Context
As genes associated with common disorders are increasingly identified, we need to progress from observing associations to identifying risk pathways. The high-activity COMT genotype, in the presence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), has previously been shown to be associated with extreme antisocial behavior. The same genotype has also been implicated in affecting cognitive function in healthy individuals. Impaired cognitive function might therefore lie on the risk pathway from genotype to clinical outcome.
Objectives
To replicate the association between COMT genotype and antisocial behavior in ADHD and to then test whether (1) impaired executive control or (2) impaired social understanding act as intermediate phenotypes for this association and lie on the risk pathway between COMT genotype and antisocial behavior.
Design
Prospective epidemiological cohort sample.
Setting
The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.
Participants
Four thousand three hundred sixty-five children with data on COMT Val158Met genotype, ADHD symptoms and diagnoses, and measures of social cognition/understanding and executive control.
Main Outcome Measures
Antisocial behavior at age 7.5 years assessed using DSM-IV conduct disorder symptoms.
Results
We replicated the association of the high-activity COMT genotype, in the presence of ADHD, with extreme antisocial behavior (odds ratio, 2.82; 95% confidence interval, 2.02-3.94; P < .001 for the most severe antisocial behavior). The high-activity COMT genotype was associated with both executive control and impaired social understanding. The strength of the association between genotype and antisocial behavior was unchanged by including executive control in the model but dropped when impaired social understanding was included (odds ratio, 1.87; 95% confidence interval, 1.26-2.76; P = .002).
Conclusions
The high-activity COMT genotype in ADHD is associated with antisocial behavior in part via impaired social understanding. Impaired executive control was also associated with the high-activity COMT genotype but may not lie on the risk pathway to antisocial behavior. The findings demonstrate the importance of testing links between genotype, intermediate phenotype, and clinical outcome in the same sample to identify potential risk pathways.
Severe antisocial behavior is of tremendous societal and clinical concern.1 The most severe and persistent subtype of antisocial behavior has its onset in childhood and is accompanied by attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),2 a disorder characterized by impulsivity, hyperactivity, and inattention. This subtype of antisocial behavior has strong biological and genetic origins3 and is a critical path into adult criminality and violence.1,2,4 Three independent studies5,6 report that this extreme antisocial behavior subtype is associated with the high-activity COMT (catechol O -methyltransferase) valine/valine (Val/Val) genotype. Having identified a robust genetic risk factor, a key step is to elucidate the specific brain mechanisms and pathways that underlie the genetic association.
It has been proposed that a powerful way of identifying mechanisms through which specific genetic risk factors influence disorder is to identify intermediate phenotypes,7,8 which are internal phenotypes that are assumed to lie on the pathway between genes and disease. A problem with this approach is that the observation that a risk genotype is associated with both a putative intermediate phenotype and a clinical outcome does not prove a causal relationship between the 2 phenotypes.9 Statistical approaches to this problem have been developed,10 but to date, studies of psychiatric and behavioral phenotypes have not been appropriately designed or sufficiently powered to apply them. Thus, the burgeoning literature reporting intermediate brain phenotypes such as performance on cognitive tasks that are associated with candidate genes11-13 might simply be identifying phenotypes that share genetic risk with psychiatric outcomes (pleiotropy) rather than illuminating disease mechanisms. Large longitudinal cohort studies in which multiple behavioral and cognitive measures have been made offer a potentially powerful means to address these issues because each link in the hypothesized risk pathway can be tested in turn in the same sample. The present study provides a proof of principle of this approach.
On the basis of previous findings, we hypothesized 2 putative intermediate phenotypes for a risk pathway between the high-activity COMT Val/Val genotype in the presence of ADHD and antisocial behavior: impaired executive control and social cognitive dysfunction. These are plausible intermediate phenotypes because both executive control (eg, planning, suppressing inappropriate responses) and social cognition (processing social information) are stable, inherited characteristics that are precursors or risk factors for subsequent severe antisocial behavior (eg, fighting, stealing).14,15
Both these intermediate phenotypes have also been linked to the COMT Val158Met variant.12-18 The COMT Val158Met genotype has been most extensively studied in relation to executive control performance. Although evidence is mixed,16,17 many research groups have found better function in Met carriers and that the high-activity COMT Val/Val carriers perform more poorly on different executive control tasks.16,18-21 The same COMT genotype has also been associated with social cognitive dysfunction, lower emotional responsiveness,21 and higher pain sensitivity.21,22 In a clinical study of children with ADHD, those with the high-activity genotype were rated as having lower social cognitive function.23 Imaging studies in both adults24 and children21,25 suggest Val/Val carriers are less responsive to emotional (fearful faces) or unpleasant stimuli. These findings suggest those with the high-activity COMT genotype may have less understanding of others' feelings or be less emotionally and socially responsive to the feelings and behavior of others.23
We set out to (1) replicate the previously observed association of COMT Val158Met genotype with the extreme antisocial behavior subtype and then (2) test the hypothesis that either poor executive control and/or social cognitive dysfunction act as intermediate phenotypes for this association or, alternatively, are independent pleiotropic effects of the COMT Val158Met genotype.
We tested these hypotheses in a representative26 population cohort of UK children, the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC).26 ALSPAC originally recruited 14 541 pregnant women resident in Avon, England, with expected delivery dates of April 1, 1991, to December 31, 1992, and an additional 548 children at 7 years old who would have been eligible but whose mothers did not enroll during pregnancy. We used data from families who participated in questionnaire and clinic-based cognitive assessments when the children were aged 7.5 and 8 years, respectively.27 Data for both points were available for 5726 individuals of European origin. Additionally, we excluded those with low IQ or pervasive developmental disorder (n = 49). Genotyping data were unavailable for others (n = 1161). Also accounting for those with missing data for individual items (n = 151), a sample of 4365 children (51% male) was available for analyses.
Ethical approval for all aspects of the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and the Local Research Ethics Committees. Parents provided written consent and their children assent at each assessment. The control of ALSPAC data ensures hypotheses are registered and only access to relevant specific data given, demonstrating the a priori nature of our putative intermediate phenotypes.
Adhd diagnoses and antisocial behavior scores
The Development and Well-being Assessment (DAWBA)28 was administered to parents when their children were aged 91 months (7.5 years; mean [SD] = 91.74 [1.56] months). The DAWBA assesses the presence of DSM-IV symptoms of psychiatric disorders as well as qualifying information regarding severity, longevity, and impact on functioning and well-being. Diagnoses of DSM-IV ADHD and conduct disorder were generated by the clinician authors of the DAWBA.28 Individuals who met diagnostic criteria for a pervasive developmental disorder (n = 7) or intellectual disability (full-scale IQ [FSIQ] <70) were excluded from analyses (n = 42).
The 7 DAWBA-derived DSM-IV conduct disorder items were rated by parents as having not, perhaps, or definitely occurred over the past 12 months (Table 1). Each reply scored 0, 1, and 2, respectively, and scores were summed to give an antisocial behavior score (mean [SD] = 0.51 [0.96]). As recommended by the ALSPAC statistical team, the prorated score, imputing individual missing items, was used. To account for the positive skew of our antisocial behavior outcome variable, as expected in a general population sample of children of this age, antisocial behavior was grouped into 5 categories of increasing severity based on scores of 0, 1, 2, 3 to 4, and 5 and higher that best resulted in more equally sized groups. Because our sample was aged 7.5 years, all antisocial behavior symptoms were of childhood onset,30 thought to represent a persistent, more severe type of antisocial behavior.1,2
From the Test of Everyday Attention for Children battery31 administered at age 8 years (mean [SD] score = 103.07 [2.67]), we selected the 2 measures considered to most closely tap executive control, the Sky search selective attention task and Opposite Worlds task.
The Sky search selective attention task assesses the time taken to identify identical pairs of spaceships within a group of mismatched pairs and loads with the Stroop Task in factor analysis (a measure of executive function).31 The Opposite Worlds trial is a measure of executive control assessed using response inhibition. Participants had to inhibit a prepotent verbal response when reading a string of the numbers by saying 2 when presented with a 1 and vice versa. A Same Worlds score, reading the numbers as they appear, was subtracted from the Opposite Worlds score to account for differences in reading ability. The task has been described as tapping executive control31 and is thought to most closely tap the same prefrontal cortex pathways as tasks such as the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Following previous analysis in this sample,16 these measures were normalized using log 10 (Sky search selective attention task) and 1/square root (Opposite Worlds task) transformations.
Social cognitive dysfunction
The measure of social cognition/social understanding was obtained using items tapping this construct from the parent-reported Skuse Social Cognition Scale at age 7.5 years29 that most closely resembled the social dysfunction items in our previous study of COMT.23 Four items (Table 1) rated on a Likert scale of not true, quite/sometimes true, and very/often true were summed to give a score between 4 and 12.
Genotyping was undertaken by KBioscience. Details of the assay design are available from their Web site (http://www.kbioscience.co.uk [select Lab Services, SNP Genotyping, and Chemistry Choice from the drop-down menu]). Only data from white participants of European origin were analyzed because of known ethnic differences in allele frequencies for this variant.
Allele frequencies were 0.51 for the Met allele and 0.49 for the Val allele; there was no deviation from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium (χ21 = 0.98; P > .10) calculated using the χ2 test. DNA was not available or was not of sufficient quality for genotyping for a number of families (n = 1013). Genotyping failed in 3.3% of the sample (n = 148). There were no significant differences on any measures for those for whom genotyping was unavailable or for whom genotyping failed. As in previous studies, we divided the sample into those with the high-activity Val/Val genotype (n = 1046; 24%) and those with 1 or more Met alleles (n = 3319; 76%).
Replicating Association Between COMT Val158Met Genotype in the Presence of ADHD and Antisocial Behavior
To replicate the previously observed association, our outcome variable of antisocial behavior was regressed onto a 3-level predictor variable of COMT × ADHD. This was the product term of COMT Val/Val genotype (vs possession of ≥1 Met alleles) × ADHD (present/absent).
Testing Intermediate Phenotypes
A series of regression analyses were used. First, association between COMT genotype in the presence of ADHD and the putative intermediate phenotype was tested. Second, the relationship between the intermediate phenotype and antisocial behavior was examined. Where both associations were significant, the association between COMT genotype in the presence of ADHD and the antisocial behavior outcome variable was tested in the presence of the intermediate variable. Where the association between COMT × ADHD and antisocial behavior is reduced in the presence of the (significant) intermediate phenotype, this indicates that the intermediate phenotype is likely to be on the risk pathway.
We used cumulative logit models for ordinal data in our main analyses. We selected this method for ease of interpretation of results (odds ratios [ORs]) and to allow for the nonnormality of data. For these models, an OR was obtained for the transition between each group and its adjacent group (providing an OR for the transition between group 0 and group 1, an OR for the transition between group 1 and group 2, and so on). This enabled us to gain insights into the change in influence of a variable at differing levels of outcome severity.
Undertaken using the GOLOGIT2 commands in Stata version 10,32 these models enable testing of the parallel lines assumption (that the effect of the independent variable is equal across each transition). Following this test, variables can then be constrained (resulting in a single OR across transitions) or unconstrained (different ORs across transitions) as required.
All analyses were performed on untransformed outcome and social cognitive dysfunction data (presented) and then repeated using transformed data. No differences in the findings were observed.
Sex was included in all analyses as a covariate but did not act as a moderator of our findings. Age was also investigated but because all individuals were assessed at similar ages, no significant effect was observed and so this covariate was dropped.
REPLICATING ASSOCIATION BETWEEN COMT Val158Met GENOTYPE IN THE PRESENCE OF ADHD AND ANTISOCIAL BEHAVIOR
We found strong evidence for association between the COMT Val158Met genotype, in the presence of ADHD, and antisocial behavior (Figure 1). The strength of association increased with higher levels of antisocial behavior (OR, 2.82; 95% confidence interval [CI], 2.02-3.94; P < .001 for >4 conduct disorder symptoms) (Table 2). As in the previous studies and as originally hypothesized,5 the COMT Val158Met genotype was only associated with this extreme antisocial behavior subtype, not with antisocial behavior in those without ADHD (β < 0.001; t = 0.02; P = .93) or ADHD alone (P>.20).
Similar results, supporting our findings, were observed when FSIQ was added as a potential covariate (OR at most severe level of antisocial behavior, 2.75; 95% CI, 1.97-3.84; P < .001), the outcome measure was a diagnosis of conduct disorder (odds ratio, 3.37; 95% CI, 1.79-6.34; P < .001), there were teacher reports of antisocial behavior (OR, 1.47; 95% CI, 1.14-1.90; P = .003), and a dimensional measure of ADHD (parent-reported symptoms) was used (OR at most severe level of antisocial behavior, 1.68; 95% CI, 1.13-1.20; P < .001).
Testing potential intermediate phenotypes
The Val/Val genotype in the presence of ADHD was associated with both of the executive control measures (Sky search selective attention: β = −0.05; t = −3.06; P = .002; Opposite Worlds verbal inhibition score: β = −0.07; t = −4.54; P = 6 × 10−6) and social cognitive dysfunction (β = 0.09; t = 5.87; P < 10−7).
We then tested the relationship between these putative intermediate phenotypes and extreme antisocial behavior. As was necessary to test for statistical purposes, social cognitive dysfunction scores and 1 of the 2 executive control tasks (Opposite Worlds) were significantly associated with antisocial behavior (OR at most severe level of antisocial behavior, 2.15; 95% CI, 1.84-2.50; P < .001 and OR, 0.02; 95% CI, 0.001-0.24; P = .002, respectively). Because the Sky search selective attention measure was not significantly associated with antisocial behavior, it was not taken forward for further analysis. We finally determined whether executive control and/or social cognitive dysfunction contributed to the association between the Val/Val genotype in those with ADHD and antisocial behavior.
Table 2 and Figure 2 show these results. The magnitude of the genotype in the presence of ADHD association with antisocial behavior remained the same when the executive control measure was included in our model (Table 2 and Figure 2). When social cognitive dysfunction was entered into the model, the strength of the association between genotype in the presence of ADHD and antisocial behavior dropped both in the level of significance and the magnitude of the ORs (2.82 to only 1.87 for those with the most severe antisocial behavior) (Table 2 and Figure 2).
As some have suggested, FSIQ might be the cognitive measure most significantly associated with COMT genotype33; we also tested this. Although FSIQ was associated with COMT genotype in the presence of ADHD and antisocial behavior, it did not serve as an intermediate phenotype because no change to the strength of the genotype-outcome association was observed when FSIQ was added to the model (COMT × ADHD association at most severe level of antisocial behavior: OR, 2.75; 95% CI, 1.97-3.84; P < .001 with FSIQ in the model).
To ensure our findings were not due to mothers rating both clinical outcome and social cognitive dysfunction, we repeated the analysis using teacher ratings of antisocial behavior (parent-teacher correlation in antisocial behavior scores: r2 = 0.24; P < .01; COMT × ADHD association with teacher reports of antisocial behavior, including social cognitive dysfunction: OR, 1.37; 95% CI, 1.04-1.80; P = .02).
Finally, we further tested the role of our putative intermediate phenotypes using structural equation modeling. Structural equation modeling analyses replicated our findings. When the effects of the 2 putative intermediate phenotypes were compared, significantly more of the COMT association with antisocial behavior in the presence of ADHD was explained by the social cognitive dysfunction pathway than through executive control (results available on request).
In the context of previous reports, our study now provides compelling evidence for association between extreme antisocial behavior in children and the COMT Val/Val genotype in people with ADHD. We next tested 2 candidate pathways that might link genotype to the antisocial phenotype, one related to executive control and the other to social cognitive dysfunction. Despite evidence for association between the COMT Val158Met variant and executive control, our findings suggest that COMT genotype in the presence of ADHD confers independent effects on executive control and antisocial behavior and that our measure of executive control may not act as a link between this genetic variant and antisocial behavior (Figure 3A). Our data more strongly support social cognitive dysfunction as an intermediate phenotype. Thus, the inclusion of social cognitive dysfunction in the model led to a drop in the magnitude of the genotype–antisocial behavior association (Figure 3B). While it accounted for a large proportion of the genotype–antisocial behavior association, our measure of social cognitive dysfunction does not explain all of it. This implies either that there are additional intermediate pathways or that our measure did not capture all the relevant variation in social cognitive dysfunction.
Our findings underline the fact that caution should be exercised before concluding that a putative intermediate phenotype lies on a pathway between genotype and behavioral and psychiatric outcomes, as has increasingly become practice in recent years.34,35 Given that it is essential to identify causal links if treatments for a disorder that target putative intermediate phenotypes are to be successful, it will be important for the relationship between putative intermediate pathways and clinical outcomes to be evaluated more widely using similar approaches to those we report herein.
As we observe herein, the majority of studies, including meta-analyses, investigating the COMT Val158Met variant as a possible risk factor for ADHD do not report a significant association,6,36,37 a finding replicated in this investigation. Our results and those from 2 previous epidemiological studies6 also show no association between the COMT Val158Met variant and antisocial behavior alone. Instead, the association appears to be specific to antisocial behavior in those with ADHD. Twin and family studies3,38 have previously demonstrated that antisocial behavior in ADHD indexes increased familial loading and heritability and some distinct (including genetic) etiological factors when compared with ADHD alone. It may be that the COMT Val158Met variant is one of the genetic risk factors within this specific group.
We chose to focus a priori on 1 variant, Val158Met, in the COMT gene for this analysis. That is because it is this specific variant that has previously been found to be associated with antisocial behavior in those with ADHD. To date, other COMT variants studied have not shown association.6 It is also the variant that has been most widely investigated in relation to putative intermediate phenotypes. However, future studies could also investigate other variants that capture COMT gene variation.
Our findings may have potentially important implications for the treatment and prevention of the myriad negative consequences of early and extreme antisocial behaviors. First, this is the fourth independent demonstration that COMT genotype influences antisocial behavior in those with ADHD. This variant, like other associated genetic risk factors for complex disorders, is not a deterministic predictor of behavior. The ORs observed, while large in relation to other common risk alleles conferring risk to common disorders, are still only modest. However, these findings do provide clues about potential biological and cognitive mechanisms that underlie an important clinical problem. For example, given that current medication used to treat ADHD appears to have limited long-term benefits on associated antisocial behavior,39COMT inhibitors might be considered for evaluation in the treatment of this specific group with comorbid ADHD and antisocial behavior. Second, the finding that social cognitive dysfunction is on the risk pathway between COMT genotype with antisocial behavior in those with ADHD suggests that targeting social cognitive skills might prevent the development of antisocial behavior in this specific high-risk group of children with ADHD. Further investigation is needed to test these possibilities.
There are some limitations to our study. Although a large comprehensive cohort sample was used, not all individuals had sufficient data for inclusion in this study and only 1 measure of executive control was used. In such a large sample with a wealth of data, it would not be possible to have administered tests to assess all aspects of executive control; it is possible that other tasks may better index an intermediate phenotype on the risk pathway between genotype and disorder. This should be studied in more detail in more focused experimental studies including more detailed testing because we do not exclude the possibility that executive control is a contributor.
Our parent-rated measure of social cognitive dysfunction may not fully capture the necessary concept. However, because our measure did appear to be on the risk pathway, it is worthy of further investigation through more detailed and focused assessments. Any cohort or observational study, no matter how large, is insufficient on its own to prove causality, and designs, such as randomized controlled trials, are needed to confirm causal hypotheses.40 Furthermore, although replicated using regression and structural equation modeling analyses, we cannot rule out the possibility of unmeasured confounding factors influencing our putative intermediate phenotypes.
Using a large cohort sample, we confirm the association between the high-activity COMT genotype and a severe subtype of antisocial behavior. For the first time, to our knowledge, we were also able to simultaneously test for links between genotype to putative intermediate phenotype and behavioral outcome in the same sample. The link between the high-activity COMT genotype, executive control, and antisocial behavior may represent independent pleiotropic effects of genotype rather than the presence of a causal pathway, whereas social cognitive dysfunction does appear to lie on the pathway between risk genotype and antisocial behavior.
Correspondence: Kate Langley, PhD, Department of Psychological Medicine and Neurology, School of Medicine, Cardiff University, Heath Park, Cardiff CF14 4XN, Wales (langleyk@cardiff.ac.uk).
Submitted for Publication: February 12, 2010; final revision received June 11, 2010; accepted June 25, 2010.
Author Contributions: This publication is the work of the authors and Dr Thapar serves as guarantor for the contents of this article.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: The UK Medical Research Council, the Wellcome Trust, and the University of Bristol provide core support for ALSPAC. Drs Langley, O’Donovan, Owen, and Thapar are supported by a Wellcome Trust program grant and MRC Centre grant.
Additional Contributions: We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses. Hollie Thomas, DPhil, provided initial statistical advice and Carol Joinson, PhD, provided early advice on ALSPAC. Gordon Harold, PhD, assisted with structural equation modeling analyses.
1.Rutter
MGiller
HHagell
A Antisocial Behavior by Young People. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1998
2.Moffitt
TE Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: a developmental taxonomy.
Psychol Rev 1993;100
(4)
674- 701
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Thapar
AHarrington
R McGuffin
P Examining the comorbidity of ADHD-related behaviours and conduct problems using a twin study design.
Br J Psychiatry 2001;179224- 229
PubMedGoogle Scholar 4.Lahey
BBMoffitt
TECaspi
A Causes of Conduct Disorder and Juvenile Delinquency. New York, NY: Guildford Press; 2003
5.Thapar
ALangley
KFowler
TRice
FTuric
DWhittinger
NAggleton
JVan den Bree
MOwen
MO’Donovan
M Catechol O-methyltransferase gene variant and birth weight predict early-onset antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62
(11)
1275- 1278
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Caspi
ALangley
KMilne
BMoffitt
TEO’Donovan
MOwen
MJPolo Tomas
MPoulton
RRutter
MTaylor
AWilliams
BThapar
A A replicated molecular genetic basis for subtyping antisocial behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008;65
(2)
203- 210
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Meyer-Lindenberg
AWeinberger
DR Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7
(10)
818- 827
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.Green
AEMunafò
MRDeYoung
CGFossella
JAFan
JGray
JR Using genetic data in cognitive neuroscience: from growing pains to genuine insights.
Nat Rev Neurosci 2008;9
(9)
710- 720
PubMedGoogle Scholar 9.Walters
JTROwen
MJ Endophenotypes in psychiatric genetics.
Mol Psychiatry 2007;12
(10)
886- 890
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.Rutter
M Epidemiological methods to tackle causal questions.
Int J Epidemiol 2009;38
(1)
3- 6
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Bestelmeyer
PEPhillips
LHCrombie
CBenson
PSt Clair
D The P300 as a possible endophenotype for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: evidence from twin and patient studies.
Psychiatry Res 2009;169
(3)
212- 219
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Durston
Sde Zeeuw
PStaal
WG Imaging genetics in ADHD: a focus on cognitive control.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2009;33
(5)
674- 689
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.Patterson
JVSandman
CARing
AJin
YBunney
WE
Jr An initial report of a new biological marker for bipolar disorder: P85 evoked brain potential.
Bipolar Disord 2009;11
(6)
596- 609
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Feshbach
ND Empathy training: a field study in affective education. In:
Feshbach
S, Fraczek
A, eds.
Aggression and Behavior Change: Biological and Social Processes. New York, NY: Praeger; 1979:234-249
Google Scholar 15.Robinson
RRoberts
WLStrayer
JKoopman
R Empathy and emotional responsiveness in delinquent and non-delinquent adolescents.
Soc Dev 2007;16
(3)
555- 579doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00396.x
Google Scholar 16.Barnett
JHHeron
JRing
SMGolding
JGoldman
DXu
KJones
PB Gender-specific effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase Val108/158Met polymorphism on cognitive function in children.
Am J Psychiatry 2007;164
(1)
142- 149
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Dickinson
DElvevåg
B Genes, cognition and brain through a COMT lens.
Neuroscience 2009;164
(1)
72- 87
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Egan
MFGoldberg
TEKolachana
BSCallicott
JHMazzanti
CMStraub
REGoldman
DWeinberger
DR Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk for schizophrenia.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2001;98
(12)
6917- 6922
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Malhotra
AKKestler
LJMazzanti
CBates
JAGoldberg
TGoldman
D A functional polymorphism in the COMT gene and performance on a test of prefrontal cognition.
Am J Psychiatry 2002;159
(4)
652- 654
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Tunbridge
EMHarrison
PJWeinberger
DR Catechol-o-methyltransferase, cognition, and psychosis: Val158Met and beyond.
Biol Psychiatry 2006;60
(2)
141- 151
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Mier
DKirsch
PMeyer-Lindenberg
A Neural substrates of pleiotropic action of genetic variation in COMT: a meta-analysis.
Mol Psychiatry 2010;15
(9)
918- 927
PubMedGoogle Scholar 22.Diatchenko
LSlade
GDNackley
AGBhalang
KSigurdsson
ABelfer
IGoldman
DXu
KShabalina
SAShagin
DMax
MBMakarov
SSMaixner
W Genetic basis for individual variations in pain perception and the development of a chronic pain condition.
Hum Mol Genet 2005;14
(1)
135- 143
PubMedGoogle Scholar 23.Fowler
TALangley
KRice
FJvan den Bree
MBRoss
KWilkinson
LSOwen
MJO’Donovan
MCThapar
A Psychopathy trait scores in adolescents with childhood ADHD: the contribution of genotypes affecting MAOA, 5HTT and COMT activity.
Psychiatr Genet 2009;19
(6)
312- 319
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Smolka
MNSchumann
GWrase
JGrüsser
SMFlor
HMann
KBraus
DFGoldman
DBüchel
CHeinz
A Catechol-O-methyltransferase val158met genotype affects processing of emotional stimuli in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex.
J Neurosci 2005;25
(4)
836- 842
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Mechelli
ATognin
S McGuire
PKPrata
DSartori
GFusar-Poli
PDe Brito
SHariri
ARViding
E Genetic vulnerability to affective psychopathology in childhood: a combined voxel-based morphometry and functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry 2009;66
(3)
231- 237
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Golding
JPembrey
MJones
RTeam
ASALSPAC Study Team, ALSPAC—the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, I: study methodology.
Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 2001;15
(1)
74- 87
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Goodman
RFord
TRichards
HGatward
RMeltzer
H The Development and Well-being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2000;41
(5)
645- 655
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Skuse
DWarrington
RBishop
DChowdhury
ULau
JMandy
WPlace
M The developmental, dimensional and diagnostic interview (3di): a novel computerized assessment for autism spectrum disorders.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2004;43
(5)
548- 558
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.American Psychiatric Association Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 3rd ed, revised. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association; 1987
31.Manly
TAnderson
VNimmo-Smith
ITurner
AWatson
PRobertson
IH The differential assessment of children's attention: the Test of Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-Ch), normative sample and ADHD performance.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001;42
(8)
1065- 1081
PubMedGoogle Scholar 32. Stata Statistical Software [computer program].
Version 10. College Station, TX: StataCorp; 2007
Google Scholar 33.Barnett
JHScoriels
LMunafò
MR Meta-analysis of the cognitive effects of the catechol-O-methyltransferase gene Val158/108Met polymorphism.
Biol Psychiatry 2008;64
(2)
137- 144
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.Jucaite
AFernell
EHalldin
CForssberg
HFarde
L Reduced midbrain dopamine transporter binding in male adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: association between striatal dopamine markers and motor hyperactivity.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;57
(3)
229- 238
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.van Haren
NEBakker
SCKahn
RS Genes and structural brain imaging in schizophrenia.
Curr Opin Psychiatry 2008;21
(2)
161- 167
PubMedGoogle Scholar 36.Turic
DWilliams
HLangley
KOwen
MThapar
AO’Donovan
MC A family based study of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2005;133B
(1)
64- 67
PubMedGoogle Scholar 37.Faraone
SVPerlis
RHDoyle
AESmoller
JWGoralnick
JJHolmgren
MASklar
P Molecular genetics of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;57
(11)
1313- 1323
PubMedGoogle Scholar 38.Faraone
SVBiederman
JMonuteaux
MC Toward guidelines for pedigree selection in genetic studies of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Genet Epidemiol 2000;18
(1)
1- 16
PubMedGoogle Scholar 39.Swanson
JArnold
LEKraemer
HCHechtman
LMolina
BHinshaw
SVitiello
BJensen
PSteinhoff
KLerner
MGreenhill
LAbikoff
HWells
KEpstein
JElliott
GNewcorn
JHoza
BWigal
TMTA Cooperative Group, Evidence, interpretation, and qualification from multiple reports of long-term outcomes in the Multimodal Treatment Study of Children With ADHD (MTA), part I: executive summary.
J Atten Disord 2008;12
(1)
4- 14
PubMedGoogle Scholar 40.Rutter
M Identifying the Environmental Causes of Disease: How Should We Decide What to Believe and When to Take Action? London, England: Academy of Medical Sciences; 2007