequenced, revealing the presence of 4 SNPs in this region. The putative promoters, upstream of or within exons 1 and 3, and adjacent introns were analyzed by direct genotyping of 7 selected SNPs covering this region (Figure 1 and eTable 2).
On sequencing of the coding region of CPLX2, no informative mutations were found in schizophrenic or healthy control subjects (eTable 3). A simple case-control association study, based on end point diagnosis and single markers, did not yield significant differences between cases and controls regarding genotypic or allelic frequencies (eTable 4). Three main haploblocks, closely resembling those described for this region by the HapMap Project (http://www.hapmap.org), were also identified in our population (Figure 1). Here, a haplotypic combination covering these 3 haploblocks and consisting of SNPs rs2443541/rs3892909/+251Ex6 appeared to be increased in cases (14.6%) vs controls (12.0%) (odds ratio [OR], 1.31; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.98-1.76; χ21 = 3.92; P = .048). This result, however, is of borderline significance and will need further confirmation. In contrast, a low-frequency haplotype (2.3% in cases vs 0.9% in controls) within haploblock 2, including SNPs rs1366116 and rs3892909, showed higher association with schizophrenia (OR, 2.47; 95% CI, 1.41-4.34; χ21 = 11.15; P < .001).
Phenotype-Based Genetic Association Study
In a first analysis of associations between genetic signatures and specific biological readouts within the GRAS group of schizophrenic patients, we focused on cognitive performance. Regarding the cognitive tests used, we followed our hypothesis of CPLX2 influencing current higher brain functions but not premorbid intelligence. This hypothesis was mainly derived from our Cplx2 -null mutant mouse study, exploring the effect of a second hit to the brain. Other neuropsychological tests performed in the frame of the GRAS project (which we looked at later for exploratory reasons) turned out to be less or not affected by CPLX2 gene variants (data not shown).
To examine the role of CPLX2 in cognition, we constructed a phenotypic intercorrelation network consisting of test results for executive functioning, reasoning, and verbal learning/memory as target (modifier) variables that are subject to potential influence of disease-modifying factors. Premorbid intelligence, representing the development dependent intellectual state at disease onset, was selected as the constitutive variable, expected to influence the cognitive phenotype and to correlate with the target variables but to be essentially independently regulated (Figure 2A). Determination of premorbid intelligence assists in estimation of the cognitive decline in brain diseases such as schizophrenia. The most frequently used tests (including the one applied herein, the Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz Test B56) measure vocabulary skills, which essentially depend on the level of education reached at the time point of disease onset.57 This vocabulary knowledge, once acquired, tends to stay on (frozen) even if other cognitive skills decline owing to the disease process. As expected, there is a highly significant correlation between the number of years in school according to the final degree as a measure of level of education and the test results of the Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz Test B (in the GRAS sample at present, r = 0.45; P < .001).
The cognitive target variables together yielded a phenotypic intercorrelation network of high quality (Cronbach α = .76), providing a solid basis for multivariate analyses. After correction for age and sex, 4 SNPs in the CPLX2 gene (among them 2 markers belonging to the haplotypes of risk described in the case-control approach) correlated with overall neurocognitive function as evaluated by our multivariate model. Tests of univariate phenotypes—executive functioning, reasoning, and verbal learning/memory—showed this association even for 6 SNPs (Table). As expected, the constitutive variable premorbid intelligence was not significantly associated with single SNPs. The Table provides a synopsis of all tested SNPs of CPLX2 for the multivariate model and the univariate phenotypes, as well as for the constitutive variable. Global functioning is presented as a disease-relevant control variable. Raw data and the influence of specific genotypes on cognition are shown in eFigure 1 and eTable 5.
Multilocus analysis, including all SNPs with any effects on cognition in individual neuropsychological tests (Table), showed that the combination of markers has a statistically significant effect on cognitive performance in the multivariate model and the univariate phenotypes (eTable 6). The haplotypic combination CTC at markers rs1366116/rs3892909/rs3822674 predicts the worst cognitive outcome for all target phenotypes and even for the constitutive variable. In fact, the CTC haplotype shows significantly decreased cognitive performance compared with all other haplotypic combinations (eTable 7).
Search for mechanisms of snp function
Allele-Dependent Reporter Gene Expression
To gain a first insight into the mechanisms by which the identified SNPs might modulate certain domains of cognition, we performed an extensive analysis of the relevant genomic sequences (see the supplementary Methods section). One of the cognition-relevant SNPs that we identified (rs3822674) is located within a predicted binding site of the hsa-miR-498 in the 3′UTR of the CPLX2 mRNA. This binding site is highly conserved among species (Figure 2B and C as well as eFigure 2 and eTable 7). Allele-dependent structural predictions and ΔΔG values indicated that the C-to-T exchange affects miR binding and might therefore modulate CPLX2 expression. To test this, we cloned the first 274 bp of the 3′UTR of CPLX2 downstream of a luciferase reporter gene (phRL-SV40; Figure 2D) and transfected this construct into N2a cells. On addition of hsa-miR-498, luciferase expression was significantly (>50%) reduced in the presence of the T allele (U = 0.00; P = .002), whereas the presence of the C allele yielded an expression comparable to that of the control (vector lacking 3′UTR insert) (Figure 2E). These results suggest a role of SNP rs3822674 in posttranscriptional regulation of CPLX2 expression that may become relevant on miR profile changes in specific neuronal subsets in response to, for example, brain injury.38
Spontaneous Genotype-Dependent CPLX2 mRNA Expression in PBMCs
To test the influence of genotype on baseline CPLX2 mRNA levels, PBMCs from patients with the CC, CT, or TT genotype at SNP rs3822674 were analyzed. There was a significant genotype effect, with the TT genotype having the highest levels of the transcript (Figure 2F).
MODELING COGNITIVE EFFECTS OF ALTERED Cplx2 EXPRESSION IN MICE
Based on the hypothesis that altered CPLX2 expression influences cognitive performance, we investigated mice with a Cplx 2-null mutation.58 These mice develop essentially normally and lack major behavioral abnormalities. To test for a potential influence of a second hit to their brains, a right parietal cortical cryolesion was applied stereotactically at a vulnerable time (ie, puberty [day 28 of life]). This lesion paradigm had originally been developed to model the neurodegenerative processes of schizophrenia47 spreading from the initiation site, the parietal lobe, to other cortical areas.59 Specific deficits in spatial memory (Morris water maze, escape latency) became evident only in Cplx2 -null mutant mice that had received a peripubertal lesion (F1,33 = 10.83; P = .002), but not in identically treated wild-type littermates (F1,23 = 2.27; P = .146) (Figure 2G). Also, only lesioned Cplx2 -null mutant mice showed absence of any preference for the target quadrant (Morris water maze, probe trial, F1,56 = 4.82; P = .032) (Figure 2H). Hence, a cognition modifier role of Cplx2 was revealed only upon combination of a Cplx2 -null mutation with an environmental cofactor (ie, parietal cryolesion as a second hit), leading to a remarkable deterioration of cognitive performance.
Collectively, these data support a modifier role of CPLX2 variants on cognitive performance in schizophrenia. Whereas our conventional case-control study revealed schizophrenia-at-risk haplotypes of the CPLX2 gene, constituting several of the investigated markers, only the PGAS allowed the specific identification of 6 cognition-related SNPs. These SNPs in turn may become relevant mainly on additional (environmental) cofactors, that is, second hits. The effects of CPLX2 genotypes on current cognition of schizophrenic patients observed herein do not exclude a comparable role in other neuropsychiatric disorders or even in healthy individuals in which, for instance, aging would be an inevitable second hit. In this regard, further extensive studies on different populations are required. Among the mechanisms mediating genotype-dependent CPLX2 expression upon second hit may be the binding of hsa-miR-498 to SNP rs3822674 in the 3′UTR of the CPLX2 mRNA that leads to its subsequent downregulation.
Measuring spontaneous CPLX2 mRNA expression in PBMCs of schizophrenic patients reveals different levels dependent on the genotype, with the best cognitive performers (TT at SNP rs3822674) showing the highest expression. In contrast, the CC carriers have lower mRNA expression and reduced cognitive capabilities. Most important, however, CC and TT carriers are not different when compared with respect to their premorbid intelligence, reflecting cognitive abilities before they were “hit by the disease.” Correspondingly, Cplx2 -null mutant mice are severely cognitively impaired only after a second hit, delivered in the present study by mild neurotrauma (juvenile parietal cortical cryolesion) and in a prior report by maternal deprivation stress.34 Intriguingly, only the T allele at SNP rs3822674 (resulting in higher baseline levels of CPLX2 mRNA and better cognition) allows binding of hsa-miR-498 and thus regulability, that is, subsequent downregulation of CPLX2 mRNA.
Current models of a dichotomous Cplx function assume that Cplxs have a facilitatory role in transmitter release, by stabilizing SNARE complexes in a highly fusogenic state, as well as an inhibitory role, by clamping SNARE complexes and thus preventing them from executing synaptic vesicle fusion until triggered by an action potential and the concomitant increase in the intrasynaptic calcium ion concentration. Indeed, calcium ion–regulated exocytosis in many different preparations is inhibited to similar degrees by increased or decreased Cplx activity.21,27
Based on these facts and the cognitive data from our patients, we conclude that the regulability necessary for maintaining or adjusting the homeostasis of CPLX2 expression may constitute a key factor in the fine tuning of synaptic function. This concept has similarly been suggested earlier on the basis of in vitro studies60 and is in agreement with the notions that (1) loss as well as overexpression of CPLXs/Cplxs can perturb presynaptic secretory function, and that (2) CPLXs combine facilitatory and inhibitory functions with respect to synaptic secretion. This functional combination has been delineated in a recent study also demonstrating that in different CPLXs the balance between facilitatory and inhibitory activities may be different.61
Considering the large number of human postmortem studies describing abnormalities in the absolute amount or the ratios of the different CPLX proteins and CPLX mRNAs,13-18,31 these dichotomous CPLX/Cplx functions and their tight regulation under physiological conditions may be of major relevance for pathological cognition seen in several neuropsychiatric diseases. The mechanistic basis for the observed alterations in tissue concentration, however, is presently far from clear.
Analyses of mouse models failed to provide evidence of simple compensatory changes in Cplx expression in the corresponding Cplx1 –, Cplx2 –, Cplx3 –, or Cplx4 –single-null mutant mice.58,62,63 Expression levels of all other presynaptic proteins tested so far were also found to be unaltered in Cplx1 -null mutant brains (α-SNAP, Munc13-1, Munc13-2, Munc13-3, Munc18-1, N -ethylmaleimide-sensitive factor, SNAP-25, synapsin I/IIa, synapsin IIb, synaptobrevin 2, synaptophysin, synaptotagmin 1, syntaxin 1, and vesicular γ-aminobutyric acid transporter), in Cplx2 -null mutant brains (α-SNAP, Munc13-1, Munc13-2, Munc13-3, Munc18-1, SNAP-25, synapsin I/IIa, synapsin IIb, synaptobrevin 2, synaptophysin, synaptotagmin 1, and syntaxin 1), and in Cplx1/2 -double-null mutant brains (SNAP-25, synaptobrevin 2, synaptotagmin 1, syntaxin 1, and vesicular γ-aminobutyric acid transporter).58 In Cplx3/4 -double-null mutant retina, only Ribeye expression was reduced among 17 presynaptic proteins tested.62 Based on these findings, we would argue that even loss of CPLX2/Cplx2 is unlikely to be compensated for by robust changes in the expression levels of other related presynaptic proteins. Functionally relevant alterations, however, may be subtle and escape detection by the available methods. Also, given that pathological states were not explored and that not all of the many dozens of proteins were tested that might act in a compensatory manner on loss of Cplx2, the possibility remains that such compensatory changes occur. However, it is almost impossible to investigate this systematically with the currently available technology and tools/reagents.
In addition to SNP rs3822674, 5 other SNPs were also associated with cognitive performance of schizophrenic patients in the present study. The mechanisms underlying the impact of these other intronic genetic variants on cognition are still unclear, but several possibilities may be considered. (1) Because of the linkage disequilibrium between these genetic markers, the influence of the 3′UTR regulatory mechanism mediated by SNP rs3822674 may be detected by several other markers along the gene. (2) Effects of the intronic variants, largely independent of the rs3822674 3′UTR mechanism, cannot be excluded at this point, for example, on transcription factor binding sites or other regulatory elements in the gene affecting expression level or splicing of transcripts.
To summarize, we propose that neurons in brain regions that control cognitive abilities require—depending on the situation—an exact control of CPLX2 expression. Subtle disturbances in the optimal amount of CPLX2/Cplx2 levels would therefore influence cognitive functions. Our data indicate (1) that this control is, at least in part, mediated by the binding of hsa-miR-498 to the 3′UTR of the CPLX2 mRNA, and (2) that a polymorphism in the binding region of hsa-miR-498 (SNP rs3822674) influences CPLX2 gene expression and thus modulates cognitive performance. Together with other genetic and environmental mechanisms that affect cognition, this genetic variant may be an important codeterminant of cognitive outcome in schizophrenia.
Correspondence: Hannelore Ehrenreich, MD, DVM, Division of Clinical Neuroscience, Max Planck Institute of Experimental Medicine, Hermann-Rein Str 3, 37075 Göttingen, Germany (ehrenreich@em.mpg.de), or Nils Brose, PhD, Department of Molecular Neurobiology, Max Planck Institute of Experimental Medicine, Hermann-Rein Str 3, 37075 Göttingen, Germany (brose@em.mpg.de).
Submitted for Publication: October 12, 2009; final revision received January 19, 2010; accepted February 24, 2010.
Author Contributions: Dr Begemann and Ms Grube contributed equally to this work.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by the Max Planck Society, by the DFG-Research Center for Molecular Physiology of the Brain, and by several private donations. All support was devoted to the GRAS project under the leadership of Dr Ehrenreich.
Additional Contributions: We thank all the patients for their participation in this study and all the collaborating centers for their support.
This article was corrected online for error in data on 9/6/2010, prior to publication of the correction in print.
3.Purcell
SMWray
NRStone
JLVisscher
PMO’Donovan
MCSullivan
PFSklar
PInternational Schizophrenia Consortium, Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
Nature 2009;460
(7256)
748- 752
PubMedGoogle Scholar 4.Shi
JLevinson
DFDuan
JSanders
ARZheng
YPe’er
IDudbridge
FHolmans
PAWhittemore
ASMowry
BJOlincy
AAmin
FCloninger
CRSilverman
JMBuccola
NGByerley
WFBlack
DWCrowe
RROksenberg
JRMirel
DBKendler
KSFreedman
RGejman
PV Common variants on chromosome 6p22.1 are associated with schizophrenia.
Nature 2009;460
(7256)
753- 757
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Stefansson
HOphoff
RASteinberg
SAndreassen
OACichon
SRujescu
DWerge
TPietiläinen
OPMors
OMortensen
PBSigurdsson
EGustafsson
ONyegaard
MTuulio-Henriksson
AIngason
AHansen
TSuvisaari
JLonnqvist
JPaunio
TBørglum
ADHartmann
AFink-Jensen
ANordentoft
MHougaard
DNorgaard-Pedersen
BBöttcher
YOlesen
JBreuer
RMöller
HJGiegling
IRasmussen
HBTimm
SMattheisen
MBitter
IRéthelyi
JMMagnusdottir
BBSigmundsson
TOlason
PMasson
GGulcher
JRHaraldsson
MFossdal
RThorgeirsson
TEThorsteinsdottir
URuggeri
MTosato
SFranke
BStrengman
EKiemeney
LAMelle
IDjurovic
SAbramova
LKaleda
VSanjuan
Jde Frutos
RBramon
EVassos
EFraser
GEttinger
UPicchioni
MWalker
NToulopoulou
TNeed
ACGe
DYoon
JLShianna
KVFreimer
NBCantor
RMMurray
RKong
AGolimbet
VCarracedo
AArango
CCostas
JJönsson
EGTerenius
LAgartz
IPetursson
HNöthen
MMRietschel
MMatthews
PMMuglia
PPeltonen
LSt Clair
DGoldstein
DBStefansson
KCollier
DAGenetic Risk and Outcome in Psychosis (GROUP), Common variants conferring risk of schizophrenia.
Nature 2009;460
(7256)
744- 747
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Reichenberg
AHarvey
PD Neuropsychological impairments in schizophrenia: Integration of performance-based and brain imaging findings [published correction appears in
Psychol Bull. 2008;134(3):382].
Psychol Bull 2007;133
(5)
833- 858
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Mirnics
KMiddleton
FALewis
DALevitt
P Analysis of complex brain disorders with gene expression microarrays: schizophrenia as a disease of the synapse.
Trends Neurosci 2001;24
(8)
479- 486
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Harrison
PJWeinberger
DR Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence [published corrections appear in
Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(4):420, and
Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(8):804].
Mol Psychiatry 2005;10
(1)
40- 68
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Karlsgodt
KHSun
DJimenez
AMLutkenhoff
ESWillhite
Rvan Erp
TGCannon
TD Developmental disruptions in neural connectivity in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.
Dev Psychopathol 2008;20
(4)
1297- 1327
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Stephan
KEFriston
KJFrith
CD Dysconnection in schizophrenia: from abnormal synaptic plasticity to failures of self-monitoring.
Schizophr Bull 2009;35
(3)
509- 527
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12. McMahon
HTMissler
MLi
CSüdhof
TC Complexins: cytosolic proteins that regulate SNAP receptor function.
Cell 1995;83
(1)
111- 119
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Harrison
PJEastwood
SL Preferential involvement of excitatory neurons in medial temporal lobe in schizophrenia.
Lancet 1998;352
(9141)
1669- 1673
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Eastwood
SLHarrison
PJ Hippocampal synaptic pathology in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and major depression: a study of complexin mRNAs.
Mol Psychiatry 2000;5
(4)
425- 432
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Sawada
KYoung
CEBarr
AMLongworth
KTakahashi
SArango
VMann
JJDwork
AJFalkai
PPhillips
AGHoner
WG Altered immunoreactivity of complexin protein in prefrontal cortex in severe mental illness.
Mol Psychiatry 2002;7
(5)
484- 492
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Knable
MBBarci
BMWebster
MJMeador-Woodruff
JTorrey
EFStanley Neuropathology Consortium, Molecular abnormalities of the hippocampus in severe psychiatric illness: postmortem findings from the Stanley Neuropathology Consortium.
Mol Psychiatry 2004;9
(6)
609- 620
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Eastwood
SLHarrison
PJ Decreased expression of vesicular glutamate transporter 1 and complexin II mRNAs in schizophrenia: further evidence for a synaptic pathology affecting glutamate neurons.
Schizophr Res 2005;73
(2-3)
159- 172
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Reim
KWegmeyer
HBrandstätter
JHXue
MRosenmund
CDresbach
THofmann
KBrose
N Structurally and functionally unique complexins at retinal ribbon synapses.
J Cell Biol 2005;169
(4)
669- 680
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Takahashi
SUjihara
HHuang
GZYagyu
KISanbo
MKaba
HYagi
T Reduced hippocampal LTP in mice lacking a presynaptic protein: complexin II.
Eur J Neurosci 1999;11
(7)
2359- 2366
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Gibson
HEReim
KBrose
NMorton
AJJones
S A similar impairment in CA3 mossy fibre LTP in the R6/2 mouse model of Huntington's disease and in the complexin II knockout mouse.
Eur J Neurosci 2005;22
(7)
1701- 1712
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Strenzke
NChanda
SKopp-Scheinpflug
CKhimich
DReim
KBulankina
AVNeef
AWolf
FBrose
NXu-Friedman
MAMoser
T Complexin-I is required for high-fidelity transmission at the endbulb of held auditory synapse.
J Neurosci 2009;29
(25)
7991- 8004
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Lee
HJSong
JYKim
JWJin
SYHong
MSPark
JKChung
JHShibata
HFukumaki
Y Association study of polymorphisms in synaptic vesicle-associated genes,
SYN2 and
CPLX2, with schizophrenia.
Behav Brain Funct 2005;115
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Kishi
TIkeda
MSuzuki
TKitajima
TYamanouchi
YKinoshita
YOzaki
NIwata
N No association of complexin1 and complexin2 genes with schizophrenia in a Japanese population.
Schizophr Res 2006;82
(2-3)
185- 189
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Brose
N Altered complexin expression in psychiatric and neurological disorders: cause or consequence?
Mol Cells 2008;25
(1)
7- 19
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Morton
AJFaull
RLEdwardson
JM Abnormalities in the synaptic vesicle fusion machinery in Huntington's disease.
Brain Res Bull 2001;56
(2)
111- 117
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.DiProspero
NAChen
EYCharles
VPlomann
MKordower
JHTagle
DA Early changes in Huntington's disease patient brains involve alterations in cytoskeletal and synaptic elements.
J Neurocytol 2004;33
(5)
517- 533
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Tannenberg
RKScott
HLTannenberg
AEDodd
PR Selective loss of synaptic proteins in Alzheimer's disease: evidence for an increased severity with APOE ε4.
Neurochem Int 2006;49
(7)
631- 639
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Sawada
KBarr
AMNakamura
MArima
KYoung
CEDwork
AJFalkai
P Phillips
AGHoner
WG Hippocampal complexin proteins and cognitive dysfunction in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62
(3)
263- 272
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Drew
CJKyd
RJMorton
AJ Complexin 1 knockout mice exhibit marked deficits in social behaviours but appear to be cognitively normal.
Hum Mol Genet 2007;16
(19)
2288- 2305
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Glynn
DBortnick
RAMorton
AJ Complexin II is essential for normal neurological function in mice.
Hum Mol Genet 2003;12
(19)
2431- 2448
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Yamauchi
YQin
LHNishihara
MSawada
KKato
KInoue
S Vulnerability of synaptic plasticity in the complexin II knockout mouse to maternal deprivation stress.
Brain Res 2005;1056
(1)
59- 67
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Glynn
DReim
KBrose
NMorton
AJ Depletion of Complexin II does not affect disease progression in a mouse model of Huntington's disease (HD); support for role for complexin II in behavioural pathology in a mouse model of HD.
Brain Res Bull 2007;72
(2-3)
108- 120
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Oliver
PLDavies
KE Interaction between environmental and genetic factors modulates schizophrenic endophenotypes in the Snap-25 mouse mutant blind-drunk.
Hum Mol Genet 2009;18
(23)
4576- 4589
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Bicker
SSchratt
G MicroRNAs: tiny regulators of synapse function in development and disease.
J Cell Mol Med 2008;12
(5A)
1466- 1476
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Hébert
SSDe Strooper
B Alterations of the microRNA network cause neurodegenerative disease.
Trends Neurosci2009324199206
PubMedGoogle Scholar 39.Ashcroft
R The Declaration of Helsinki. In: Emanuel
E, Grady
C, Crouch
R, eds.
The Oxford Textbook of Clinical Research Ethics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2007:141-149
Google Scholar 40.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1994
41.Wittchen
H-UZaudig
MFydrich
T SCID-I: Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Disorders. Goettingen, Germany: Hogrefe; 1997
42. McGlashan
THFenton
WS Classical subtypes for schizophrenia: literature review for
DSM-IV. Schizophr Bull 1991;17
(4)
609- 632
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Jakobsson
MScholz
SWScheet
PGibbs
JRVanLiere
JMFung
HCSzpiech
ZADegnan
JHWang
KGuerreiro
RBras
JMSchymick
JCHernandez
DGTraynor
BJSimon-Sanchez
JMatarin
MBritton
Avan de Leemput
JRafferty
IBucan
MCann
HMHardy
JARosenberg
NASingleton
AB Genotype, haplotype and copy-number variation in worldwide human populations.
Nature 2008;451
(7181)
998- 1003
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Li
JZAbsher
DMTang
HSouthwick
AMCasto
AMRamachandran
SCann
HMBarsh
GSFeldman
MCavalli-Sforza
LLMyers
RM Worldwide human relationships inferred from genome-wide patterns of variation.
Science 2008;319
(5866)
1100- 1104
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Xing
JWatkins
WSWitherspoon
DJZhang
YGuthery
SLThara
RMowry
BJBulayeva
KWeiss
RBJorde
LB Fine-scaled human genetic structure revealed by SNP microarrays.
Genome Res 2009;19
(5)
815- 825
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Steffens
MLamina
CIllig
TBettecken
TVogler
REntz
PSuk
EKToliat
MRKlopp
NCaliebe
AKönig
IRKöhler
KLudemann
JDiaz Lacava
AFimmers
RLichtner
PZiegler
AWolf
AKrawczak
MNūrnberg
PHampe
JSchreiber
SMeitinger
TWichmann
HERoeder
KWienker
TFBaur
MP SNP-based analysis of genetic substructure in the German population.
Hum Hered 2006;62
(1)
20- 29
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Sirén
ALRadyushkin
KBoretius
SKämmer
DRiechers
CCNatt
OSargin
DWatanabe
TSperling
SMichaelis
TPrice
JMeyer
BFrahm
JEhrenreich
H Global brain atrophy after unilateral parietal lesion and its prevention by erythropoietin.
Brain 2006;129
(pt 2)
480- 489
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Dudbridge
F Likelihood-based association analysis for nuclear families and unrelated subjects with missing genotype data.
Hum Hered 2008;66
(2)
87- 98
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Armitage
PBerry
GMatthews
JNS Statistical Methods in Medical Research. 4th ed. Oxford, England: Blackwell; 2002
50.Cronbach
LJ Coefficient alpha and the internal structure of tests.
Psychometrika 1951;16
(3)
297- 334
Google ScholarCrossref 51.Blom
G Statistical Estimates and Transformed Beta Variables. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1958
52.SAS Institute, Inc SAS Software, Version 9.1 of the SAS System for Windows. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc; 2001
53.Purcell
SNeale
BTodd-Brown
KThomas
LFerreira
MABender
DMaller
JSklar
Pde Bakker
PIDaly
MJSham
PC PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses.
Am J Hum Genet 2007;81
(3)
559- 575
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Raevskaya
NMDergunova
LVVladychenskaya
IPStavchansky
VVOborina
MVPoltaraus
ABLimborska
SA Structural organization of the human complexin 2 gene (
CPLX2) and aspects of its functional activity.
Gene 2005;359127- 137
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Barrett
JCFry
BMaller
JDaly
MJ Haploview: analysis and visualization of LD and haplotype maps.
Bioinformatics 2005;21
(2)
263- 265
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Lehrl
STriebig
GFischer
B Multiple choice vocabulary test MWT as a valid and short test to estimate premorbid intelligence.
Acta Neurol Scand 1995;91
(5)
335- 345
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Woodberry
KAGiuliano
AJSeidman
LJ Premorbid IQ in schizophrenia: a meta-analytic review.
Am J Psychiatry 2008;165
(5)
579- 587
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Reim
KMansour
MVaroqueaux
F McMahon
HTSüdhof
TCBrose
NRosenmund
C Complexins regulate a late step in Ca
2+-dependent neurotransmitter release.
Cell 2001;104
(1)
71- 81
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Thompson
PMVidal
CGiedd
JNGochman
PBlumenthal
JNicolson
RToga
AWRapoport
JL Mapping adolescent brain change reveals dynamic wave of accelerated gray matter loss in very early-onset schizophrenia.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98
(20)
11650- 11655
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Edwardson
JMWang
CTGong
BWyttenbach
ABai
JJackson
MBChapman
ERMorton
AJ Expression of mutant huntingtin blocks exocytosis in PC12 cells by depletion of complexin II.
J Biol Chem 2003;278
(33)
30849- 30853
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Xue
MLin
YQPan
HReim
KDeng
HBellen
HJRosenmund
C Tilting the balance between facilitatory and inhibitory functions of mammalian and
Drosophila complexins orchestrates synaptic vesicle exocytosis.
Neuron 2009;64
(3)
367- 380
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Reim
KRegus-Leidig
HAmmermüller
JEl-Kordi
ARadyushkin
KEhrenreich
HBrandstätter
JHBrose
N Aberrant function and structure of retinal ribbon synapses in the absence of complexin 3 and complexin 4.
J Cell Sci 2009;122
(Pt 9)
1352- 1361
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Xue
MStradomska
AChen
HBrose
NZhang
WRosenmund
CReim
K Complexins facilitate neurotransmitter release at excitatory and inhibitory synapses in mammalian central nervous system.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2008;105
(22)
7875- 7880
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref