[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Sample stimuli from the Affect Matching/Labeling task and Competitive Reaction Time Task (CRT). A, On all trials, participants chose the item on the bottom of the display that matched the target item at the top of the display. Target faces were 50% female, and facial expressions were fearful or angry on 80% of trials and happy or surprised on the remaining 20%. Trials were grouped by task condition and presented as blocks. Each block contained five 5-second trials and was preceded by a 2-second instruction cue and followed by 16-second of fixation. Participants completed 4 blocks of each condition over 2 sequential functional runs, counterbalanced across participants. Each functional run lasted 8 minutes, 36 seconds. Predictions regarding activation are based on previous studies. B, Noise intensity settings available to the participant on each trial ranged from 0 to 10, with 0 being no noise, level 1 calibrated to 60 dB, and level 10 calibrated to 110 dB. Duration settings ranged from 1 to 5 seconds in 0.5-second increments. The task consisted of 25 trials divided into 4 blocks: trial 1, measuring unprovoked aggression, and three 8-trial blocks that gradually increased opponent noise settings (means, 3.5, 6.0, and 8.5 during blocks 2, 3, and 4, respectively). Participants were predetermined to win 50% of the trials, selected at random. IFG indicates inferior frontal gyrus; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging.

Sample stimuli from the Affect Matching/Labeling task and Competitive Reaction Time Task (CRT). A, On all trials, participants chose the item on the bottom of the display that matched the target item at the top of the display. Target faces were 50% female, and facial expressions were fearful or angry on 80% of trials and happy or surprised on the remaining 20%. Trials were grouped by task condition and presented as blocks. Each block contained five 5-second trials and was preceded by a 2-second instruction cue and followed by 16-second of fixation. Participants completed 4 blocks of each condition over 2 sequential functional runs, counterbalanced across participants. Each functional run lasted 8 minutes, 36 seconds. Predictions regarding activation are based on previous studies.55,56 B, Noise intensity settings available to the participant on each trial ranged from 0 to 10, with 0 being no noise, level 1 calibrated to 60 dB, and level 10 calibrated to 110 dB. Duration settings ranged from 1 to 5 seconds in 0.5-second increments. The task consisted of 25 trials divided into 4 blocks: trial 1, measuring unprovoked aggression, and three 8-trial blocks that gradually increased opponent noise settings (means, 3.5, 6.0, and 8.5 during blocks 2, 3, and 4, respectively). Participants were predetermined to win 50% of the trials, selected at random. IFG indicates inferior frontal gyrus; fMRI, functional magnetic resonance imaging.

Figure 2. 
Perpetrated aggression across competitive reaction time task (CRT) blocks. Repeated-measures analysis of variance showed a significant block × group interaction (F3,75 = 2.88; P = .04). Follow-up t tests revealed that methamphetamine-dependent (MA) and control participants did not differ in aggressive responding during trial 1 (t25 < 1), block 2 (t25 = 1.38), or block 3 (t25 = 1.34; all P > .18) but MA-dependent participants scored significantly higher than controls during block 4 (t25 = 2.36; P = .03). Owing to the small sample size, it was not possible to include demographic covariates; however, entering each covariate separately in follow-up analyses did not change the block × group interaction.

Perpetrated aggression across competitive reaction time task (CRT) blocks. Repeated-measures analysis of variance showed a significant block × group interaction (F3,75 = 2.88; P = .04). Follow-up t tests revealed that methamphetamine-dependent (MA) and control participants did not differ in aggressive responding during trial 1 (t25 < 1), block 2 (t25 = 1.38), or block 3 (t25 = 1.34; all P > .18) but MA-dependent participants scored significantly higher than controls during block 4 (t25 = 2.36; P = .03). Owing to the small sample size, it was not possible to include demographic covariates; however, entering each covariate separately in follow-up analyses did not change the block × group interaction.

Figure 3. 
Inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and amygdala activation patterns during the Affect Matching/Labeling task. A, Methamphetamine-dependent (MA) participants (n = 36) showed less bilateral ventral IFG activation than controls (n = 33) on the affect match vs shape match contrast (Table 2). Demographic covariates had no effect on activation. Whole-brain statistical maps are overlaid onto a structural template provided by SPM. B, Repeated-measures analysis of variance of amygdala region-of-interest values showed a significant effect of condition (F2,92 = 55.12; P < .001) but no effect of group (F1,46 < 1; P = .65) or group × condition interaction (F2,92 < 1; P = .54). Across participants, amygdala activation was lower during the affect label than affect match condition (t47 = 2.69; P = .01) and lower than both affect match and affect label during the shape match condition (t47 = 10.09; t47 = 7.50; both P < .001). Demographic variables had no effect on the magnitude of decrease between conditions. C, Psychophysiological interaction analysis investigating the prefrontal cortex for regions that showed a greater inverse relationship with the amygdala during the affect label than affect match condition, identified dorsal IFG, predominantly on the right (Table 2). There was no difference between MA and control participants within these regions. Demographic variables did not influence connectivity. Statistical maps are overlaid onto a structural template provided by SPM. Orientation is neurological. Clusters in A and C did not overlap.

Inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and amygdala activation patterns during the Affect Matching/Labeling task. A, Methamphetamine-dependent (MA) participants (n = 36) showed less bilateral ventral IFG activation than controls (n = 33) on the affect match vs shape match contrast (Table 2). Demographic covariates had no effect on activation. Whole-brain statistical maps are overlaid onto a structural template provided by SPM. B, Repeated-measures analysis of variance of amygdala region-of-interest values showed a significant effect of condition (F2,92 = 55.12; P < .001) but no effect of group (F1,46 < 1; P = .65) or group × condition interaction (F2,92 < 1; P = .54). Across participants, amygdala activation was lower during the affect label than affect match condition (t47 = 2.69; P = .01) and lower than both affect match and affect label during the shape match condition (t47 = 10.09; t47 = 7.50; both P < .001). Demographic variables had no effect on the magnitude of decrease between conditions. C, Psychophysiological interaction analysis investigating the prefrontal cortex for regions that showed a greater inverse relationship with the amygdala during the affect label than affect match condition, identified dorsal IFG, predominantly on the right (Table 2). There was no difference between MA and control participants within these regions. Demographic variables did not influence connectivity. Statistical maps are overlaid onto a structural template provided by SPM. Orientation is neurological. Clusters in A and C did not overlap.

Figure 4. 
Relationships between decreased amygdala activity and aggression. Amygdala decrease denotes the magnitude of decrease in amygdala activation between affect match and affect label conditions, calculated as the difference between amygdala region-of-interest values (left and right combined) during each condition. A, Linear regression analysis showed that amygdala decrease inversely related to self-reported aggression (calculated as a composite of Aggression Questionnaire and State Trait Anger Expression Inventory trait anger and anger expression scores) in controls (β = −0.47; t = 2.42; P = .03) but not methamphetamine-dependent (MA) participants (β = 0.13; t < 1; P = .56). Age also showed a significant effect. B, Decreased amygdala activation inversely related to perpetrated aggression (competitive reaction time task [CRT] score at peak aggression [block 4]) in both groups. This effect was statistically significant in methamphetamine-dependent participants (β = −0.58; t = 4.47; P = .01) but not controls (β = −0.32; t = 1.10; P = .30). Sex also showed a significant effect in the analysis. All significant regression models survived Holm-Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Relationships between decreased amygdala activity and aggression. Amygdala decrease denotes the magnitude of decrease in amygdala activation between affect match and affect label conditions, calculated as the difference between amygdala region-of-interest values (left and right combined) during each condition. A, Linear regression analysis showed that amygdala decrease inversely related to self-reported aggression (calculated as a composite of Aggression Questionnaire and State Trait Anger Expression Inventory trait anger and anger expression scores) in controls (β = −0.47; t = 2.42; P = .03) but not methamphetamine-dependent (MA) participants (β = 0.13; t < 1; P = .56). Age also showed a significant effect. B, Decreased amygdala activation inversely related to perpetrated aggression (competitive reaction time task [CRT] score at peak aggression [block 4]) in both groups. This effect was statistically significant in methamphetamine-dependent participants (β = −0.58; t = 4.47; P = .01) but not controls (β = −0.32; t = 1.10; P = .30). Sex also showed a significant effect in the analysis. All significant regression models survived Holm-Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Figure 5. 
Relationship between ventral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) activation and emotional insight. Linear regression analysis showed that across control participants (n = 25), ventral IFG activation during affect matching was inversely related to scores on the difficulty identifying feelings subscale of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) (β = −0.42; t = 2.16; P = .04). Individual ventral IFG activation values were calculated as the cluster-weighted average of parameter estimates in left and right ventral IFG clusters (Figure 3A). However, the model did not survive Holm-Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Relationship between ventral inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) activation and emotional insight. Linear regression analysis showed that across control participants (n = 25), ventral IFG activation during affect matching was inversely related to scores on the difficulty identifying feelings subscale of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS) (β = −0.42; t = 2.16; P = .04). Individual ventral IFG activation values were calculated as the cluster-weighted average of parameter estimates in left and right ventral IFG clusters (Figure 3A). However, the model did not survive Holm-Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons.

Table 1. 
Demographic and Methamphetamine Use Characteristics of Participantsa
Demographic and Methamphetamine Use Characteristics of Participantsa
Table 2. 
Methamphetamine Abstinence Measures by Subsample
Methamphetamine Abstinence Measures by Subsample
Table 3. 
Outcome Measures by Subsamplea
Outcome Measures by Subsamplea
Table 4. 
Functional MRI Clusters During Affect Matching/Labeling Task Performancea
Functional MRI Clusters During Affect Matching/Labeling Task Performancea
1.
Logan  BKFligner  CLHaddix  T Cause and manner of death in fatalities involving methamphetamine.  J Forensic Sci 1998;43 (1) 28- 34PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Maxwell  JC Emerging research on methamphetamine.  Curr Opin Psychiatry 2005;18 (3) 235- 242PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Szuster  RR Methamphetamine in psychiatric emergencies.  Hawaii Med J 1990;49 (10) 389- 391PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
Swanson  SMSise  CBSise  MJSack  DIHolbrook  TLPaci  GM The scourge of methamphetamine: impact on a level I trauma center.  J Trauma 2007;63 (3) 531- 537PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Tominaga  GTGarcia  GDzierba  AWong  J Toll of methamphetamine on the trauma system.  Arch Surg 2004;139 (8) 844- 847PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Cartier  JFarabee  DPrendergast  ML Methamphetamine use, self-reported violent crime, and recidivism among offenders in California who abuse substances.  J Interpers Violence 2006;21 (4) 435- 445PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Cohen  JBDickow  AHorner  KZweben  JEBalabis  JVandersloot  DReiber  CMethamphetamine Treatment Project, Abuse and violence history of men and women in treatment for methamphetamine dependence.  Am J Addict 2003;12 (5) 377- 385PubMedGoogle Scholar
8.
McKetin  RMcLaren  JLubman  DIHides  L Hostility among methamphetamine users experiencing psychotic symptoms.  Am J Addict 2008;17 (3) 235- 240PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Zweben  JECohen  JBChristian  DGalloway  GPSalinardi  MParent  DIguchi  MMethamphetamine Treatment Project, Psychiatric symptoms in methamphetamine users.  Am J Addict 2004;13 (2) 181- 190PubMedGoogle Scholar
10.
Gonzales  RMooney  LRawson  RA The methamphetamine problem in the United States.  Annu Rev Public Health 2010;31385- 398PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Watanabe-Galloway  SRyan  SHansen  KHullsiek  BMuli  VMalone  AC Effects of methamphetamine abuse beyond individual users.  J Psychoactive Drugs 2009;41 (3) 241- 248PubMedGoogle Scholar
12.
Boles  SMMiotto  K Substance abuse and violence: a review of the literature.  Aggress Violent Behav 2003;8 (2) 155- 17410.1016/S1359-1789(01)00057-XGoogle Scholar
13.
Jaffe  APedersen  WCFisher  DG  et al.  Drug use, personality and partner violence: a model of separate, additive, contributions in an active drug user sample.  Open Addict J 2009;239- 47Google Scholar
14.
Stretesky  PB National case-control study of homicide offending and methamphetamine use.  J Interpers Violence 2009;24 (6) 911- 924PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Tyner  EAFremouw  WJ The relation of methamphetamine use and violence: a critical review.  Aggress Violent Behav 2008;13 (4) 285- 297doi:10.1016/j.avb.2008.04.005Google Scholar
16.
Baskin-Sommers  ASommers  I The co-occurrence of substance use and high-risk behaviors.  J Adolesc Health 2006;38 (5) 609- 611PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Sommers  IBaskin  D Methamphetamine use and violence.  J Drug Issues 2006;36 (1) 77- 96Google Scholar
18.
Payer  DELieberman  MDMonterosso  JRXu  JFong  TWLondon  ED Differences in cortical activity between methamphetamine-dependent and healthy individuals performing a facial affect matching task.  Drug Alcohol Depend 2008;93 (1-2) 93- 102PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Henry  JDMazur  MRendell  PG Social-cognitive difficulties in former users of methamphetamine.  Br J Clin Psychol 2009;48 (Pt 3) 323- 327PubMedGoogle Scholar
20.
Sekine  YOuchi  YTakei  NYoshikawa  ENakamura  KFutatsubashi  MOkada  HMinabe  YSuzuki  KIwata  YTsuchiya  KJTsukada  HIyo  MMori  N Brain serotonin transporter density and aggression in abstinent methamphetamine abusers.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (1) 90- 100PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Anderson  CABushman  BJ Human aggression.  Annu Rev Psychol 2002;5327- 51PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Simon  SLDean  ACCordova  XMonterosso  JRLondon  ED Methamphetamine dependence and neuropsychological functioning: evaluating change during early abstinence.  J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2010;71 (3) 335- 344PubMedGoogle Scholar
23.
Scott  JCWoods  SPMatt  GEMeyer  RAHeaton  RKAtkinson  JHGrant  I Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis.  Neuropsychol Rev 2007;17 (3) 275- 297PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Salo  RNordahl  TEPossin  KLeamon  MGibson  DRGalloway  GPFlynn  NMHenik  APfefferbaum  ASullivan  EV Preliminary evidence of reduced cognitive inhibition in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.  Psychiatry Res 2002;111 (1) 65- 74PubMedGoogle Scholar
25.
Salo  RNordahl  TEMoore  CWaters  CNatsuaki  YGalloway  GPKile  SSullivan  EV A dissociation in attentional control: evidence from methamphetamine dependence.  Biol Psychiatry 2005;57 (3) 310- 313PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Monterosso  JRAron  ARCordova  XXu  JLondon  ED Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse.  Drug Alcohol Depend 2005;79 (2) 273- 277PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Salo  RUrsu  SBuonocore  MHLeamon  MHCarter  C Impaired prefrontal cortical function and disrupted adaptive cognitive control in methamphetamine abusers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry 2009;65 (8) 706- 709PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Paulus  MPHozack  NFrank  LBrown  GGSchuckit  MA Decision making by methamphetamine-dependent subjects is associated with error-rate-independent decrease in prefrontal and parietal activation.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (1) 65- 74PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Monterosso  JDomier  CAinslie  GXu  JCordova  XLondon  ED Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects performing a parametric delay-discounting task.  Hum Brain Mapp 2007;28 (5) 383- 393doi:10.1002/hbm.20281Google Scholar
30.
Hoffman  WFMoore  MTemplin  RMcFarland  BHitzemann  RJMitchell  SH Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188 (2) 162- 170PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Goldstein  RZCraig  ADBechara  AGaravan  HChildress  ARPaulus  MPVolkow  ND The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction.  Trends Cogn Sci 2009;13 (9) 372- 380PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Homer  BDSolomon  TMMoeller  RWMascia  ADeRaleau  LHalkitis  PN Methamphetamine abuse and impairment of social functioning: a review of the underlying neurophysiological causes and behavioral implications.  Psychol Bull 2008;134 (2) 301- 310PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Verdejo-García  APérez-García  M Substance abusers' self-awareness of the neurobehavioral consequences of addiction.  Psychiatry Res 2008;158 (2) 172- 180PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Davidson  RJPutnam  KMLarson  CL Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation: a possible prelude to violence.  Science 2000;289 (5479) 591- 594PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Adolphs  R Is the human amygdala specialized for processing social information?  Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;985326- 340PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
LeDoux  J The amygdala.  Curr Biol 2007;17 (20) R868- R874PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Costafreda  SGBrammer  MJDavid  ASFu  CH Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies.  Brain Res Rev 2008;58 (1) 57- 70PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Sergerie  KChochol  CArmony  JL The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32 (4) 811- 830PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Adolphs  R Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms.  Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 2002;1 (1) 21- 62doi:10.1177/1534582302001001003Google Scholar
40.
Sprengelmeyer  RRausch  MEysel  UTPrzuntek  H Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions.  Proc Biol Sci 1998;265 (1409) 1927- 1931PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Hornak  JRolls  ETWade  D Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage.  Neuropsychologia 1996;34 (4) 247- 261PubMedGoogle Scholar
42.
Lange  KWilliams  LMYoung  AWBullmore  ETBrammer  MJWilliams  SCGray  JAPhillips  ML Task instructions modulate neural responses to fearful facial expressions.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (3) 226- 232PubMedGoogle Scholar
43.
Posamentier  MTAbdi  H Processing faces and facial expressions.  Neuropsychol Rev 2003;13 (3) 113- 143PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Barbas  H Flow of information for emotions through temporal and orbitofrontal pathways.  J Anat 2007;211 (2) 237- 249PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Petrides  MPandya  DN Efferent association pathways from the rostral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey.  J Neurosci 2007;27 (43) 11573- 11586PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Quirk  GJBeer  JS Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies.  Curr Opin Neurobiol 2006;16 (6) 723- 727PubMedGoogle Scholar
47.
Woermann  FGvan Elst  LTKoepp  MJFree  SLThompson  PJTrimble  MRDuncan  JS Reduction of frontal neocortical grey matter associated with affective aggression in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: an objective voxel by voxel analysis of automatically segmented MRI.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;68 (2) 162- 169PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Raine  ABuchsbaum  MLaCasse  L Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission tomography.  Biol Psychiatry 1997;42 (6) 495- 508PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Raine  AMeloy  JRBihrle  SStoddard  JLaCasse  LBuchsbaum  MS Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers.  Behav Sci Law 1998;16 (3) 319- 332PubMedGoogle Scholar
50.
Bufkin  JLLuttrell  VR Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice.  Trauma Violence Abuse 2005;6 (2) 176- 191PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
van Elst  LTWoermann  FGLemieux  LThompson  PJTrimble  MR Affective aggression in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: a quantitative MRI study of the amygdala.  Brain 2000;123 (pt 2) 234- 243PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Coccaro  EFMcCloskey  MSFitzgerald  DAPhan  KL Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;62 (2) 168- 178PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
New  ASHazlett  EABuchsbaum  MSGoodman  MMitelman  SANewmark  RTrisdorfer  RHaznedar  MMKoenigsberg  HWFlory  JSiever  LJ Amygdala-prefrontal disconnection in borderline personality disorder.  Neuropsychopharmacology 2007;32 (7) 1629- 1640PubMedGoogle Scholar
54.
Pietrini  PGuazzelli  MBasso  GJaffe  KGrafman  J Neural correlates of imaginal aggressive behavior assessed by positron emission tomography in healthy subjects.  Am J Psychiatry 2000;157 (11) 1772- 1781PubMedGoogle Scholar
55.
Lieberman  MDEisenberger  NICrockett  MJTom  SMPfeifer  JHWay  BM Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli.  Psychol Sci 2007;18 (5) 421- 428PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Hariri  ARBookheimer  SYMazziotta  JC Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system.  Neuroreport 2000;11 (1) 43- 48PubMedGoogle Scholar
57.
Hariri  ARMattay  VSTessitore  AFera  FWeinberger  DR Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (6) 494- 501PubMedGoogle Scholar
58.
Goldin  PRMcRae  KRamel  WGross  JJ The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.  Biol Psychiatry 2008;63 (6) 577- 586PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Phan  KLFitzgerald  DANathan  PJMoore  GJUhde  TWTancer  ME Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry 2005;57 (3) 210- 219PubMedGoogle Scholar
60.
Schaefer  SMJackson  DCDavidson  RJAguirre  GKKimberg  DYThompson-Schill  SL Modulation of amygdalar activity by the conscious regulation of negative emotion.  J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14 (6) 913- 921PubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Beauregard  MLévesque  JBourgouin  P Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion.  J Neurosci 2001;21 (18) RC165PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Urry  HLvan Reekum  CMJohnstone  TKalin  NHThurow  MESchaefer  HSJackson  CAFrye  CJGreischar  LLAlexander  ALDavidson  RJ Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults.  J Neurosci 2006;26 (16) 4415- 4425PubMedGoogle Scholar
63.
Ochsner  KNGross  JJ The cognitive control of emotion.  Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9 (5) 242- 249PubMedGoogle Scholar
64.
Driscoll  DTranel  DAnderson  SW The effects of voluntary regulation of positive and negative emotion on psychophysiological responsiveness.  Int J Psychophysiol 2009;72 (1) 61- 66PubMedGoogle Scholar
65.
Wager  TDDavidson  MLHughes  BLLindquist  MAOchsner  KN Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.  Neuron 2008;59 (6) 1037- 1050PubMedGoogle Scholar
66.
Ochsner  KNRay  RDCooper  JCRobertson  ERChopra  SGabrieli  JDGross  JJ For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion.  Neuroimage 2004;23 (2) 483- 499PubMedGoogle Scholar
67.
Aron  ARRobbins  TWPoldrack  RA Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex.  Trends Cogn Sci 2004;8 (4) 170- 177PubMedGoogle Scholar
68.
Thompson  PMHayashi  KMSimon  SLGeaga  JAHong  MSSui  YLee  JYToga  AWLing  WLondon  ED Structural abnormalities in the brains of human subjects who use methamphetamine.  J Neurosci 2004;24 (26) 6028- 6036PubMedGoogle Scholar
69.
Payer  DLondon  ED Methamphetamine and the brain: findings from brain imaging studies. JM  RollIn:eds.Methamphetamine Addiction: From Basic Science To Treatment. New York, NY Guildford Press2009;
70.
Baicy  KLondon  ED Corticolimbic dysregulation and chronic methamphetamine abuse.  Addiction 2007;102(Suppl 1)5- 15PubMedGoogle Scholar
71.
Aron  JLPaulus  MP Location, location: using functional magnetic resonance imaging to pinpoint brain differences relevant to stimulant use.  Addiction 2007;102(suppl 1)33- 43PubMedGoogle Scholar
72.
Hoffman  WFSchwartz  DLHuckans  MSMcFarland  BHMeiri  GStevens  AAMitchell  SH Cortical activation during delay discounting in abstinent methamphetamine dependent individuals.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;201 (2) 183- 193PubMedGoogle Scholar
73.
Kim  YTLee  JJSong  HJKim  JHKwon  DHKim  MNYoo  DSLee  HJKim  HJChang  Y Alterations in cortical activity of male methamphetamine abusers performing an empathy task: fMRI study.  Hum Psychopharmacol 2010;25 (1) 63- 70PubMedGoogle Scholar
74.
Sekine  YMinabe  YOuchi  YTakei  NIyo  MNakamura  KSuzuki  KTsukada  HOkada  HYoshikawa  EFutatsubashi  MMori  N Association of dopamine transporter loss in the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices with methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms.  Am J Psychiatry 2003;160 (9) 1699- 1701PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
London  EDSimon  SLBerman  SMMandelkern  MALichtman  AMBramen  JShinn  AKMiotto  KLearn  JDong  YMatochik  JAKurian  VNewton  TWoods  RRawson  RLing  W Mood disturbances and regional cerebral metabolic abnormalities in recently abstinent methamphetamine abusers.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61 (1) 73- 84PubMedGoogle Scholar
76.
Goldstein  RZVolkow  NDChang  LWang  GJFowler  JSDepue  RAGur  RC The orbitofrontal cortex in methamphetamine addiction: involvement in fear.  Neuroreport 2002;13 (17) 2253- 2257PubMedGoogle Scholar
77.
Li  CSSinha  R Inhibitory control and emotional stress regulation: neuroimaging evidence for frontal-limbic dysfunction in psycho-stimulant addiction.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32 (3) 581- 597PubMedGoogle Scholar
78.
Chang  LAlicata  DErnst  TVolkow  N Structural and metabolic brain changes in the striatum associated with methamphetamine abuse.  Addiction 2007;102(suppl 1)16- 32PubMedGoogle Scholar
79.
Berkman  ETLieberman  MD Using neuroscience to broaden emotion regulation: theoretical and methodological considerations.  Soc Pers Psychol Compass 2009;3 (4) 475- 493doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00186.xGoogle Scholar
80.
Tabibnia  GLieberman  MDCraske  MG The lasting effect of words on feelings: words may facilitate exposure effects to threatening images.  Emotion 2008;8 (3) 307- 317PubMedGoogle Scholar
81.
Esterling  BAL’Abate  LMurray  EJPennebaker  JW Empirical foundations for writing in prevention and psychotherapy: mental and physical health outcomes.  Clin Psychol Rev 1999;19 (1) 79- 96PubMedGoogle Scholar
82.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JBW The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-IP).  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1995;
83.
Tottenham  NTanaka  JWLeon  ACMcCarry  TNurse  MHare  TAMarcus  DJWesterlund  ACasey  BJNelson  C The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants.  Psychiatry Res 2009;168 (3) 242- 249PubMedGoogle Scholar
84.
Bushman  BJBaumeister  RF Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: does self-love or self-hate lead to violence?  J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;75 (1) 219- 229PubMedGoogle Scholar
85.
Anderson  CABushman  BJ External validity of “trivial” experiments: the case of laboratory aggression.  Rev Gen Psychol 1997;1 (1) 19- 41doi:10.1037/1089-2680.1.1.19Google Scholar
86.
Bernstein  SRichardson  DHammock  G Convergent and discriminant validity of the Taylor and Buss measures of physical aggression.  Aggress Behav 1987;13 (1) 15- 24doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1987)13:1<15::AID-AB2480130104>3.0.CO;2-KGoogle Scholar
87.
Giancola  PRZeichner  A An investigation of gender differences in alcohol-related aggression.  J Stud Alcohol 1995;56 (5) 573- 579PubMedGoogle Scholar
88.
Buss  AHWarren  WL Aggression Questionnaire: Manual.  Los Angeles, CA Western Psychological Services2000;
89.
Spielberger  CD Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory.  Odessa, FL Psychological Assessment Resources1988;
90.
Bagby  RMTaylor  GJParker  JD The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–II: convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity.  J Psychosom Res 1994;38 (1) 33- 40PubMedGoogle Scholar
91.
Zorick  TNestor  LMiotto  KSugar  CHellemann  GScanlon  GRawson  RLondon  ED Withdrawal symptoms in abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects.  Addiction 2010;105 (10) 1809- 1818PubMedGoogle Scholar
92.
Srisurapanont  MJarusuraisin  NJittiwutikan  J Amphetamine withdrawal I: reliability, validity and factor structure of a measure.  Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1999;33 (1) 89- 93PubMedGoogle Scholar
93.
 MacStim 3.0 WhiteAnt Occasional Publishing Web site.http://www.brainmapping.org/WhiteAnt/macstim.htmlAccessed June 162010;
94.
 Resonance Technology Inc Web site http://www.mrivideo.com/Accessed June 162010;
95.
 Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging Web site http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm5/Accessed June 162010;
96.
Patenaude  B Bayesian Statistical Models of Shape and Appearance for Subcortical Brain Segmentation.  Oxford, England University of Oxford2007;
97.
Brett  MAnton  JLValabregue  RPoline  JB Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox.  Neuroimage 2002;16 (2) 1140- 1141Google Scholar
98.
Friston  KJBuechel  CFink  GRMorris  JRolls  EDolan  RJ Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging.  Neuroimage 1997;6 (3) 218- 229PubMedGoogle Scholar
99.
Gitelman  DRPenny  WDAshburner  JFriston  KJ Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution.  Neuroimage 2003;19 (1) 200- 207PubMedGoogle Scholar
100.
Maldjian  JALaurienti  PJKraft  RABurdette  JH An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets.  Neuroimage 2003;19 (3) 1233- 1239PubMedGoogle Scholar
101.
Lieberman  MDCunningham  WA Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2009;4 (4) 423- 428PubMedGoogle Scholar
102.
Ashburner  JFriston  KJ Voxel-based morphometry: the methods.  Neuroimage 2000;11 (6 Pt 1) 805- 821PubMedGoogle Scholar
103.
Fusar-Poli  PPlacentino  ACarletti  FLandi  PAllen  PSurguladze  SBenedetti  FAbbamonte  MGasparotti  RBarale  FPerez  JMcGuire  PPoliti  P Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies.  J Psychiatry Neurosci 2009;34 (6) 418- 432PubMedGoogle Scholar
104.
Haxby  JVHoffman  EAGobbini  MI Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication.  Biol Psychiatry 2002;51 (1) 59- 67PubMedGoogle Scholar
105.
Reker  MOhrmann  PRauch  AVKugel  HBauer  JDannlowski  UArolt  VHeindel  WSuslow  T Individual differences in alexithymia and brain response to masked emotion faces.  Cortex 2010;46 (5) 658- 667PubMedGoogle Scholar
106.
Kano  MFukudo  SGyoba  JKamachi  MTagawa  MMochizuki  HItoh  MHongo  MYanai  K Specific brain processing of facial expressions in people with alexithymia: an H2 15O-PET study.  Brain 2003;126 (Pt 6) 1474- 1484PubMedGoogle Scholar
107.
Bermond  BVorst  HCMoormann  PP Cognitive neuropsychology of alexithymia: implications for personality typology.  Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2006;11 (3) 332- 360PubMedGoogle Scholar
108.
Lapworth  KDawe  SDavis  PKavanagh  DYoung  RSaunders  J Impulsivity and positive psychotic symptoms influence hostility in methamphetamine users.  Addict Behav 2009;34 (4) 380- 385PubMedGoogle Scholar
109.
Shamay-Tsoory  SGTomer  RBerger  BDGoldsher  DAharon-Peretz  J Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage.  Cogn Behav Neurol 2005;18 (1) 55- 67PubMedGoogle Scholar
110.
Blair  RJCipolotti  L Impaired social response reversal: a case of ‘acquired sociopathy.’  Brain 2000;123 (Pt 6) 1122- 1141PubMedGoogle Scholar
111.
Blair  RJ The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior.  Brain Cogn 2004;55 (1) 198- 208PubMedGoogle Scholar
Original Article
March 7, 2011

Neural Correlates of Affect Processing and Aggression in Methamphetamine Dependence

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Departments of Psychiatry and Biobehavioral Sciences (Drs Payer, Lieberman, and London), Psychology (Dr Lieberman), and the Molecular and Medical Pharmacology, David Geffen School of Medicine, and Brain Research Institute (Dr London), University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(3):271-282. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.154

Methamphetamine (MA) abuse is associated with a propensity for irritability, hostility, and aggression, resulting in high rates of interpersonal violence, emergency department/trauma center visits, assault, weapons charges,1-9 and, ultimately, public health and safety burdens.10,11 Despite the frequent co-occurrence of aggression with MA abuse,12-14 however, the nature of their relationship remains debated.15-17 Few laboratory studies have evaluated socioemotional function in MA-dependent individuals,18,19 and only 1 has directly assessed aggression.20 The aim of this study, therefore, was to delineate possible relationships between brain function, emotion processing, and aggression in individuals who abuse MA.

Aggression (particularly impulsive aggression) is defined as any action toward another person that is elicited by provocation, driven by anger, and intended to cause harm. Its generation is conceptualized by the General Aggression Model,21in which internal states are translated into either impulsive aggression or thoughtful action, depending on the success of appraisal and decision processes. These processes require introspection (ie, appraisal and evaluation of one's internal state) but they are only deployed if sufficient cognitive resources are available. As such, both cognitive capacity and emotional insight are necessary to produce a thoughtful outcome, while failure of either faculty can result in aggression.

Both faculties have been investigated in MA-dependent individuals. Studies of cognitive capacity22,23 have suggested deficits in attentional control,24 response inhibition,25,26 cognitive flexibility,27 and decision making.28-30 Similarly, studies of emotional insight31,32 have described poor self-awareness33 and difficulty with facial affect recognition and theory of mind.19 Disturbances in either capacity described by the General Aggression Model could therefore contribute to MA-related aggression but these links have not been tested directly.

Neurobiologically, aggression is associated with emotion processing circuitry, particularly the amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC).34 Whereas the amygdala mediates rapid, automatic responses to social stimuli,35,36 especially emotional facial expressions,37,38 the PFC mediates the more deliberative aspects of emotion processing,39 with its ventral sectors implicated in semantic processing and integration of emotional information40-42 as well as response selection and behavior control.43 The PFC can modulate amygdala activity through direct and indirect connections,44-46 and aggressive behavior relies on the integrity of this connectivity. Low PFC activity, high amygdala activity, and disruption of their connections have been linked to aggressive behavior in violent and psychiatric populations,47-53 and healthy individuals performing emotion regulation tasks including restraint from aggression54 exhibit PFC activation, reduced amygdala activity,55-61 and lowered markers of physiological arousal and subjective distress.62-64 These studies have consistently demonstrated involvement of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), often on the right side,55,65,66 which contributes to inhibitory control.67

Individuals who abuse methamphetamine show abnormalities in this circuitry, suggesting a link between neurobiological deficits and their propensity for aggression. In the PFC (particularly the IFG68), numerous structural, neurochemical, and metabolic differences have been identified,69,70 and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has uncovered deficits in PFC activation during cognitive27,29,71,72 and socioemotional tasks.18,73 Examination of subcortical regions has also uncovered MA-related neurochemical and metabolic abnormalities in the amygdala.20,74,75 These neurobiological differences have been linked to moods, psychiatric states, and personality traits that can influence aggression70,74-78 and, in one study, related to aggression itself.20 However, no study has directly linked functional differences to emotion processing and aggression.

To address this issue, we previously conducted an fMRI study investigating neural responses to emotional facial expressions in MA-dependent individuals.18 Surprisingly, the study found no difference between MA-dependent and control participants in amygdala response but revealed activation differences in the right IFG. Because one of the roles ascribed to the right IFG is inhibitory control, including control over emotional responses,79 we reasoned that the IFG finding may relate to emotion dysregulation in the MA group. However, because the task did not assess emotion regulation directly, it was not possible to test this hypothesis. The study presented here, therefore, extended the task to include such a condition.

The added task condition (affect labeling) involves verbal labeling of emotional facial expressions, which, unlike the previously used visual matching condition (affect matching), requires symbolic representation of affect. In healthy individuals, affect labeling produces neural activation patterns that are consistent with emotion regulation (ie, increased right IFG and lowered amygdala activity55-57) and is accompanied by decreased markers of negative emotion.57,80 “Putting feelings into words,” therefore, incidentally recruits PFC resources whose activity can influence the amygdala, thereby regulating those feelings.81

This study used fMRI to investigate the integrity of the PFC-amygdala circuit in MA-dependent and control participants and used self-reported and behavioral measures to relate brain function to aggression and associated traits. Specific objectives of the study were to (1) quantify and compare aggression in MA-dependent and control participants, (2) determine whether the previously observed difference in right-sided IFG activation18 reflects a deficit in emotion regulation, and (3) investigate how these activation patterns relate to aggression.

Methods
Participants and study procedure

All procedures were approved by the UCLA Office for the Protection of Research Subjects. Individuals who used MA but were not seeking treatment (MA group) and healthy control individuals (control group) between the ages of 18 and 55 years were recruited using radio, internet, and newspaper advertisements. Following an explanation of the study, participants gave written informed consent and were screened for eligibility using questionnaires, psychiatric diagnostic interviewing (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders82), and a medical examination. Participants in the MA group were required to meet DSM-IV criteria for current MA dependence and to demonstrate recent MA use by providing a urine sample that tested positive. Exclusion criteria for all participants were any current axis I diagnosis other than MA dependence or substance-induced mood or anxiety disorder in the MA group or nicotine abuse/dependence in both groups; use of psychotropic medications or substances, except some marijuana or alcohol (not qualifying for abuse or dependence); central nervous system, cardiovascular, pulmonary, or systemic disease; human immunodeficiency virus, severe hepatic impairment, hematocrit lower than 32, prostatic hypertrophy, or chronic inflammation; pregnancy; lack of English fluency; and MRI contraindications.

Eligible MA-dependent participants were admitted to the UCLA General Clinical Research Center and participated on a residential basis for 15 to 31 days. They were required to abstain from MA for the duration of the study, verified by urine screening, and no testing occurred in the first 4 days to allow residual MA to clear. Measures presented here were collected over the first 15 days, with the order slightly varied for each participant. Control participants visited the laboratory only on test days and were required to provide urine samples on each test day that tested negative for illicit substances. On completion of the study, participants were compensated with cash, gift certificates, and vouchers.

A total of 76 individuals (39 MA-dependent participants, 37 controls) participated in the study. Owing to subject attrition, late addition of measures to the study protocol, and inconsistencies in data collection, not all participants completed all measures. In addition, fMRI runs were discarded for excessive head movement, problems acquiring behavioral data, chance performance on the task, claustrophobic reaction, or missing structural data. Of the participants with acceptable fMRI data, 21 (11 MA-dependent participants, 10 controls) had participated in a previously described study,18 while 48 (25 MA-dependent participants, 23 controls) performed an updated version of the task (described below).

Measures
Task Paired With fMRI

The Affect Matching/Labeling Task55,56 is a visual match-to-sample task using face stimuli83 and is designed to elicit characteristic PFC and amygdala activation patterns during each of 3 conditions: affect match, affect label, and shape match (Figure 1A).

Out-of-Scanner Measures
Competitive Reaction Time Task

The Competitive Reaction Time Task (CRT84) is a measure of perpetrated aggression, operationalized as the amount of aversive noise to which a participant is willing to subject another person. It captures one of the hallmark features of aggression: delivery of a noxious stimulus to a victim with the intent and expectation of harming the victim.85 External, convergent, discriminant, and construct validity of the task have been established in previous studies.85-87

In this task (Figure 1B), participants believed they were competing against another person in a reaction time game (pressing a button faster than their opponent following a “go” cue) and that the loser of each trial would be subjected to a noise blast selected by the winner of the trial. In reality, opponent responses were computer controlled. Noise settings selected by the participant for delivery to the opponent on each trial were the outcome measure. Participants were debriefed immediately following completion of the task.

Aggression Questionnaire

For this 34-item paper-and-pencil questionnaire,88 participants indicated how much each item reflected their behavior on a 5-point scale.

State Trait Anger Expression Inventory

For the paper-and-pencil State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI),89 composed of 3 scales (state anger, 15 items; trait anger, 10 items; anger expression, 32 items), participants indicated agreement with each item on a 4-point scale.

Toronto Alexithymia Scale

For the 20-item, paper-and-pencil Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS),90 participants rated their agreement with each item on a 5-point scale, yielding 3 measures: difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, and externally oriented thinking.

MA Abstinence Measures
Methamphetamine Withdrawal Questionnaire

This 30-item, rater-scored questionnaire, described in detail elsewhere,91 is an adaptation of the Amphetamine Withdrawal Questionnaire.92 Participants in the MA group indicated the severity of functional, emotional, and physical withdrawal symptoms on a 4-point scale.

Visual Analog Scale for Craving

Participants in the MA group completed this measure daily, indicating current levels of MA craving on a 15-cm line marked from 0 to 100 in 10-point increments.

Apparatus

Functional MRI was performed on a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Allegra (Erlangen, Germany) using a single-channel head coil. Functional images were acquired using a standard T2*-weighted gradient-echo echo-planar imaging pulse sequence to collect blood oxygen level–dependent signal. Acquisition parameters were time to repetition, 2500 milliseconds; echo time, 28 milliseconds; flip angle, 80°; and matrix, 64 × 64. Each volume consisted of 36 interleaved slices, parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line, with slice thickness of 2.5 mm and a 25% distance factor. Each of 2 functional runs resulted in 210 volumes. T2-weighted and high-resolution T1-weighted (magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient-echo) structural scans were also acquired.

Stimulus displays for the Affect Matching/Labeling task were generated using MacStim software93 on an Apple MacBook computer (Cupertino, California) and presented through magnet-compatible video goggles.94 Responses were registered using a magnet-compatible button box.94 The CRT was performed using the HyperCard version of the program on an Apple MacBook computer, with noise blasts delivered through TDK headphones (Uniondale, New York).

Data analysis
Imaging Data

Preprocessing. Functional MRI data were processed using SPM5.95 To correct for head motion (within 3-mm translation or 5° rotation; movement beyond these parameters was exclusionary), functional images were spatially realigned to the mean image of the time series, using a least squares approach and 6-parameter rigid body spatial transformation. Images were then coregistered to individual structural templates to allow for localization of activation and subsequent spatial normalization.

Amygdala Region-of-Interest Analysis. Amygdala regions of interest (ROIs) were drawn on individual magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient-echo images using FSL FIRST software.96 Functional scans were smoothed with a 5-mm Gaussian kernel and masked with the ROIs. Using the MarsBaR toolbox,97 a general linear model was applied at each voxel within the ROIs, containing a regressor for each condition of the task (affect match, shape match, and affect label for the subset completing this condition), and fixation as an implicit baseline. Condition blocks were modeled as boxcar functions, convolved with a hemodynamic response function provided by SPM. After fitting the general linear model, parameter estimates were averaged across all voxels in the ROI, and the resulting values exported for further analysis.

Whole-Brain Analysis. For individual whole-brain analyses, functional images were smoothed with an 8-mm Gaussian kernel, and the general linear model described above was applied at each voxel across the brain. After fitting the model for each participant, the resulting maps of parameter estimates were spatially normalized to a standard template provided by SPM using a 12-parameter affine transformation and passed to a group-level random-effects analysis. The group-level model combined the previously described18 and added sample, and included sample, group, and sex as factors and age and education as covariates of no interest to account for any potentially confounding effects.

Psychophysiological Interaction Analysis. Effective connectivity between IFG and the amygdala was assessed using the psychophysiological interaction function in SPM5. Psychophysiological interaction analysis uses a multiple regression approach to isolate regions showing a differential relationship with a target region depending on psychological context and can be interpreted as the context-specific influence one brain region exerts over another.98,99 In the present study, individual FIRST-generated amygdala ROIs served as the target, conditions of the affect matching/labeling task as the manipulated context, and IFG as a potential source region for connectivity.

For each participant, regressors that modeled amygdala activity, task conditions, and the amygdala × condition interaction were entered into whole-brain multiple regression analysis. Given our a priori hypotheses,55,56 analyses were restricted to IFG using the PickAtlas toolbox.100 After estimating the model for each participant, a linear contrast was specified for a greater inverse relationship with the amygdala during the affect label than affect match condition. The resulting statistical maps were spatially normalized to the standard SPM template and passed to a group-level random effects analysis, with group and sex as factors and age and education as covariates of no interest.

All whole-brain group analyses were assessed at a statistical threshold of P < .005 with a cluster criterion of at least 30 contiguous voxels, offering a good balance between type I and type II error.101

Brain Structure

To account for potentially confounding structural differences,68 we examined volumetric information from individual FIRST- generated amygdala ROIs and IFG gray matter volume using voxel-based morphometry.102 For the voxel-based morphometry analysis, individual magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient-echo images were manually aligned to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line, segmented into 3 tissue types, spatially normalized to a standard template, modulated to adjust for nonlinear warping, and smoothed using a 12-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. Signal intensity values, representing an index of regional gray matter volume, were then extracted from voxels of interest for further analysis.

Behavioral and ROI Data

The remaining data were analyzed in SPSS 16.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois), using analysis of variance (ANOVA) and regression models. Because we were unable to match the groups for age and education, and aggression and associated neurocircuitry vary with age and sex,103 demographic variables were entered into all analyses as covariates of no interest.

Results
Participant characteristics

Demographic measures are detailed in the eTable 1. The MA and control groups did not differ in sex composition but, on average, MA-dependent participants were older than controls and had completed fewer years of education. Almost all MA-dependent participants but only about half of the controls smoked cigarettes; however, the number of cigarettes per day did not differ between groups among those who smoked. Current alcohol use was low across participants and did not differ between groups. Methamphetamine use characteristics indicated moderately heavy use in the present sample (Table 1). Withdrawal symptoms and cravings tended to decrease between intake and test days but not all differences reached statistical significance (Table 2). Neither MA use nor abstinence measures correlated with outcome measures.

Aggression and trait characteristics

To compare self-reported aggression between groups, we performed univariate ANOVAs on Aggression Questionnaire, STAXI trait anger, and STAXI anger expression scores, with group as a between-subjects factor and demographic variables as covariates of no interest. All tests showed significant differences between groups, with higher scores in MA-dependent than control participants (Table 3).

To compare perpetrated aggression between groups, we examined CRT performance. Noise intensity and duration settings correlated during all blocks (all r > 0.66; all P < .001) and were summed to form a composite score. Repeated-measures ANOVA of these scores, with group as a between-subjects factor and block as a within-subjects factor, showed a significant block × group interaction. Follow-up tests revealed higher scores in MA-dependent than control participants during block 4 (peak provocation) but no significant group differences for trial 1, block 2, or block 3 (Figure 2).

To evaluate group differences in alexithymia, we performed univariate ANOVAs on TAS subscales, with group as a between-subjects factor and demographic variables as covariates. The MA-dependent participants reported more difficulty identifying feelings than controls but no differences in describing feelings or externally oriented thinking (Table 3). Across control participants, TAS difficulty identifying feelings correlated with STAXI trait anger (r = 0.57; P = .009) and anger expression (r = 0.47; P = .04). Across MA-dependent participants, TAS total scores correlated with STAXI anger expression (r = 0.42; P = .04).

Functional mri
Affect Matching

Across all participants, the affect match vs shape match contrast showed widespread activation consistent with the neural system for face processing,103,104 including in the bilateral amygdala and IFG (Table 4). Within these regions, t tests comparing groups revealed lower activation in MA-dependent than control participants in bilateral ventral IFG, predominantly on the right (Figure 3A; Table 4). No regions showed greater activation in MA-dependent than control participants.

To account for potential volumetric differences between groups,68 we examined individual gray matter concentration in these ventral IFG clusters using voxel-based morphometry. An ANOVA testing voxel-based morphometry values for group differences showed a trend toward lower gray matter concentration in the MA group (F1,54 = 2.86; P = .10) (in addition to effects of age and sex). To test whether local gray matter concentration influenced task-related ventral IFG activation, we entered gray matter concentration as a covariate into an ANOVA, comparing activation between groups. Activation values (average parameter estimates in ventral IFG clusters) correlated between left and right clusters (r = 0.57; P < .001) and were combined by calculating a cluster-weighted average. The ANOVA showed no effect of gray matter concentration on these values, while the group difference remained (Table 3).

Finally, we investigated amygdala activation for differences between groups. Volume of amygdala ROIs differed by sex but not group (F1,64 = 1.21; P = .28). Left and right amygdala activation values (average parameter estimates across ROI voxels) correlated with one another (r = 0.68; P < .001) and were combined by calculating their average. An ANOVA of these values, with group as a factor and ROI volume as a covariate, revealed no effect of volume or group difference in activation (Tables 2 and 3).

Affect Labeling

To test the hypothesis that, owing to IFG dysfunction, MA-dependent participants would fail to lower amygdala activity during affect labeling, we performed a repeated-measures ANOVA on amygdala activation values, with group as a between-subjects factor and condition (affect match, affect label, shape match) as a within-subjects factor. Activation values from left and right amygdala ROIs correlated during all task conditions (all r > 0.61; all P < .001), and were combined by calculating the average. The ANOVA showed a significant effect of condition, and follow-up tests revealed that, as predicted, amygdala activity during the affect label condition was lower than during the affect match condition. Activation during the shape match condition was lower than during both conditions involving faces. Contrary to prediction, however, we found no group difference or group × condition interaction (Figure 3B).

To identify brain regions associated with this reduction in amygdala activation, we tested voxels across IFG for a greater inverse relationship with amygdala activity (ie, greater functional connectivity) during affect label than affect match using psychophysiological interaction analysis. Dorsal IFG showed the expected pattern of connectivity, predominantly on the right (Figure 3C, Table 4). The clusters did not overlap with the ventral IFG clusters that showed a group difference during affect matching. Within the dorsal IFG clusters, no voxels differed between MA-dependent and control participants, suggesting successful IFG recruitment and subsequent amygdala regulation in both groups.

Behavioral correlates of decreased amygdala activity

Individual decreases in amygdala activity were calculated as the difference in activation between affect match and affect label conditions. To determine the functional significance of this decrease, values were entered as independent variables into linear regression models, with aggression scores as the outcome variables and demographic measures as covariates of no interest. Self-reported aggression scores were intercorrelated (all r > 0.47; all P < .01) and were combined into a composite score by calculating their average.

The model examining these scores showed a relationship between decreased amygdala activity and self-reported aggression in control but not MA-dependent participants (Figure 4A). However, MA-dependent participants showed a relationship between decreased amygdala activity and CRT scores (Figure 4B). Control participants showed a similar relationship (Figure 4B) that did not reach statistical significance, possibly because of low statistical power owing to the small subsample. When the 2 groups were combined to increase statistical power, decreased amygdala activity, controlling for group, showed a significant inverse relationship with CRT performance (r = −0.45; P = .03).

Behavioral correlates of ventral ifg activity

Because low ventral IFG activity in the MA group (Figure 3A) did not signify emotion dysregulation, we investigated its functional significance using linear regression. Ventral IFG activation (cluster-weighted average of left and right clusters) was entered as the independent variable, with behavioral measures as outcomes and demographic measures as covariates. In controls, ventral IFG activation did not directly relate to aggression but showed a significant inverse relationship with scores on the difficulty identifying feelings subscale of the TAS (Figure 5), suggesting that ventral IFG contributes to emotional insight. In MA-dependent participants, ventral IFG activation did not relate to TAS scores, suggesting a decoupling owing to their functional deficit in this region.

Comment

The results are consistent with the view that emotional insight, in addition to emotion regulation, contributes to aggression,21 and that this capacity involves the ventral IFG.105-107 Low ventral IFG activity and associated alexithymia in MA-dependent individuals may therefore precipitate aggression despite successful emotion regulation. Because results were found in early abstinence and did not relate to MA use history or withdrawal, they also suggest that at least some proportion of MA-related aggression is mediated by personality characteristics rather than acute intoxication, withdrawal, or MA use history.

In this study, MA-dependent participants self-reported higher aggression than controls, replicating previous findings20 and confirming descriptions from community samples.1-9 The MA-dependent participants also perpetrated more aggression on the CRT, where, despite similar initial behavior to controls, they escalated aggression more steeply following provocation. These results provide the first laboratory description of MA-related aggression patterns and suggest that aggression occurs as an increasingly disproportionate response to interpersonal interaction, rather than a preemptive attack.

According to the General Aggression Model,21 failure of either emotion regulation or emotional insight can account for such a pattern. Despite our hypotheses' focus on emotion regulation, however, we found no deficit in this capacity in MA-dependent participants, as affect labeling resulted in dorsal IFG recruitment and lowered amygdala activity across participants. These activation patterns related to self-reported aggression in controls and perpetrated aggression in all participants, suggesting that they represent a neural signature for successful emotion regulation55,57 and that this capacity is relevant to the restraint of aggression in both groups.

Instead, poor emotional insight may underlie MA-related aggression. The General Aggression Model states that even in the presence of sufficient cognitive capacity (ie, emotion regulation), behavior can be aggressive if assessment of internal states is unsuccessful. Alexithymia scores in the MA sample support this view, showing greater difficulty identifying feelings, which, in turn, related to self-reported aggression. This finding is consistent with evidence of impaired introspection and social comprehension in drug addiction31,32 and evidence that MA-related hostility results in part from misinterpretation of the world as a hostile and threatening place.108

Importantly, our imaging data suggest that emotional insight relies on the ventral IFG, a region showing dysfunction in MA-dependent participants. During facial affect matching, MA-dependent participants showed low activity in bilateral ventral IFG (not overlapping with the dorsal IFG region implicated in amygdala regulation), while amygdala activation did not differ between groups. These results replicate and extend our previous findings18 and suggest that, while amygdala-dependent automatic reactions to socioemotional cues are comparable with those of healthy individuals, IFG-dependent deliberative processing is compromised. Ventral IFG is implicated in the recognition, representation, and comprehension of emotionally salient information, including the mental and emotional states of oneself and others,40-42,109 and neurocognitive models suggest that its activity can influence behavioral outcomes by modulating hypothalamic fight-or-flight responses following comprehension of socioemotional cues.110,111 The inverse correlation between ventral IFG activity and alexithymia observed in the controls is consistent with this evidence and suggests that low ventral IFG activity (as exhibited by MA-dependent participants) reflects a limited capacity to identify feelings. In line with a previously described relationship between ventral IFG function and harm avoidance/fear in MA-dependent individuals,76 this deficit could diminish the motivation to temper maladaptive interpersonal behavior, thus escalating aggression.

Beyond increasing the likelihood of aggression, the same deficit could also contribute to the unreliable self-reporting observed among MA-dependent participants. The finding that in the MA group, decreased amygdala activity related to perpetrated aggression but not self-report of this aggression suggests that, owing to limited insight, objective tasks characterize their behavior more reliably than subjective self-report.

Together, the data are consistent with theoretical21 and neurocognitive111 models of aggression and suggest that a deficit in the evaluation of internal states rather than insufficient cognitive capacity precipitates MA-related aggression. Amygdala activation during affect matching suggests appropriate immediate responses, and successful amygdala regulation by dorsal IFG during affect labeling suggests sufficient cognitive capacity; however, low ventral IFG activation and associated alexithymia suggest limited evaluation of internal states, thus favoring aggressive outcomes.

Several limitations of the study should be noted. First, we were unable to match MA-dependent and control participants for age, education, psychiatric history, and smoking status, potentially confounding group differences. Although we included demographic covariates in analyses and performed follow-up tests, we recognize that MA-dependent and control participants likely differ in ways other than MA exposure and that these factors need to be distinguished in future studies. Second, MA-related abnormalities in neurovascular coupling or hemodynamic response could have influenced fMRI results. To minimize such effects, the study used a blocked design but this decreased temporal resolution. The surprising lack of a group difference in amygdala activity or regulation could therefore reflect the low temporal resolution of the design (or the low spatial resolution of fMRI) rather than true equivalence in function. Further, amygdala and ventral PFC are susceptible to signal dropout, potentially obscuring the data. Third, decreased amygdala activation between affect match and label conditions could have resulted from factors other than inhibitory control processes such as differences in stimulus parameters or attention. However, Lieberman et al55 have shown that amygdala activity decreases with affect labeling but not perceptual and attentional control conditions, and other studies57,80 have shown associated decreases in subjective measures of emotion, making incidental emotion regulation a plausible interpretation. Finally, although our data suggest that alexithymia is a crucial contributor to MA-related aggression, the possibility that additional trait characteristics (eg, impulsivity, volatile temper, sensation-seeking) mediate this relationship cannot be excluded.

These limitations notwithstanding, the study adds important neurobiological components to the examination of aggression in MA dependence. The findings suggest that emotion regulation, at least when elicited incidentally, can be successful in MA-dependent individuals but that dysfunction of ventral IFG contributes to heightened aggression by limiting emotional insight. In the continued pursuit of intervention strategies focused on stress-related relapse prevention and improved personal and social function, future studies may therefore benefit from taking these socioemotional considerations into account.

Correspondence: Edythe D. London, PhD, UCLA Semel Institute, 740 Westwood Plaza, Room C8-528, Los Angeles, CA 90095 (elondon@mednet.ucla.edu).

Submitted for Publication: January 19, 2010; final revision received June 16, 2010; accepted August 25, 2010.

Published Online: November 1, 2010. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.154

Author Contributions: Dr London takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of analyses. Drs London and Payer had full access to the data in the study.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

Funding/Support: This study was supported by National Institutes of Health grants R01 DA020726, R01 DA015179, P20 DA022539 (Dr London) and R01 MH084116 (Dr Lieberman); individual fellowship F31 DA025422 (Dr Payer); Guggenheim Grant 20070111 (Dr Lieberman); institutional training grants T90 DA022768, T32 DA024635, and M01 RR00865 (UCLA General Clinical Research Center); endowments from the Katherine K. and Thomas P. Pike Chair in Addiction Studies; and the Marjorie Green Family Trust (Dr London).

Disclaimer: The funding sources had no role in the design or conduct of the study, collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data, or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Previous Presentations: This study was presented in part at the annual meetings of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping; June 18-23, 2009; San Francisco, California; and the Society for Neuroscience; October 17-21, Chicago, Illinois.

Additional Information: A subset of the sample described in this article was used in a previous publication.18

Additional Contributions: The authors thank Todd Zorick, MD, PhD, for clinical oversight of the study; Catherine Sugar, PhD, for statistical advice; Sarah Wilson, MA, for coordination of the study; Angelica Morales for helpful comments and contribution of voxel-based morphometry data; Kristina Mouzakis and Greg Shipman for database support; and Christine Baker, Clayton Clement, Natalie DeShetler, Bahar Ebrat, Lisa Giragosian, Tom Hanson, MA, Lindsay King, Nathasha Moallem, Brittany Sumerel, and Mary Walker Susselman, CNMT, RT(N)(MR), for participant recruitment, screening, and retention. We also thank 3 anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.

References
1.
Logan  BKFligner  CLHaddix  T Cause and manner of death in fatalities involving methamphetamine.  J Forensic Sci 1998;43 (1) 28- 34PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Maxwell  JC Emerging research on methamphetamine.  Curr Opin Psychiatry 2005;18 (3) 235- 242PubMedGoogle Scholar
3.
Szuster  RR Methamphetamine in psychiatric emergencies.  Hawaii Med J 1990;49 (10) 389- 391PubMedGoogle Scholar
4.
Swanson  SMSise  CBSise  MJSack  DIHolbrook  TLPaci  GM The scourge of methamphetamine: impact on a level I trauma center.  J Trauma 2007;63 (3) 531- 537PubMedGoogle Scholar
5.
Tominaga  GTGarcia  GDzierba  AWong  J Toll of methamphetamine on the trauma system.  Arch Surg 2004;139 (8) 844- 847PubMedGoogle Scholar
6.
Cartier  JFarabee  DPrendergast  ML Methamphetamine use, self-reported violent crime, and recidivism among offenders in California who abuse substances.  J Interpers Violence 2006;21 (4) 435- 445PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Cohen  JBDickow  AHorner  KZweben  JEBalabis  JVandersloot  DReiber  CMethamphetamine Treatment Project, Abuse and violence history of men and women in treatment for methamphetamine dependence.  Am J Addict 2003;12 (5) 377- 385PubMedGoogle Scholar
8.
McKetin  RMcLaren  JLubman  DIHides  L Hostility among methamphetamine users experiencing psychotic symptoms.  Am J Addict 2008;17 (3) 235- 240PubMedGoogle Scholar
9.
Zweben  JECohen  JBChristian  DGalloway  GPSalinardi  MParent  DIguchi  MMethamphetamine Treatment Project, Psychiatric symptoms in methamphetamine users.  Am J Addict 2004;13 (2) 181- 190PubMedGoogle Scholar
10.
Gonzales  RMooney  LRawson  RA The methamphetamine problem in the United States.  Annu Rev Public Health 2010;31385- 398PubMedGoogle Scholar
11.
Watanabe-Galloway  SRyan  SHansen  KHullsiek  BMuli  VMalone  AC Effects of methamphetamine abuse beyond individual users.  J Psychoactive Drugs 2009;41 (3) 241- 248PubMedGoogle Scholar
12.
Boles  SMMiotto  K Substance abuse and violence: a review of the literature.  Aggress Violent Behav 2003;8 (2) 155- 17410.1016/S1359-1789(01)00057-XGoogle Scholar
13.
Jaffe  APedersen  WCFisher  DG  et al.  Drug use, personality and partner violence: a model of separate, additive, contributions in an active drug user sample.  Open Addict J 2009;239- 47Google Scholar
14.
Stretesky  PB National case-control study of homicide offending and methamphetamine use.  J Interpers Violence 2009;24 (6) 911- 924PubMedGoogle Scholar
15.
Tyner  EAFremouw  WJ The relation of methamphetamine use and violence: a critical review.  Aggress Violent Behav 2008;13 (4) 285- 297doi:10.1016/j.avb.2008.04.005Google Scholar
16.
Baskin-Sommers  ASommers  I The co-occurrence of substance use and high-risk behaviors.  J Adolesc Health 2006;38 (5) 609- 611PubMedGoogle Scholar
17.
Sommers  IBaskin  D Methamphetamine use and violence.  J Drug Issues 2006;36 (1) 77- 96Google Scholar
18.
Payer  DELieberman  MDMonterosso  JRXu  JFong  TWLondon  ED Differences in cortical activity between methamphetamine-dependent and healthy individuals performing a facial affect matching task.  Drug Alcohol Depend 2008;93 (1-2) 93- 102PubMedGoogle Scholar
19.
Henry  JDMazur  MRendell  PG Social-cognitive difficulties in former users of methamphetamine.  Br J Clin Psychol 2009;48 (Pt 3) 323- 327PubMedGoogle Scholar
20.
Sekine  YOuchi  YTakei  NYoshikawa  ENakamura  KFutatsubashi  MOkada  HMinabe  YSuzuki  KIwata  YTsuchiya  KJTsukada  HIyo  MMori  N Brain serotonin transporter density and aggression in abstinent methamphetamine abusers.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (1) 90- 100PubMedGoogle Scholar
21.
Anderson  CABushman  BJ Human aggression.  Annu Rev Psychol 2002;5327- 51PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Simon  SLDean  ACCordova  XMonterosso  JRLondon  ED Methamphetamine dependence and neuropsychological functioning: evaluating change during early abstinence.  J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2010;71 (3) 335- 344PubMedGoogle Scholar
23.
Scott  JCWoods  SPMatt  GEMeyer  RAHeaton  RKAtkinson  JHGrant  I Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis.  Neuropsychol Rev 2007;17 (3) 275- 297PubMedGoogle Scholar
24.
Salo  RNordahl  TEPossin  KLeamon  MGibson  DRGalloway  GPFlynn  NMHenik  APfefferbaum  ASullivan  EV Preliminary evidence of reduced cognitive inhibition in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.  Psychiatry Res 2002;111 (1) 65- 74PubMedGoogle Scholar
25.
Salo  RNordahl  TEMoore  CWaters  CNatsuaki  YGalloway  GPKile  SSullivan  EV A dissociation in attentional control: evidence from methamphetamine dependence.  Biol Psychiatry 2005;57 (3) 310- 313PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Monterosso  JRAron  ARCordova  XXu  JLondon  ED Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse.  Drug Alcohol Depend 2005;79 (2) 273- 277PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Salo  RUrsu  SBuonocore  MHLeamon  MHCarter  C Impaired prefrontal cortical function and disrupted adaptive cognitive control in methamphetamine abusers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry 2009;65 (8) 706- 709PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Paulus  MPHozack  NFrank  LBrown  GGSchuckit  MA Decision making by methamphetamine-dependent subjects is associated with error-rate-independent decrease in prefrontal and parietal activation.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (1) 65- 74PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Monterosso  JDomier  CAinslie  GXu  JCordova  XLondon  ED Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects performing a parametric delay-discounting task.  Hum Brain Mapp 2007;28 (5) 383- 393doi:10.1002/hbm.20281Google Scholar
30.
Hoffman  WFMoore  MTemplin  RMcFarland  BHitzemann  RJMitchell  SH Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188 (2) 162- 170PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Goldstein  RZCraig  ADBechara  AGaravan  HChildress  ARPaulus  MPVolkow  ND The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction.  Trends Cogn Sci 2009;13 (9) 372- 380PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Homer  BDSolomon  TMMoeller  RWMascia  ADeRaleau  LHalkitis  PN Methamphetamine abuse and impairment of social functioning: a review of the underlying neurophysiological causes and behavioral implications.  Psychol Bull 2008;134 (2) 301- 310PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Verdejo-García  APérez-García  M Substance abusers' self-awareness of the neurobehavioral consequences of addiction.  Psychiatry Res 2008;158 (2) 172- 180PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Davidson  RJPutnam  KMLarson  CL Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation: a possible prelude to violence.  Science 2000;289 (5479) 591- 594PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Adolphs  R Is the human amygdala specialized for processing social information?  Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;985326- 340PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
LeDoux  J The amygdala.  Curr Biol 2007;17 (20) R868- R874PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Costafreda  SGBrammer  MJDavid  ASFu  CH Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies.  Brain Res Rev 2008;58 (1) 57- 70PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Sergerie  KChochol  CArmony  JL The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32 (4) 811- 830PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Adolphs  R Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms.  Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 2002;1 (1) 21- 62doi:10.1177/1534582302001001003Google Scholar
40.
Sprengelmeyer  RRausch  MEysel  UTPrzuntek  H Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions.  Proc Biol Sci 1998;265 (1409) 1927- 1931PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Hornak  JRolls  ETWade  D Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage.  Neuropsychologia 1996;34 (4) 247- 261PubMedGoogle Scholar
42.
Lange  KWilliams  LMYoung  AWBullmore  ETBrammer  MJWilliams  SCGray  JAPhillips  ML Task instructions modulate neural responses to fearful facial expressions.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (3) 226- 232PubMedGoogle Scholar
43.
Posamentier  MTAbdi  H Processing faces and facial expressions.  Neuropsychol Rev 2003;13 (3) 113- 143PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Barbas  H Flow of information for emotions through temporal and orbitofrontal pathways.  J Anat 2007;211 (2) 237- 249PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Petrides  MPandya  DN Efferent association pathways from the rostral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey.  J Neurosci 2007;27 (43) 11573- 11586PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Quirk  GJBeer  JS Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies.  Curr Opin Neurobiol 2006;16 (6) 723- 727PubMedGoogle Scholar
47.
Woermann  FGvan Elst  LTKoepp  MJFree  SLThompson  PJTrimble  MRDuncan  JS Reduction of frontal neocortical grey matter associated with affective aggression in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: an objective voxel by voxel analysis of automatically segmented MRI.  J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;68 (2) 162- 169PubMedGoogle Scholar
48.
Raine  ABuchsbaum  MLaCasse  L Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission tomography.  Biol Psychiatry 1997;42 (6) 495- 508PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
Raine  AMeloy  JRBihrle  SStoddard  JLaCasse  LBuchsbaum  MS Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers.  Behav Sci Law 1998;16 (3) 319- 332PubMedGoogle Scholar
50.
Bufkin  JLLuttrell  VR Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice.  Trauma Violence Abuse 2005;6 (2) 176- 191PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
van Elst  LTWoermann  FGLemieux  LThompson  PJTrimble  MR Affective aggression in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: a quantitative MRI study of the amygdala.  Brain 2000;123 (pt 2) 234- 243PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Coccaro  EFMcCloskey  MSFitzgerald  DAPhan  KL Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression.  Biol Psychiatry 2007;62 (2) 168- 178PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
New  ASHazlett  EABuchsbaum  MSGoodman  MMitelman  SANewmark  RTrisdorfer  RHaznedar  MMKoenigsberg  HWFlory  JSiever  LJ Amygdala-prefrontal disconnection in borderline personality disorder.  Neuropsychopharmacology 2007;32 (7) 1629- 1640PubMedGoogle Scholar
54.
Pietrini  PGuazzelli  MBasso  GJaffe  KGrafman  J Neural correlates of imaginal aggressive behavior assessed by positron emission tomography in healthy subjects.  Am J Psychiatry 2000;157 (11) 1772- 1781PubMedGoogle Scholar
55.
Lieberman  MDEisenberger  NICrockett  MJTom  SMPfeifer  JHWay  BM Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli.  Psychol Sci 2007;18 (5) 421- 428PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Hariri  ARBookheimer  SYMazziotta  JC Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system.  Neuroreport 2000;11 (1) 43- 48PubMedGoogle Scholar
57.
Hariri  ARMattay  VSTessitore  AFera  FWeinberger  DR Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;53 (6) 494- 501PubMedGoogle Scholar
58.
Goldin  PRMcRae  KRamel  WGross  JJ The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.  Biol Psychiatry 2008;63 (6) 577- 586PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Phan  KLFitzgerald  DANathan  PJMoore  GJUhde  TWTancer  ME Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.  Biol Psychiatry 2005;57 (3) 210- 219PubMedGoogle Scholar
60.
Schaefer  SMJackson  DCDavidson  RJAguirre  GKKimberg  DYThompson-Schill  SL Modulation of amygdalar activity by the conscious regulation of negative emotion.  J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14 (6) 913- 921PubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Beauregard  MLévesque  JBourgouin  P Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion.  J Neurosci 2001;21 (18) RC165PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Urry  HLvan Reekum  CMJohnstone  TKalin  NHThurow  MESchaefer  HSJackson  CAFrye  CJGreischar  LLAlexander  ALDavidson  RJ Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults.  J Neurosci 2006;26 (16) 4415- 4425PubMedGoogle Scholar
63.
Ochsner  KNGross  JJ The cognitive control of emotion.  Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9 (5) 242- 249PubMedGoogle Scholar
64.
Driscoll  DTranel  DAnderson  SW The effects of voluntary regulation of positive and negative emotion on psychophysiological responsiveness.  Int J Psychophysiol 2009;72 (1) 61- 66PubMedGoogle Scholar
65.
Wager  TDDavidson  MLHughes  BLLindquist  MAOchsner  KN Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.  Neuron 2008;59 (6) 1037- 1050PubMedGoogle Scholar
66.
Ochsner  KNRay  RDCooper  JCRobertson  ERChopra  SGabrieli  JDGross  JJ For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion.  Neuroimage 2004;23 (2) 483- 499PubMedGoogle Scholar
67.
Aron  ARRobbins  TWPoldrack  RA Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex.  Trends Cogn Sci 2004;8 (4) 170- 177PubMedGoogle Scholar
68.
Thompson  PMHayashi  KMSimon  SLGeaga  JAHong  MSSui  YLee  JYToga  AWLing  WLondon  ED Structural abnormalities in the brains of human subjects who use methamphetamine.  J Neurosci 2004;24 (26) 6028- 6036PubMedGoogle Scholar
69.
Payer  DLondon  ED Methamphetamine and the brain: findings from brain imaging studies. JM  RollIn:eds.Methamphetamine Addiction: From Basic Science To Treatment. New York, NY Guildford Press2009;
70.
Baicy  KLondon  ED Corticolimbic dysregulation and chronic methamphetamine abuse.  Addiction 2007;102(Suppl 1)5- 15PubMedGoogle Scholar
71.
Aron  JLPaulus  MP Location, location: using functional magnetic resonance imaging to pinpoint brain differences relevant to stimulant use.  Addiction 2007;102(suppl 1)33- 43PubMedGoogle Scholar
72.
Hoffman  WFSchwartz  DLHuckans  MSMcFarland  BHMeiri  GStevens  AAMitchell  SH Cortical activation during delay discounting in abstinent methamphetamine dependent individuals.  Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;201 (2) 183- 193PubMedGoogle Scholar
73.
Kim  YTLee  JJSong  HJKim  JHKwon  DHKim  MNYoo  DSLee  HJKim  HJChang  Y Alterations in cortical activity of male methamphetamine abusers performing an empathy task: fMRI study.  Hum Psychopharmacol 2010;25 (1) 63- 70PubMedGoogle Scholar
74.
Sekine  YMinabe  YOuchi  YTakei  NIyo  MNakamura  KSuzuki  KTsukada  HOkada  HYoshikawa  EFutatsubashi  MMori  N Association of dopamine transporter loss in the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices with methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms.  Am J Psychiatry 2003;160 (9) 1699- 1701PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
London  EDSimon  SLBerman  SMMandelkern  MALichtman  AMBramen  JShinn  AKMiotto  KLearn  JDong  YMatochik  JAKurian  VNewton  TWoods  RRawson  RLing  W Mood disturbances and regional cerebral metabolic abnormalities in recently abstinent methamphetamine abusers.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61 (1) 73- 84PubMedGoogle Scholar
76.
Goldstein  RZVolkow  NDChang  LWang  GJFowler  JSDepue  RAGur  RC The orbitofrontal cortex in methamphetamine addiction: involvement in fear.  Neuroreport 2002;13 (17) 2253- 2257PubMedGoogle Scholar
77.
Li  CSSinha  R Inhibitory control and emotional stress regulation: neuroimaging evidence for frontal-limbic dysfunction in psycho-stimulant addiction.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32 (3) 581- 597PubMedGoogle Scholar
78.
Chang  LAlicata  DErnst  TVolkow  N Structural and metabolic brain changes in the striatum associated with methamphetamine abuse.  Addiction 2007;102(suppl 1)16- 32PubMedGoogle Scholar
79.
Berkman  ETLieberman  MD Using neuroscience to broaden emotion regulation: theoretical and methodological considerations.  Soc Pers Psychol Compass 2009;3 (4) 475- 493doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00186.xGoogle Scholar
80.
Tabibnia  GLieberman  MDCraske  MG The lasting effect of words on feelings: words may facilitate exposure effects to threatening images.  Emotion 2008;8 (3) 307- 317PubMedGoogle Scholar
81.
Esterling  BAL’Abate  LMurray  EJPennebaker  JW Empirical foundations for writing in prevention and psychotherapy: mental and physical health outcomes.  Clin Psychol Rev 1999;19 (1) 79- 96PubMedGoogle Scholar
82.
First  MBSpitzer  RLGibbon  MWilliams  JBW The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-IP).  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1995;
83.
Tottenham  NTanaka  JWLeon  ACMcCarry  TNurse  MHare  TAMarcus  DJWesterlund  ACasey  BJNelson  C The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants.  Psychiatry Res 2009;168 (3) 242- 249PubMedGoogle Scholar
84.
Bushman  BJBaumeister  RF Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: does self-love or self-hate lead to violence?  J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;75 (1) 219- 229PubMedGoogle Scholar
85.
Anderson  CABushman  BJ External validity of “trivial” experiments: the case of laboratory aggression.  Rev Gen Psychol 1997;1 (1) 19- 41doi:10.1037/1089-2680.1.1.19Google Scholar
86.
Bernstein  SRichardson  DHammock  G Convergent and discriminant validity of the Taylor and Buss measures of physical aggression.  Aggress Behav 1987;13 (1) 15- 24doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1987)13:1<15::AID-AB2480130104>3.0.CO;2-KGoogle Scholar
87.
Giancola  PRZeichner  A An investigation of gender differences in alcohol-related aggression.  J Stud Alcohol 1995;56 (5) 573- 579PubMedGoogle Scholar
88.
Buss  AHWarren  WL Aggression Questionnaire: Manual.  Los Angeles, CA Western Psychological Services2000;
89.
Spielberger  CD Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory.  Odessa, FL Psychological Assessment Resources1988;
90.
Bagby  RMTaylor  GJParker  JD The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–II: convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity.  J Psychosom Res 1994;38 (1) 33- 40PubMedGoogle Scholar
91.
Zorick  TNestor  LMiotto  KSugar  CHellemann  GScanlon  GRawson  RLondon  ED Withdrawal symptoms in abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects.  Addiction 2010;105 (10) 1809- 1818PubMedGoogle Scholar
92.
Srisurapanont  MJarusuraisin  NJittiwutikan  J Amphetamine withdrawal I: reliability, validity and factor structure of a measure.  Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1999;33 (1) 89- 93PubMedGoogle Scholar
93.
 MacStim 3.0 WhiteAnt Occasional Publishing Web site.http://www.brainmapping.org/WhiteAnt/macstim.htmlAccessed June 162010;
94.
 Resonance Technology Inc Web site http://www.mrivideo.com/Accessed June 162010;
95.
 Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging Web site http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm5/Accessed June 162010;
96.
Patenaude  B Bayesian Statistical Models of Shape and Appearance for Subcortical Brain Segmentation.  Oxford, England University of Oxford2007;
97.
Brett  MAnton  JLValabregue  RPoline  JB Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox.  Neuroimage 2002;16 (2) 1140- 1141Google Scholar
98.
Friston  KJBuechel  CFink  GRMorris  JRolls  EDolan  RJ Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging.  Neuroimage 1997;6 (3) 218- 229PubMedGoogle Scholar
99.
Gitelman  DRPenny  WDAshburner  JFriston  KJ Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution.  Neuroimage 2003;19 (1) 200- 207PubMedGoogle Scholar
100.
Maldjian  JALaurienti  PJKraft  RABurdette  JH An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets.  Neuroimage 2003;19 (3) 1233- 1239PubMedGoogle Scholar
101.
Lieberman  MDCunningham  WA Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale.  Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2009;4 (4) 423- 428PubMedGoogle Scholar
102.
Ashburner  JFriston  KJ Voxel-based morphometry: the methods.  Neuroimage 2000;11 (6 Pt 1) 805- 821PubMedGoogle Scholar
103.
Fusar-Poli  PPlacentino  ACarletti  FLandi  PAllen  PSurguladze  SBenedetti  FAbbamonte  MGasparotti  RBarale  FPerez  JMcGuire  PPoliti  P Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies.  J Psychiatry Neurosci 2009;34 (6) 418- 432PubMedGoogle Scholar
104.
Haxby  JVHoffman  EAGobbini  MI Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication.  Biol Psychiatry 2002;51 (1) 59- 67PubMedGoogle Scholar
105.
Reker  MOhrmann  PRauch  AVKugel  HBauer  JDannlowski  UArolt  VHeindel  WSuslow  T Individual differences in alexithymia and brain response to masked emotion faces.  Cortex 2010;46 (5) 658- 667PubMedGoogle Scholar
106.
Kano  MFukudo  SGyoba  JKamachi  MTagawa  MMochizuki  HItoh  MHongo  MYanai  K Specific brain processing of facial expressions in people with alexithymia: an H2 15O-PET study.  Brain 2003;126 (Pt 6) 1474- 1484PubMedGoogle Scholar
107.
Bermond  BVorst  HCMoormann  PP Cognitive neuropsychology of alexithymia: implications for personality typology.  Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2006;11 (3) 332- 360PubMedGoogle Scholar
108.
Lapworth  KDawe  SDavis  PKavanagh  DYoung  RSaunders  J Impulsivity and positive psychotic symptoms influence hostility in methamphetamine users.  Addict Behav 2009;34 (4) 380- 385PubMedGoogle Scholar
109.
Shamay-Tsoory  SGTomer  RBerger  BDGoldsher  DAharon-Peretz  J Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage.  Cogn Behav Neurol 2005;18 (1) 55- 67PubMedGoogle Scholar
110.
Blair  RJCipolotti  L Impaired social response reversal: a case of ‘acquired sociopathy.’  Brain 2000;123 (Pt 6) 1122- 1141PubMedGoogle Scholar
111.
Blair  RJ The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior.  Brain Cogn 2004;55 (1) 198- 208PubMedGoogle Scholar
×