[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table 1. 
Persistence of Retrospectively Reported Childhood DSM-IV /ACDS ADHD Into Adulthood (n = 91)a
Persistence of Retrospectively Reported Childhood DSM-IV /ACDS ADHD Into Adulthood (n = 91)a
Table 2. 
Prevalence of DSM-IV and Other ACDS Criterion A Symptoms of Adult ADHD in Respondents With Narrowly Defined and Other Broadly Defined DSM-IV /ACDS Diagnoses of Adult ADHD Compared With Other Respondents and Results of the Rotated (Promax) Principal Axis Factor Analysisa
Prevalence of DSM-IV and Other ACDS Criterion A Symptoms of Adult ADHD in Respondents With Narrowly Defined and Other Broadly Defined DSM-IV /ACDS Diagnoses of Adult ADHD Compared With Other Respondents and Results of the Rotated (Promax) Principal Axis Factor Analysisa
Table 3. 
Mean Proportions of Adult Executive Functioning, Inattention/Hyperactivity, and Impulsivity Symptoms Reported by Respondents With Narrowly Defined or Broadly Defined DSM-IV /ACDS Adult ADHDa
Mean Proportions of Adult Executive Functioning, Inattention/Hyperactivity, and Impulsivity Symptoms Reported by Respondents With Narrowly Defined or Broadly Defined DSM-IV /ACDS Adult ADHDa
Table 4. 
Diagnostic Concordance of the Best Subset of ACDS Items With DSM-IV /ACDS Narrowly Defined and Broadly Defined Diagnoses of Adult ADHD Using Several Different Scoring Rules (n = 345)a
Diagnostic Concordance of the Best Subset of ACDS Items With DSM-IV /ACDS Narrowly Defined and Broadly Defined Diagnoses of Adult ADHD Using Several Different Scoring Rules (n = 345)a
1.
Lahey  BBApplegate  B McBurnett  KBiederman  JGreenhill  LHynd  GWBarkley  RANewcorn  JJensen  PRichters  JGarfinkel  BKerdyk  LFrisk  PJOliendick  TPerez  DHart  ELWaldman  IShaffer  D DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents.  Am J Psychiatry19941511116731685PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Spitzer  RLDavies  MBarkley  RA The DSM-III-R field trial of disruptive behavior disorders.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1990295690697PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Biederman  JFaraone  SVSpencer  TJMick  EMonuteaux  MCAleardi  M Functional impairments in adults with self-reports of diagnosed ADHD: a controlled study of 1001 adults in the community.  J Clin Psychiatry2006674524540PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
de Graaf  RKessler  RCFayyad  Jten Have  MAlonso  JAngermeyer  MBorges  GDemyttenaere  KGasquet  Ide Girolamo  GHaro  JMJin  RKaram  EGOrmel  JPosada-Villa  J The prevalence and effects of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on the performance of workers: results from the WHO World Mental Health Survey Initiative.  Occup Environ Med20086512835842PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Kessler  RCAdler  LAmes  MBarkley  RABirnbaum  HGreenberg  PJohnston  JASpencer  TUstün  TB The prevalence and effects of adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder on work performance in a nationally representative sample of workers.  J Occup Environ Med2005476565572PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Kooij  JJBuitelaar  JKvan den Oord  EJFurer  JWRijnders  CAHodiamont  PP Internal and external validity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a population-based sample of adults.  Psychol Med2005356817827PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Verster  JCBekker  EMde Roos  MMinova  AEijken  EJKooij  JJBuitelaar  JKKenemans  JLVerbaten  MNOlivier  BVolkerts  ER Methylphenidate significantly improves driving performance of adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomized crossover trial.  J Psychopharmacol2008223230237PubMedGoogle Scholar
8.
Weyandt  LLDuPaul  GJ ADHD in college students.  J Atten Disord2006101919PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Faraone  SVBiederman  JSpencer  TWilens  TSeidman  LJMick  EDoyle  AE Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: an overview.  Biol Psychiatry2000481920PubMedGoogle Scholar
10.
Mannuzza  SKlein  RGMoulton  JL  III Persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adulthood: what have we learned from the prospective follow-up studies?  J Atten Disord20037293100PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Wolraich  MLWibbelsman  CJBrown  TEEvans  SWGotlieb  EMKnight  JRRoss  ECShubiner  HHWender  EHWilens  T Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents: a review of the diagnosis, treatment, and clinical implications.  Pediatrics2005115617341746PubMedGoogle Scholar
12.
Barkley  RAMurphy  KRFischer  M ADHD in Adults: What the Science Says. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008
13.
Biederman  JFaraone  SMilberger  SCurtis  SChen  LMarrs  AOuellette  CMoore  PSpencer  T Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence: results from a four-year prospective follow-up study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1996353343351PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Biederman  JMick  EFaraone  SV Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom type.  Am J Psychiatry20001575816818PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Hart  ELLahey  BBLoeber  RApplegate  BFrick  PJ Developmental change in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: a four-year longitudinal study.  J Abnorm Child Psychol1995236729749PubMedGoogle Scholar
16.
Kessler  RCAdler  LAmes  MDemler  OFaraone  SHiripi  EHowes  MJJin  RSecnik  KSpencer  TÜstün  TBWalters  EE The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population.  Psychol Med2005352245256PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Lara  CFayyad  Jde Graaf  RKessler  RCAguilar-Gaxiola  SAngermeyer  MDemytteneare  Kde Girolamo  GHaro  JMJin  RKaram  EGLépine  JPMora  MEOrmel  JPosada-Villa  JSampson  N Childhood predictors of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results from the World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative.  Biol Psychiatry20096514654PubMedGoogle Scholar
18.
Larsson  HLichtenstein  PLarsson  J-O Genetic contributions to the development of ADHD subtypes from childhood to adolescence.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2006458973981PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Millstein  RBWilens  TEBiederman  JSpencer  TJ Presenting ADHD symptoms and subtypes in clinically referred adults with ADHD.  J Atten Disord199723159166Google Scholar
20.
Conners  CKErhardt  DEpstein  JNParker  JDASitarenios  GSparrow  E Self-ratings of ADHD symptoms in adults, I: factor structure and normative data.  J Atten Disord199933141151Google Scholar
21.
DuPaul  GJSchaughency  EAWeyandt  LLTripp  GKiesner  JOta  KStanish  H Self-report of ADHD symptoms in university students: cross-gender and cross-national prevalence.  J Learn Disabil2001344370379PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Wender  P Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Adults. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998
23.
Weyandt  LLIwaszuk  WFulton  KOllerton  MBeatty  NFouts  HSchepman  SGreenlaw  C The internal restlessness scale: performance of college students with and without ADHD.  J Learn Disabil2003364382389PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Faraone  SVBiederman  JSpencer  T Diagnostic efficiency of symptom items for identifying adult ADHD.  J ADHD Relat Disord2010123848Google Scholar
25.
Ward  MFWender  PHReimherr  FW The Wender Utah Rating Scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.  Am J Psychiatry19931506885890PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Adler  LCohen  J Diagnosis and evaluation of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Psychiatr Clin North Am2004272187201PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Kessler  RCMerikangas  KR The National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): background and aims.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res20041326068PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Kessler  RCBerglund  PChiu  WTDemler  OHeeringa  SHiripi  EJin  RPennell  BEWalters  EEZaslavsky  AZheng  H The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): design and field procedures.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res20041326992PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Kessler  RCUstün  TB The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI).  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res200413293121PubMedGoogle Scholar
30.
VanderWeele  T The use of propensity score methods in psychiatric research.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res200615295103PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Brod  MJohnston  JAble  SSwindle  R Validation of the adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder quality-of-life scale (AAQoL): a disease-specific quality-of-life measure.  Qual Life Res2006151117129PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Kessler  RCAdler  LAGruber  MJSarawate  CASpencer  TVan Brunt  DL Validity of the World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) Screener in a representative sample of health plan members.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res20071625265PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Spencer  TBiederman  JWilens  TFaraone  SPrince  JGerard  KDoyle  RParekh  AKagan  JBearman  SK Efficacy of a mixed amphetamine salts compound in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry2001588775782PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
S pencer  TBiederman  JWilens  TPrince  JHatch  MJones  JHarding  MFaraone  SVSeidman  L Effectiveness and tolerability of tomoxetine in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.  Am J Psychiatry19981555693695PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Spencer  TWilens  TBiederman  JFaraone  SVAblon  JSLapey  K A double-blind, crossover comparison of methylphenidate and placebo in adults with childhood-onset attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry1995526434443PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
Amador-Campos  JAForns-Santacana  MMartorell-Balanzó  BGuàrdia-Olmos  JPeró-Cebollero  M Confirmatory factor analysis of parents' and teachers' ratings of DSM-IV symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a Spanish sample.  Psychol Rep2005973847860PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Burns  GLBoe  BWalsh  JASommers-Flanagan  RTeegarden  LA A confirmatory factor analysis on the DSM-IV ADHD and ODD symptoms: what is the best model for the organization of these symptoms?  J Abnorm Child Psychol2001294339349PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Hudziak  JJHeath  ACMadden  PFReich  WBucholz  KKSlutske  WBierut  LJNeuman  RJTodd  RD Latent class and factor analysis of DSM-IV ADHD: a twin study of female adolescents.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1998378848857PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Rohde  LABarbosa  GPolanczyk  GEizirik  MRasmussen  ERNeuman  RJTodd  RD Factor and latent class analysis of DSM-IV ADHD symptoms in a school sample of Brazilian adolescents.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2001406711718PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Neter  JKutner  MWasserman  WNachtsheim  C Applied Linear Statistical Models. 4th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 1996
41.
Wolter  KM Introduction to Variance Estimation. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag; 1985
42.
SAS Institute Inc SAS/STAT® Software, Version 9.1 for Unix. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc; 2002
43.
Landis  JRKoch  GG The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data.  Biometrics1977331159174PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Zucker  MMorris  MKIngram  SMMorris  RDBakeman  R Concordance of self- and informant ratings of adults' current and childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms.  Psychol Assess2002144379389PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Faraone  SVBiederman  JMick  E The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies.  Psychol Med2006362159165PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Kessler  RCAdler  LABarkley  RBiederman  JConners  CKFaraone  SVGreenhill  LLJaeger  SSecnik  KSpencer  TUstün  TBZaslavsky  AM Patterns and predictors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder persistence into adulthood: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.  Biol Psychiatry2005571114421451PubMedGoogle Scholar
47.
Nigg  JTStavro  GEttenhofer  MHambrick  DZMiller  THenderson  JM Executive functions and ADHD in adults: evidence for selective effects on ADHD symptom domains.  J Abnorm Psychol20051144706717PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Barkley  RA The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspective.  Neuropsychol Rev2001111129PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Brown  TE Executive functions and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: implications of two conflicting views.  Int J Disabil Dev Educ20065313546Google Scholar
50.
Pennington  BFOzonoff  S Executive functions and developmental psychopathology.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry19963715187PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Willcutt  EGDoyle  AENigg  JTFaraone  SVPennington  BF Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review.  Biol Psychiatry2005571113361346PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Boonstra  AMOosterlaan  JSergeant  JABuitelaar  JK Executive functioning in adult ADHD: a meta-analytic review.  Psychol Med200535810971108PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2000
54.
Span  SAEarleywine  MStrybel  TZ Confirming the factor structure of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adult, nonclinical samples.  J Psychopathol Behav Assess2002242129136Google Scholar
55.
Cleland  CMagura  SFoote  JRosenblum  AKosanke  N Factor structure of the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS) for substance users.  Addict Behav200631712771282PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Barkley  RAMurphy  KR Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests.  Arch Clin Neuropsychol2010253157173PubMedGoogle Scholar
1 Comment for this article
EXPAND ALL
Adult ADHD?
Jeffrey A. Mattes, MD | Psychopharmacology Research Association of Princeton
Letter to the Editor: The Kessler et al(1) article regarding symptom criteria for adult ADHD raises several questions:
1. Should ADHD be the diagnosis for adults whose primary difficulty is executive dysfunction, not attentional problems nor hyperactivity, and who did not necessarily have clinically significant attentional difficulties nor hyperactivity as a child? Is a time for another change in terminology, e.g., “Inadequate Cortical Inhibitory Control”? (Should we return to “Minimal Brain Dysfunction”?)
2. If we are dealing with a life-long condition characterized by poor executive functioning and impulsivity, how and why would this be distinguished from personality
disorders which have overlapping symptoms.
3. Perhaps most important, since it has long been clear that response to stimulants is not a diagnostic test for ADHD,(2) and that stimulants improve certain symptoms (e.g., inattention) more than others, can one assume that stimulants will be helpful for patients diagnosed with expanded criteria? Should we recall the complications of widespread stimulant use for weight loss in 1950’s, which led to current restrictions? It is not uncommon (for me) to find patients who think they have ADHD, who think they function better on stimulants, but who are not functioning better by any objective criteria.
4. Is it possible that the drug companies selling stimulants for ADHD have, in any way, encouraged the expansion of the diagnostic criteria?
Best regards.
Sincerely,
Jeffrey A. Mattes, M.D.
References
1. Kessler RC, Green JG, Adler LA, Barkley RA, Chatterji S, Faraone SV, Finkelman M, Greenhill LL, Gruber MJ, Jewell M, Russo LJ, Sampson NA, VanBrunt DL. Structure and Diagnosis of Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(11):1168-1178.
2. Rapoport JL, Buchsbaum MS, Zohn TP, Weingartner H, Ludlow C, Mikkelson EJ. Dextroamphetamine: Cognitive and behavioral effects in normal prepubertal boys. Science. 1978;199:560-563.

Conflict of Interest: None declared
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Original Article
November 2010

Structure and Diagnosis of Adult Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder: Analysis of Expanded Symptom Criteria From the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Department of Health Care Policy, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts (Dr Kessler, Mr Gruber, and Ms Sampson); School of Education, Boston University, Boston (Dr Green); Department of Psychiatry and Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, New York University Langone School of Medicine and Psychiatry Service, New York VA Harbor Healthcare Systems, New York (Dr Adler); Department of Psychiatry, Medical University of South Carolina, Charleston (Dr Barkley); Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Barkley and Faraone) and Neuroscience and Physiology (Dr Faraone), State University of New York Upstate Medical University, Syracuse; Department of Health Statistics and Informatics, World Health Organization, Geneva, Switzerland (Dr Chatterji); Department of Research Administration, Tufts University School of Dental Medicine (Dr Finkelman); Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Columbia University–New York Psychiatric Institute, New York (Dr Greenhill); EPI-Q, Oakbrook, Illinois (Dr Jewell); Shire Pharmaceuticals Research & Development, Wayne, Pennsylvania (Dr Russo); and Health Outcomes, Lilly Research Laboratories, Indianapolis, Indiana (Dr Van Brunt).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(11):1168-1178. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.146
Abstract

Context  Controversy exists about the appropriate criteria for a diagnosis of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Objective  To examine the structure and symptoms most predictive of DSM-IV adult ADHD.

Design  The data are from clinical interviews in enriched subsamples of the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (n = 131) and a survey of a large managed health care plan (n = 214). The physician-administered Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale (ACDS) was used to assess childhood ADHD and expanded symptoms of current adult ADHD. Analyses examined the stability of symptoms from childhood to adulthood, the structure of adult ADHD, and the adult symptoms most predictive of current clinical diagnoses.

Setting  The ACDS was administered telephonically by clinical research interviewers with extensive experience in the diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD.

Participants  An enriched sample of community respondents.

Main Outcome Measure  Diagnoses of DSM-IV /ACDS adult ADHD.

Results  Almost half of the respondents (45.7%) who had childhood ADHD continued to meet the full DSM-IV criteria for current adult ADHD, with 94.9% of these patients having current attention-deficit disorder and 34.6% having current hyperactivity disorder. Adult persistence was much greater for inattention than for hyperactivity/impulsivity. Additional respondents met the full criteria for current adult ADHD despite not having met the full childhood criteria. A 3-factor structure of adult symptoms included executive functioning (EF), inattention/hyperactivity, and impulsivity. Stepwise logistic regression found EF problems to be the most consistent and discriminating predictors of adult DSM-IV /ACDS ADHD.

Conclusions  These findings document the greater persistence of inattentive than of hyperactive/impulsive childhood symptoms of ADHD in adulthood but also show that inattention is not specific to ADHD because it is strongly associated with other adult mental disorders. In comparison, EF problems are more specific and consistently important predictors of DSM-IV adult ADHD despite not being in the DSM-IV, suggesting that the number of EF symptoms should be increased in the DSM-V/ICD-11.

Although the diagnostic criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) were originally developed for children,1,2 the prevalence, consequences, and responsiveness to treatment of ADHD in adults are now well documented.3-8 We also know that the clinical profile and manifestations of ADHD evolve with age,9-11 raising questions about the stability of ADHD symptoms across time and the most appropriate diagnostic criteria for adults. Many studies12-19 have found that symptoms of hyperactivity and impulsivity (IM) decline with age, although they persist in some cases and sometimes are the presenting concerns in adult ADHD, whereas deficits in attention persist and become more varied in adult cases. These results raise the possibility that the symptoms of adult ADHD might profitably be modified in upcoming DSM-V and ICD-11 revisions.

In response to concerns that the DSM-IV criteria are inadequate to characterize adult ADHD, several proposals have been made to expand the DSM-IV and ICD-10 symptoms.20-23 With few exceptions,12,24,25 however, empirical studies have not attempted to determine the value of newly proposed symptoms. Two recent studies addressed this issue. Barkley and colleagues12 studied patients evaluated at an ADHD clinic, clinic controls, and a convenience sample of community controls. They compared the predictive validity of DSM-IV and theoretically derived non- DSM symptoms of adult ADHD in distinguishing between cases and noncases. Of the 7 discriminating items found in that study, only 1 was a DSM-IV symptom, and most of the others described deficits in executive functioning (EF). Faraone and colleagues24 compared adults with and without ADHD on the same items used by Barkley et al12 and concluded that the algorithm by Barkley et al was an efficient predictor of DSM-IV adult ADHD.

The present article describes a study designed to extend the analyses of Barkley et al12 and Faraone et al24 beyond their restricted samples by considering 2 national community samples of adults screened for adult ADHD. Enriched (for positive screens) subsamples from these 2 samples were administered the Adult ADHD Clinical Diagnostic Scale (ACDS),26 a semistructured clinical interview that incorporates a full assessment of DSM-IV ADHD and also a variety of additional questions designed to assess non- DSM symptoms found in the clinical experience of the scale developers to be typical of patients with adult ADHD. We examined the persistence of ACDS symptoms from childhood to adulthood in these samples, the structure of adult symptoms, and the symptoms most strongly predictive of DSM-IV adult ADHD. These results are not designed to prove the validity of the diagnosis of adult ADHD, which is still considered controversial in some quarters, but to ask what the best criteria are for diagnosing it under the assumption that it is a valid diagnosis.

Methods
The samples

The first sample included 131 second-stage respondents from the adult ADHD clinical reappraisal study of the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R).27 As detailed elsewhere,28 the NCS-R is a face-to-face household survey of 9282 adults in the continental United States. The World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI)29 was used to assess DSM-IV disorders in the NCS-R. The NCS-R ADHD clinical reappraisal study was conducted to validate the CIDI assessment of adult ADHD in a probability sample of NCS-R respondents aged 18 to 44 years that oversampled those positive for adult ADHD on the CIDI. A blinded clinical reappraisal interview was administered to these respondents telephonically by a team of clinical research interviewers experienced in the diagnosis and treatment of adult ADHD. A $25 incentive was offered for participation. Verbal informed consent was obtained before administering the interviews. These recruitment and consent procedures were approved by the human subjects committees of the University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, and Harvard Medical School. The 131 completed interviews were weighted to adjust for oversampling of CIDI cases. A second weight was then multiplied by the first based on a propensity score logistic regression weighting equation30 to adjust for minor discrepancies between the weighted clinical sample and the total NCS-R sample on a multivariate profile of sociodemographic variables. A more detailed discussion of the clinical study design is reported elsewhere.16

The second sample consisted of 214 third-stage respondents from a survey of adult ADHD among subscribers to a large managed health care plan. The initial survey of 20 011 subscribers (first stage) was performed for another purpose31 but included a screening scale of adult ADHD.16 A second-stage sample of 668 respondents oversampled the first-stage who screened positive 6 months later to estimate the stability of the screening scale scores. In the third stage, a subsample of second-stage respondents was administered the ACDS to validate the screening scale.32 A $25 incentive was offered for participation. Verbal informed consent was obtained before administering the interviews. These recruitment and consent procedures were approved and a Health Insurance Portability and Accountability Act waiver was granted by an independent central institutional review board (Quorum Review, Inc, Seattle, Washington). The 214 respondents in this third-stage assessment were weighted to adjust for the oversampling of screened positives by assigning a weight to each respondent such that the sum of weights in each sampling stratum divided by the sample size equaled the proportion of respondents in that sampling stratum in the original sample. A second weight was then multiplied by the first based on a propensity score logistic regression weighting equation30 that adjusted for minor discrepancies between the weighted sample and the total subscriber population on a multivariate profile of sociodemographic characteristics and information about past medical claims. A more detailed discussion of the design of this study is reported elsewhere.32 (This earlier article reported a sample size of 154 NCS-R respondents and 218 managed health care plan respondents rather than the 131 and 214, respectively, reported herein. The smaller samples were due to the age restriction of 18-44 years in the NCS-R and missing data in the managed health care sample.)

Measures

Version 1.2 of the ACDS,26 used in both clinical reappraisal surveys reported herein, has been used in a variety of clinical studies of adult ADHD.33-35 The interview begins with a retrospective assessment of all symptoms of childhood ADHD and then makes an expanded assessment of recent (past 6 months) symptoms of adult ADHD that includes all 9 DSM-IV Criterion A symptoms of inattention (AD) and 9 of hyperactivity/IM (HD) plus 14 non- DSM symptoms believed to be relevant to adult ADHD based on the clinical experience of the ACDS developers. The latter items assess difficulties with planning and organization, inattention, and mood lability. Most of these additional items are similar to symptoms proposed by Wender22 in his Utah criteria for the diagnosis of adult ADHD.

A DSM-IV /ACDS diagnosis of adult ADHD required respondents to have 6 to 9 DSM-IV symptoms of either AD or HD during childhood and during the 6 months before interview (DSM-IV Criterion A), at least 2 Criterion A symptoms before age 7 years (Criterion B), some impairment in at least 2 domains of functioning in the past 6 months linked to the ADHD symptoms (Criterion C), and clinically significant impairment in at least 1 domain of functioning in the same period linked to the ADHD symptoms (Criterion D). Impairment was linked to ADHD symptoms overall rather than to specific symptoms, which means that impairment due to a specific symptom was not required to classify a symptom as having occurred. Criterion E (that the symptoms do not occur exclusively during the course of a pervasive developmental disorder or psychotic disorder and are not better accounted for by another mental disorder) was not operationalized, and ADHD not otherwise specified was not diagnosed. None of the 14 non- DSM symptom items was used in making diagnoses. The DSM-IV requirement of impairment before age 7 years was not operationalized.

The ACDS was administered in the NCS-R clinical reappraisal study by 4 experienced PhD-level clinical interviewers who received 40 hours of training from 2 board-certified psychiatrists who specialize in research on adult ADHD. Each interviewer had to complete 5 practice interviews for which symptom ratings matched those of the trainers before beginning interviews. The ACDS was administered in the managed care sample by 6 PhD-level clinical psychologists or MA-level social workers experienced in administering the ACDS in clinical studies. Weekly calibration meetings were used to prevent drift in both studies. All the clinical interviews in both studies were tape recorded, and a random 20% were reviewed by a supervising psychiatrist. Agreement was greater than 95% of the cases checked in each of the 2 samples.

Analysis methods

Data from the 2 samples were pooled for joint analysis to increase the precision of the estimates. Post hoc within-sample analyses showed substantive findings to be consistent across samples. Cross-tabulations were used to examine the persistence of childhood ADHD into adulthood. Principal axis factor analysis was used to examine the structure of ACDS Criterion A symptoms of adult ADHD to determine whether the separation of criteria into distinct AD and HD factors typically found in youth36-39 also exists in adults. Stepwise logistic regression analysis followed by all-possible-subsets (APS) logistic regression analysis were used to determine the combination of items that best predicted DSM-IV /ACDS adult ADHD. The APS analysis is a method used to select a best subset from a larger set of predictors when the latter includes several highly intercorrelated items.40 In such situations, 2 or more different subsets sometimes have approximately equivalent overall associations with the outcome. Conventional stepwise regression analysis can select a suboptimal subset owing to minor differences in bivariate associations. The APS analysis protects against this problem by generating results for a large number of different models with a fixed number of predictors determined from an earlier stepwise analysis, each time deleting 1 or more items from the selection set so as to discover all subsets that have high and approximately comparable overall associations with the outcome. Once this full range of subsets is known, the researcher can select the 1 subset that contains the predictors most consistently in the different subsets.

Although diagnoses were based on the 18 DSM-IV symptoms, there is no logical necessity that any small number of these 18 will be better predictors than will be non- DSM items because diagnoses are nonlinear transformations of the sum of the symptom count. Non- DSM symptoms might be better indicators of this transformation (ie, 6-9 vs 0-5 of the AD or HD symptoms) than are DSM symptoms. This analysis was designed to investigate this possibility to determine whether the most highly diagnostic symptom questions include ones not currently in the DSM-IV. Because the data were weighted, the design-based Taylor series method41 implemented in a SAS macro42 was used to estimate standard errors and evaluate statistical significance.

Results
Persistence of childhood adhd

Of adults retrospectively reporting childhood ADHD (n = 91, representing a weighted 7.9% of all respondents; n = 49 in the NCS-R and n = 42 in the managed health care plan), a weighted 45.7% (n = 55, a weighted 3.6% of all respondents; n = 33 in the NCS-R and n = 22 in the managed health care plan) continued to meet the full criteria at interview. Childhood AD symptoms were much more predictive of adult persistence than were childhood HD symptoms (Table 1). Specifically, 60.8% of respondents with childhood AD only (ie, without childhood HD) met the criteria for AD as adults, whereas only 12.1% with childhood HD only (ie, without childhood AD) met the criteria for HD as adults (the difference was significant at χ21 = 6.8, P = .01). Persistence of AD does not differ from that of HD, in comparison, in respondents who had both AD and HD in childhood, with adult AD only in 6.2% of such cases and HD only in 2.3% (χ21 = 0.4, P = .44). In the 32 respondents who had the combined type as children, the adult combined type is most common (34.9%). Current AD is much more common than is current HD in all persistent cases combined, with 94.9% (SD = 10.5) having current AD and 34.6% (SD = 22.7) having current HD. In addition to the 55 respondents who met the full criteria for ADHD both in childhood and at interview, 35 others (n = 11 in the NCS-R and n = 24 in the managed health care plan) met the full criteria for ADHD at interview despite not reporting that they met the full criteria in childhood. All of these cases, however, reported 2 or more symptoms before age 7 years.

Prevalence, structure, and bivariate associations of symptoms with diagnoses

All ACDS adult symptoms were more prevalent in respondents with narrowly defined (ie, meeting the full childhood and adult criteria) DSM-IV /ACDS adult ADHD (27.2%-98.0%) and in those with other broadly defined (ie, some childhood symptoms before age 7 years and meeting the full adult criteria) adult ADHD (13.5%-97.0%) than in other respondents (0.8%-32.8%) (Table 2). Twenty-four of 32 bivariate odds ratios (ORs) between individual symptoms and narrowly defined adult ADHD were statistically significant compared with respondents who met neither narrow nor broad criteria (OR, 6.6-694.6), and 28 bivariate ORs were significant comparing broadly defined (ie, narrowly or other broadly defined) cases with other respondents (OR, 5.1-186.7).

Principal axis factor analysis found 5 unrotated factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 (17.4, 2.9, 2.4, 1.6, and 1.3). Promax rotation showed that the last 2 factors were unique (ie, included a high factor loading on only 1 item), leading us to focus on the 3-factor solution. Replication of the factor analysis in the 2 subsamples showed good stability of results. The items in the first factor, which we refer to as EF, represent difficulties with planning and organizational skills considered hallmarks of EF. These include 3 DSM symptoms of AD (“makes careless mistakes,” “difficulty organizing tasks,” and “loses things”) plus 6 non- DSM symptoms involving difficulties in planning, prioritizing, multitasking, remembering details, meeting deadlines, and maintaining self-discipline. The items in the second factor, which we refer to as inattention-hyperactivity (IH), include the remaining DSM inattention symptoms plus 5 of 9 DSM hyperactivity symptoms and 3 non- DSM symptoms (“bores easily,” “others keep life in order,” and “cannot work unless under a deadline”). The items in the third factor, which we refer to as IM, include all DSM IM symptoms in addition to the remaining DSM hyperactivity symptoms and 2 non- DSM symptoms (“mood changes frequently” and “sensitive to criticism”). Pearson correlations between factors are 0.51 for EF-IH, 0.38 for EF-IM, and 0.39 for IH-IM. Narrowly defined cases have a different symptom profile than do other broadly defined cases (Table 3). Specifically, narrowly defined cases have significantly higher proportions of EF (77.6% vs 67.8%, t = 5.1, P < .001) and IH (76.3% vs 61.5%, t = 7.5, P < .001) symptoms and a significantly lower proportion of IM symptoms (46.3% vs 61.4%, t = 4.0, P = .001) than do other broadly defined cases.

Aps logistic regression analysis

Stepwise logistic regression was used to predict DSM-IV /ACDS adult ADHD from ACDS symptoms. Four symptoms captured all the significant predictive effects. The APS regression analysis selected the 10 four-symptom subsets with the highest predictive associations. Three EF items and 1 IH item emerged in this analysis as most consistently predictive of broadly defined ADHD, and 2 EF and 2 IH items emerged as the most consistently predictive of narrowly defined ADHD. No IM items emerged as consistently predictive. One EF item was in the significant predictive set of both narrowly and broadly defined ADHD: “difficulty prioritizing work” (10 of 10 in narrowly defined ADHD and 8 of 10 in broadly defined ADHD). The other important EF predictor of narrowly defined ADHD was “trouble planning ahead” (3 of 10). The other 2 important EF predictors of broadly defined ADHD were “cannot complete tasks on time” (10 of 10) and “makes careless mistakes” (7 of 10). Only the last of these 4 EF items is in the DSM-IV. One IH item was predictive of both narrowly and broadly defined ADHD: “difficulty sustaining attention” (7 of 10 in narrowly defined ADHD and 10 of 10 in broadly defined ADHD). The other item, “cannot work unless under a deadline,” was important only in narrowly defined ADHD (8 of 10). Only the first of these 2 IH items is in the DSM-IV.

Dichotomous prediction of clinical diagnoses

We tested a series of dichotomous scoring rules to predict clinical diagnoses from the predictors described in the previous paragraph. The best rule was to require 3 or 4 of 4 items to predict narrowly defined ADHD and 2 to 4 of 4 items to predict broadly defined ADHD (Table 4). The prevalence estimates based on these scoring rules are not significantly different from the ACDS estimates (narrowly defined ADHD: χ21 = 1.2, P = .27; broadly defined ADHD: χ21 = 2.6, P = .11). Individual-level concordance with clinical diagnoses was also very good (narrowly defined ADHD: κ = 0.79, area under the receiver operating characteristic curve [AUC] = 0.93; broadly defined ADHD: κ = 0.89, AUC = 0.98).43 Most ACDS cases (88.1% narrowly defined ADHD and 96.7% broadly defined ADHD) were detected using these rules, and most ACDS noncases (98.7% narrowly defined ADHD; 98.5% broadly defined ADHD) were correctly classified as noncases.

Because we wanted to find symptoms specific to adult ADHD, we examined whether the 4 best-predicting symptoms also significantly predicted other DSM-IV /CIDI diagnoses in the NCS-R (the only sample in which these other disorders were assessed) after controlling for total ACDS scores. This was performed in a series of prediction equations, each of which included the total ACDS score plus 1 other ACDS symptom to predict other DSM-IV disorders. If any especially strong association between individual ACDS symptoms and other disorders existed beyond the general comorbidity with the total ACDS scores, a question might be raised about item confounding. Logistic regression analysis was used to perform this analysis by predicting the 6-month prevalence of any DSM-IV /CIDI mood disorder, anxiety disorder, substance use disorder, and behavioral disorder (other than ADHD) from each ACDS item in the 4-item scales controlling for total ACDS scores. Total ACDS scores were significant predictors in every one of these equations, documenting that adult ADHD is significantly comorbid with a wide range of other DSM-IV disorders. However, none of the EF symptoms predicted any of these outcomes significantly once total ACDS scores were controlled for. Both AD items, in comparison, were significant in 1 of these equations: “difficulty sustaining attention” predicting anxiety disorders (OR = 11.6, 95% confidence interval = 2.2-60.4) and “cannot work unless under a deadline” predicting behavioral disorders (13.9, 2.3-83.9).

Based on these results, we explored the possibility of deleting the AD items in the prediction scales and focusing only on the EF items (Table 4). The best scoring rule in these reduced sets was to require both EF items to predict narrowly defined ADHD and 2 to 3 items to predict broadly defined ADHD. These rules generated weighted prevalence estimates similar to the ACDS estimates (narrowly defined ADHD: χ21 = 0.3, P = .58; broadly defined ADHD: χ21 = 0.0, P = .98) and good individual-level concordance with ACDS diagnoses (narrowly defined ADHD: κ = 0.70, AUC = 0.83; broadly defined ADHD: κ = 0.87, AUC = 0.93). Most ACDS cases (66.9% narrowly defined ADHD and 87.0% broadly defined ADHD) were detected using these rules, and most ACDS noncases (99.2% narrowly defined ADHD and 99.0% broadly defined ADHD) were correctly classified as noncases.

Comment

This study has several limitations. First, logistical-financial considerations forced us to base clinical interviews on telephone administration, which could have reduced the validity of clinical assessments. Second, diagnoses were based on self-report even though collateral reports from spouses and others can add important information about adult ADHD.44 Third, as in most studies of adult ADHD, childhood symptoms were reported retrospectively. These retrospective reports may have been affected by recall bias and the presence or absence of current symptoms.

Another limitation relates to the use of stepwise regression methods to select the most highly predictive symptoms. Stepwise methods can capitalize on chance. Although we used APS analysis to address this problem, caution should, nonetheless, be used in interpreting results before cross-validation. A related limitation is that most non- DSM items in the ACDS assessed EF problems. Impulsivity, in comparison, was assessed using a much smaller set of symptoms (only 2 non- DSM items and the 3 DSM-IV items). The role of IM, consequently, could have been underestimated in this analysis. Consistent with this possibility, the non- DSM ACDS symptoms do not include 3 IM symptoms found by Barkley et al12 to be predictive of adult ADHD (“makes decisions impulsively,” “difficulty stopping activities or behavior when he or she should do so,” and “more likely to drive a motor vehicle much faster than others”). A final limitation is that interpretation depends on the thresholds established for determining the presence or absence of symptoms, which were not specified in enough detail in the DSM system to provide firm guidance for the ACDS assessments.

In the context of these limitations, the estimate that 3.6% of respondents meet the DSM-IV criteria for both childhood and adult ADHD and the finding that these cases represent nearly half of all adults who retrospectively reported childhood ADHD are generally consistent with previous studies.14,45,46 The present results are also consistent with previous studies in finding that symptom profiles change with age, as childhood AD is much more persistent than is childhood HD.14,15,17,18 We also found that the prevalence of adult ADHD increased substantially when we did not require full criteria for ADHD in childhood and that broadly defined adult ADHD had more adult IM and less EF and IH problems than did narrowly defined adult ADHD. Additional research, ideally in longitudinal samples, is needed to investigate the stability of these specifications. Another topic for future research concerns subthreshold manifestations. We did not explore subthreshold adult symptoms but required either 6 AD or 6 HD symptoms in adulthood even though the DSM-V ADHD and Disruptive Behavior Disorders Work Group is considering the possibility of requiring as few as 3 symptoms for a diagnosis of adult ADHD.

The finding of a distinct adult EF symptom factor is consistent with several other studies12,20,47 finding EF problems to be cardinal features of adult ADHD. The fact that 3 DSM-IV AD items loaded on the EF factor (“difficulty organizing tasks,” “makes careless mistakes,” and “loses things”) is consistent with the suggestion that some inattention may be a manifestation of deficits in working memory, suggesting an underlying effect of difficulty in EF.12 It is important to note in this regard, however, that the term EF is defined in a variety of ways in the literature48-50 and is used herein in a relatively nontechnical way to refer to observable deficits in the performance of self-regulatory functions in daily life, such as the ability to organize, prioritize, and integrate cognitive functions. This focus on daily functions might not have good correspondence with EF as measured in cognitive performance tests.51 Ongoing research using such tests might document more subtle distinctions in EF problems that relate to different manifestations of adult ADHD.47,52

The finding that symptoms of AD and HD load together on a second factor is inconsistent with AD and HD being conceptualized as distinct in the DSM-IV. This finding is also indirectly inconsistent with the finding of separate AD and IH factors in studies of childhood ADHD.36-39 However, the finding of a single adult IH factor is consistent with the finding of a similar factor in another study of adult ADHD using the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scale.20 This replication supports the view of some experts that whereas HD in childhood is primarily motoric, HD in adulthood is more reflective of internal restlessness.23 The DSM-IV acknowledges this by noting that symptoms of HD in adolescence and adulthood “take the form of feelings of restlessness and difficulty engaging in quiet sedentary activities.”53(p79) In this regard, conceptual models of internal restlessness frequently incorporate traditional symptoms of AD (ie, mind wanders and distracted by sounds and visual stimuli).23 Furthermore, even in factor analytic studies that find that symptoms of AD and HD load on separate factors, these factors are often highly correlated.54 The finding that IM symptoms split off from those of HD is also consistent with several previous studies12,20,54,55 and is especially striking because only a few IM items were included in the ACDS.

The factor analysis results suggest that the higher relative prevalence of AD only than of HD only in adulthood than in childhood is due not merely to age-related changes in symptom expression but also to age-related changes in symptom structure. This finding of a pathoplastic effect of age regarding symptoms of ADHD illustrates the fact that criteria sets sometimes need to be different for segments of the population defined on the basis of sociodemographic characteristics. In the case of adult ADHD, symptoms associated with deficits in EF seem to be key symptoms of this sort that emerge as more important in adulthood than in childhood.

An important finding is that EF problems are consistently important predictors of adult clinical diagnoses of ADHD in respondents who met the full criteria for childhood ADHD and in those who had only some childhood symptoms before age 7 years. Unlike the other highly predictive adult symptoms, all of which involve AD, none of the adult EF symptoms had significant comorbidity with other classes of adult DSM-IV disorders after controlling for the general gradient of adult ADHD. This suggests that EF symptoms are those most specifically differentiating adult ADHD from other adult DSM disorders. A corollary is that although AD is the aspect of childhood ADHD most likely to persist into adulthood, it would be a mistake to think of AD as the most important discriminating feature of adult ADHD owing to the strong associations of AD with other adult mental disorders.

The most highly predictive EF symptoms in this analysis are not in the DSM-IV. Indeed, only 1 of the 4 most predictive symptoms of narrowly defined adult ADHD was a DSM-IV symptom, and 2 of the remaining 3 items were EF problems. Three of the 4 most predictive symptoms of broadly defined adult ADHD were EF problems. These findings are broadly consistent with those of Barkley et al12 and Faraone et al,24 who found that a variety of non- DSM symptoms of EF problems performed better than did DSM-IV symptoms in distinguishing patients with adult ADHD from clinical controls. Although some of the most predictive non– DSM-IV items in these analyses load on the IH factor (“cannot work unless under a deadline” and “difficulty sustaining attention”), these symptoms also reflect deficits in initiating and sustaining work effort, which are typically considered self-regulatory components of EF.49

These results are consistent with the suggestion that diagnostic criteria for adult ADHD in future DSM and ICD revisions should include more EF items, augmenting evidence that EF problems are evident in virtually all adults with ADHD.56 Although these findings might be taken to support the view that adult ADHD should be conceptualized as largely a disorder of problems in EF,48,49 such a view overinterprets the data because AD is strongly persistent from ADHD in childhood to adulthood and because Barkley and Faraone and their coworkers also found that some aspects of IM predict adult ADHD. Nonetheless, the present results highlight the importance of EF. More work is needed to determine whether an expanded version of the most predictive items in the present analysis could be used as a brief screening scale for adult ADHD. Although these items have strong face validity in tapping core symptoms of EF problems, they were applied herein to a relatively small sample. The importance of these items consequently needs to be cross-validated in other samples to determine whether they would perform consistently as well as in the present study in predicting clinical diagnoses of adult ADHD.

Back to top
Article Information

Correspondence: Ronald C. Kessler, PhD, Department of Health Care Policy, Harvard Medical School, 180 Longwood Ave, Boston, MA 02115 (kessler@hcp.med.harvard.edu).

Submitted for Publication: December 17, 2009; final revision received May 7, 2010; accepted May 23, 2010.

Author Contributions: Dr Kessler had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Financial Disclosure: Dr Kessler has been a consultant for AstraZeneca, Analysis Group Inc, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cerner-Galt Associates, Eli Lilly & Co, GlaxoSmithKline Inc, HealthCore Inc, Health Dialog, Integrated Benefits Institute, John Snow Inc, Kaiser Permanente, Matria Inc, Mensante, Merck & Co Inc, Ortho-McNeil Janssen Scientific Affairs, Pfizer Inc, Primary Care Network, Research Triangle Institute, Sanofi-Aventis Groupe, Shire US Inc, SRA International Inc, Takeda Global Research & Development, Transcept Pharmaceuticals Inc, and Wyeth-Ayerst; has served on advisory boards for Appliance Computing II, Eli Lilly & Co, Mindsite, Ortho-McNeil Janssen Scientific Affairs, and Wyeth-Ayerst; and has had research support for his epidemiologic studies from Analysis Group Inc, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly & Co, EPI-Q, GlaxoSmithKline, Johnson & Johnson Pharmaceuticals, Ortho-McNeil Janssen Scientific Affairs, Pfizer Inc, Sanofi-Aventis Groupe, and Shire US Inc. Dr Adler has received grant/research support from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Pfizer, Shire US Inc, Eli Lilly & Co, Ortho-McNeil, Jannsen, Johnson & Johnson, the National Institute of Drug Abuse, and the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH); has served on speakers' bureaus for Ortho-McNeil, Jannsen, Johnson & Johnson, and Shire US Inc but no longer participates in speakers' bureaus; has served on advisory boards and has consulted for Eli Lilly & Co, Major League Baseball, Mindsite, Organon, Ortho McNeil, Jannsen, Johnson & Johnson, and Shire US Inc; and has also served as a consultant for i3 Research and EPI-Q. In the previous year he has received grant/research support, consulted with, or served on advisory boards or speakers' bureaus for Abbott Laboratories, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Cortex Pharmaceuticals, Eli Lilly & Co, Major League Baseball, Merck & Co, Mindsite, National Institute of Drug Abuse, New River Pharmaceuticals, Organon, Ortho-McNeil, Jannsen, Johnson & Johnson, Pfizer, Psychogenics, Sanofi-Aventis Pharmaceuticals, and Shire US Inc. He has received royalty payments (as inventor) from New York University for license of adult ADHD scales and training material since 2004. Dr Barkley has served as a consultant/speaker to Eli Lilly & Co, Shire US Inc, Medice, Novartis, Janssen-Ortho, and Janssen-Cilag. In the past year, Dr Faraone has received consulting fees from and has been on advisory boards for Eli Lilly & Co, McNeil, and Shire US Inc and has received research support from Eli Lilly & Co, Pfizer, Shire US Inc, and the National Institutes of Health. In previous years, Dr Faraone has received consulting fees from, has been on advisory boards for, or has been a speaker for Shire US Inc, McNeil, Janssen, Novartis, Pfizer, and Eli Lilly & Co. In previous years, he has received research support from Eli Lilly & Co, Shire US Inc, Pfizer, and the National Institutes of Health. Dr Greenhill has served as chairman of the Data Safety Monitoring Board for the Ziprasidone Pediatric Clinical Trials for Pfizer; has received research support from J & J Pharmaceuticals, Forest Pharmaceuticals, and the NIMH; and has received travel support and honoraria from the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. He currently serves as president of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. Dr Jewell has served as a consultant in his capacity at EPI-Q to Abbott Laboratories, AstraZeneca, Bayer Pharmaceuticals, Bristol-Meyers Squibb, Cephalon, Chiron, Genentech, Gilead, Gold Standard, Intermune, IVAX, Merck Pharmaceutical, Novartis Pharmaceuticals, Ortho-McNeil Pharmaceuticals, Pfizer, Roche, Sanofi-Aventis, Shire US Inc, and Takeda. Dr Russo is a full-time employee of Shire Pharmaceuticals Research & Development. Dr Van Brunt is a full-time employee of Lilly Research Laboratories, Eli Lilly & Co. He has received stock and stock options as part of his compensation.

Funding/Support: Data collection for the NCS-R is supported by the NIMH (U01-MH60220) with supplemental support from the National Institute of Drug Abuse, the Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration, and the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation (grant 044780). Data collection for the NCS-R clinical reappraisal study and for the survey and clinical reappraisal study of the health benefits company sample was funded by Eli Lilly & Co. This report was prepared under the auspices of the World Health Organization ICD-11 Chapter 5 (Mental and Behavioural Disorders) Epidemiology Working Group with the support of an unrestricted educational grant from Shire Pharmaceuticals.

Role of the Sponsor: Funding for this secondary analysis was obtained based on an investigator-initiated proposal. The funder of the managed health plan survey (Eli Lilly & Co) gave permission to use the data from that survey for the purposes of these analyses. The funder did not specify the design, conduct of the data analysis, or interpretation of the results. The only involvement of Eli Lilly & Co and Shire Pharmaceuticals in the design and conduct of the data analysis, interpretation of results, or preparation of the manuscript was through the participation of Dr Van Brunt (Eli Lilly & Co) and Dr Russo (Shire) as collaborators. They and all other collaborators participated in a series of telephone meetings to plan analyses, review results as they emerged, and discuss interpretations of results.

Additional Information: All the NCS-R data are publically available for secondary analysis. Instructions on how to download the public-use data files can be found at http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/ncs. The managed care sample database can be obtained for secondary analysis by contacting Nancy Sampson at sampson@hcp.med.harvard.edu.

Additional Contributions: We acknowledge the primary investigators for the National Comorbidity Survey Replication on which this article is based. They are Ronald C. Kessler, PhD, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts; Kathleen Merikangas, PhD, co-principal investigator, NIMH, Bethesda, Maryland; Doreen Koretz, MD, co-principal investigator, Harvard University; William Eaton, PhD, The Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore, Maryland; Jane McLeod, PhD, Indiana University, Bloomington; Mark Olfson, MD, MPH, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, New York; Harold Pincus, MD, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pennsylvania; Philip Wang, MD, DrPH, Harvard Medical School; Kenneth Wells, MD, MPH, University of California, Los Angeles; and Elaine Wethington, PhD, Cornell University, Ithaca, New York.

References
1.
Lahey  BBApplegate  B McBurnett  KBiederman  JGreenhill  LHynd  GWBarkley  RANewcorn  JJensen  PRichters  JGarfinkel  BKerdyk  LFrisk  PJOliendick  TPerez  DHart  ELWaldman  IShaffer  D DSM-IV field trials for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents.  Am J Psychiatry19941511116731685PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Spitzer  RLDavies  MBarkley  RA The DSM-III-R field trial of disruptive behavior disorders.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1990295690697PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Biederman  JFaraone  SVSpencer  TJMick  EMonuteaux  MCAleardi  M Functional impairments in adults with self-reports of diagnosed ADHD: a controlled study of 1001 adults in the community.  J Clin Psychiatry2006674524540PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
de Graaf  RKessler  RCFayyad  Jten Have  MAlonso  JAngermeyer  MBorges  GDemyttenaere  KGasquet  Ide Girolamo  GHaro  JMJin  RKaram  EGOrmel  JPosada-Villa  J The prevalence and effects of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on the performance of workers: results from the WHO World Mental Health Survey Initiative.  Occup Environ Med20086512835842PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Kessler  RCAdler  LAmes  MBarkley  RABirnbaum  HGreenberg  PJohnston  JASpencer  TUstün  TB The prevalence and effects of adult attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder on work performance in a nationally representative sample of workers.  J Occup Environ Med2005476565572PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Kooij  JJBuitelaar  JKvan den Oord  EJFurer  JWRijnders  CAHodiamont  PP Internal and external validity of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in a population-based sample of adults.  Psychol Med2005356817827PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Verster  JCBekker  EMde Roos  MMinova  AEijken  EJKooij  JJBuitelaar  JKKenemans  JLVerbaten  MNOlivier  BVolkerts  ER Methylphenidate significantly improves driving performance of adults with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomized crossover trial.  J Psychopharmacol2008223230237PubMedGoogle Scholar
8.
Weyandt  LLDuPaul  GJ ADHD in college students.  J Atten Disord2006101919PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Faraone  SVBiederman  JSpencer  TWilens  TSeidman  LJMick  EDoyle  AE Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adults: an overview.  Biol Psychiatry2000481920PubMedGoogle Scholar
10.
Mannuzza  SKlein  RGMoulton  JL  III Persistence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder into adulthood: what have we learned from the prospective follow-up studies?  J Atten Disord20037293100PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Wolraich  MLWibbelsman  CJBrown  TEEvans  SWGotlieb  EMKnight  JRRoss  ECShubiner  HHWender  EHWilens  T Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder among adolescents: a review of the diagnosis, treatment, and clinical implications.  Pediatrics2005115617341746PubMedGoogle Scholar
12.
Barkley  RAMurphy  KRFischer  M ADHD in Adults: What the Science Says. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2008
13.
Biederman  JFaraone  SMilberger  SCurtis  SChen  LMarrs  AOuellette  CMoore  PSpencer  T Predictors of persistence and remission of ADHD into adolescence: results from a four-year prospective follow-up study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1996353343351PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Biederman  JMick  EFaraone  SV Age-dependent decline of symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: impact of remission definition and symptom type.  Am J Psychiatry20001575816818PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Hart  ELLahey  BBLoeber  RApplegate  BFrick  PJ Developmental change in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in boys: a four-year longitudinal study.  J Abnorm Child Psychol1995236729749PubMedGoogle Scholar
16.
Kessler  RCAdler  LAmes  MDemler  OFaraone  SHiripi  EHowes  MJJin  RSecnik  KSpencer  TÜstün  TBWalters  EE The World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population.  Psychol Med2005352245256PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Lara  CFayyad  Jde Graaf  RKessler  RCAguilar-Gaxiola  SAngermeyer  MDemytteneare  Kde Girolamo  GHaro  JMJin  RKaram  EGLépine  JPMora  MEOrmel  JPosada-Villa  JSampson  N Childhood predictors of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: results from the World Health Organization World Mental Health Survey Initiative.  Biol Psychiatry20096514654PubMedGoogle Scholar
18.
Larsson  HLichtenstein  PLarsson  J-O Genetic contributions to the development of ADHD subtypes from childhood to adolescence.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2006458973981PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Millstein  RBWilens  TEBiederman  JSpencer  TJ Presenting ADHD symptoms and subtypes in clinically referred adults with ADHD.  J Atten Disord199723159166Google Scholar
20.
Conners  CKErhardt  DEpstein  JNParker  JDASitarenios  GSparrow  E Self-ratings of ADHD symptoms in adults, I: factor structure and normative data.  J Atten Disord199933141151Google Scholar
21.
DuPaul  GJSchaughency  EAWeyandt  LLTripp  GKiesner  JOta  KStanish  H Self-report of ADHD symptoms in university students: cross-gender and cross-national prevalence.  J Learn Disabil2001344370379PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Wender  P Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder in Adults. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1998
23.
Weyandt  LLIwaszuk  WFulton  KOllerton  MBeatty  NFouts  HSchepman  SGreenlaw  C The internal restlessness scale: performance of college students with and without ADHD.  J Learn Disabil2003364382389PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Faraone  SVBiederman  JSpencer  T Diagnostic efficiency of symptom items for identifying adult ADHD.  J ADHD Relat Disord2010123848Google Scholar
25.
Ward  MFWender  PHReimherr  FW The Wender Utah Rating Scale: an aid in the retrospective diagnosis of childhood attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.  Am J Psychiatry19931506885890PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Adler  LCohen  J Diagnosis and evaluation of adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Psychiatr Clin North Am2004272187201PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Kessler  RCMerikangas  KR The National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): background and aims.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res20041326068PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Kessler  RCBerglund  PChiu  WTDemler  OHeeringa  SHiripi  EJin  RPennell  BEWalters  EEZaslavsky  AZheng  H The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): design and field procedures.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res20041326992PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Kessler  RCUstün  TB The World Mental Health (WMH) Survey Initiative Version of the World Health Organization (WHO) Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI).  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res200413293121PubMedGoogle Scholar
30.
VanderWeele  T The use of propensity score methods in psychiatric research.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res200615295103PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Brod  MJohnston  JAble  SSwindle  R Validation of the adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder quality-of-life scale (AAQoL): a disease-specific quality-of-life measure.  Qual Life Res2006151117129PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Kessler  RCAdler  LAGruber  MJSarawate  CASpencer  TVan Brunt  DL Validity of the World Health Organization Adult ADHD Self-Report Scale (ASRS) Screener in a representative sample of health plan members.  Int J Methods Psychiatr Res20071625265PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Spencer  TBiederman  JWilens  TFaraone  SPrince  JGerard  KDoyle  RParekh  AKagan  JBearman  SK Efficacy of a mixed amphetamine salts compound in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry2001588775782PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
S pencer  TBiederman  JWilens  TPrince  JHatch  MJones  JHarding  MFaraone  SVSeidman  L Effectiveness and tolerability of tomoxetine in adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.  Am J Psychiatry19981555693695PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Spencer  TWilens  TBiederman  JFaraone  SVAblon  JSLapey  K A double-blind, crossover comparison of methylphenidate and placebo in adults with childhood-onset attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.  Arch Gen Psychiatry1995526434443PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
Amador-Campos  JAForns-Santacana  MMartorell-Balanzó  BGuàrdia-Olmos  JPeró-Cebollero  M Confirmatory factor analysis of parents' and teachers' ratings of DSM-IV symptoms of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in a Spanish sample.  Psychol Rep2005973847860PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Burns  GLBoe  BWalsh  JASommers-Flanagan  RTeegarden  LA A confirmatory factor analysis on the DSM-IV ADHD and ODD symptoms: what is the best model for the organization of these symptoms?  J Abnorm Child Psychol2001294339349PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Hudziak  JJHeath  ACMadden  PFReich  WBucholz  KKSlutske  WBierut  LJNeuman  RJTodd  RD Latent class and factor analysis of DSM-IV ADHD: a twin study of female adolescents.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry1998378848857PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Rohde  LABarbosa  GPolanczyk  GEizirik  MRasmussen  ERNeuman  RJTodd  RD Factor and latent class analysis of DSM-IV ADHD symptoms in a school sample of Brazilian adolescents.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry2001406711718PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Neter  JKutner  MWasserman  WNachtsheim  C Applied Linear Statistical Models. 4th ed. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill; 1996
41.
Wolter  KM Introduction to Variance Estimation. New York, NY: Springer-Verlag; 1985
42.
SAS Institute Inc SAS/STAT® Software, Version 9.1 for Unix. Cary, NC: SAS Institute Inc; 2002
43.
Landis  JRKoch  GG The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data.  Biometrics1977331159174PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Zucker  MMorris  MKIngram  SMMorris  RDBakeman  R Concordance of self- and informant ratings of adults' current and childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms.  Psychol Assess2002144379389PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Faraone  SVBiederman  JMick  E The age-dependent decline of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis of follow-up studies.  Psychol Med2006362159165PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Kessler  RCAdler  LABarkley  RBiederman  JConners  CKFaraone  SVGreenhill  LLJaeger  SSecnik  KSpencer  TUstün  TBZaslavsky  AM Patterns and predictors of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder persistence into adulthood: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.  Biol Psychiatry2005571114421451PubMedGoogle Scholar
47.
Nigg  JTStavro  GEttenhofer  MHambrick  DZMiller  THenderson  JM Executive functions and ADHD in adults: evidence for selective effects on ADHD symptom domains.  J Abnorm Psychol20051144706717PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Barkley  RA The executive functions and self-regulation: an evolutionary neuropsychological perspective.  Neuropsychol Rev2001111129PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Brown  TE Executive functions and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: implications of two conflicting views.  Int J Disabil Dev Educ20065313546Google Scholar
50.
Pennington  BFOzonoff  S Executive functions and developmental psychopathology.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry19963715187PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Willcutt  EGDoyle  AENigg  JTFaraone  SVPennington  BF Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review.  Biol Psychiatry2005571113361346PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Boonstra  AMOosterlaan  JSergeant  JABuitelaar  JK Executive functioning in adult ADHD: a meta-analytic review.  Psychol Med200535810971108PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2000
54.
Span  SAEarleywine  MStrybel  TZ Confirming the factor structure of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in adult, nonclinical samples.  J Psychopathol Behav Assess2002242129136Google Scholar
55.
Cleland  CMagura  SFoote  JRosenblum  AKosanke  N Factor structure of the Conners Adult ADHD Rating Scale (CAARS) for substance users.  Addict Behav200631712771282PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Barkley  RAMurphy  KR Impairment in occupational functioning and adult ADHD: the predictive utility of executive function (EF) ratings versus EF tests.  Arch Clin Neuropsychol2010253157173PubMedGoogle Scholar
×