Methamphetamine (MA) abuse is associated with a propensity for irritability, hostility, and aggression, resulting in high rates of interpersonal violence, emergency department/trauma center visits, assault, weapons charges,1-9 and, ultimately, public health and safety burdens.10,11 Despite the frequent co-occurrence of aggression with MA abuse,12-14 however, the nature of their relationship remains debated.15-17 Few laboratory studies have evaluated socioemotional function in MA-dependent individuals,18,19 and only 1 has directly assessed aggression.20 The aim of this study, therefore, was to delineate possible relationships between brain function, emotion processing, and aggression in individuals who abuse MA.
Aggression (particularly impulsive aggression) is defined as any action toward another person that is elicited by provocation, driven by anger, and intended to cause harm. Its generation is conceptualized by the General Aggression Model,21in which internal states are translated into either impulsive aggression or thoughtful action, depending on the success of appraisal and decision processes. These processes require introspection (ie, appraisal and evaluation of one's internal state) but they are only deployed if sufficient cognitive resources are available. As such, both cognitive capacity and emotional insight are necessary to produce a thoughtful outcome, while failure of either faculty can result in aggression.
Both faculties have been investigated in MA-dependent individuals. Studies of cognitive capacity22,23 have suggested deficits in attentional control,24 response inhibition,25,26 cognitive flexibility,27 and decision making.28-30 Similarly, studies of emotional insight31,32 have described poor self-awareness33 and difficulty with facial affect recognition and theory of mind.19 Disturbances in either capacity described by the General Aggression Model could therefore contribute to MA-related aggression but these links have not been tested directly.
Neurobiologically, aggression is associated with emotion processing circuitry, particularly the amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC).34 Whereas the amygdala mediates rapid, automatic responses to social stimuli,35,36 especially emotional facial expressions,37,38 the PFC mediates the more deliberative aspects of emotion processing,39 with its ventral sectors implicated in semantic processing and integration of emotional information40-42 as well as response selection and behavior control.43 The PFC can modulate amygdala activity through direct and indirect connections,44-46 and aggressive behavior relies on the integrity of this connectivity. Low PFC activity, high amygdala activity, and disruption of their connections have been linked to aggressive behavior in violent and psychiatric populations,47-53 and healthy individuals performing emotion regulation tasks including restraint from aggression54 exhibit PFC activation, reduced amygdala activity,55-61 and lowered markers of physiological arousal and subjective distress.62-64 These studies have consistently demonstrated involvement of the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), often on the right side,55,65,66 which contributes to inhibitory control.67
Individuals who abuse methamphetamine show abnormalities in this circuitry, suggesting a link between neurobiological deficits and their propensity for aggression. In the PFC (particularly the IFG68), numerous structural, neurochemical, and metabolic differences have been identified,69,70 and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) has uncovered deficits in PFC activation during cognitive27,29,71,72 and socioemotional tasks.18,73 Examination of subcortical regions has also uncovered MA-related neurochemical and metabolic abnormalities in the amygdala.20,74,75 These neurobiological differences have been linked to moods, psychiatric states, and personality traits that can influence aggression70,74-78 and, in one study, related to aggression itself.20 However, no study has directly linked functional differences to emotion processing and aggression.
To address this issue, we previously conducted an fMRI study investigating neural responses to emotional facial expressions in MA-dependent individuals.18 Surprisingly, the study found no difference between MA-dependent and control participants in amygdala response but revealed activation differences in the right IFG. Because one of the roles ascribed to the right IFG is inhibitory control, including control over emotional responses,79 we reasoned that the IFG finding may relate to emotion dysregulation in the MA group. However, because the task did not assess emotion regulation directly, it was not possible to test this hypothesis. The study presented here, therefore, extended the task to include such a condition.
The added task condition (affect labeling) involves verbal labeling of emotional facial expressions, which, unlike the previously used visual matching condition (affect matching), requires symbolic representation of affect. In healthy individuals, affect labeling produces neural activation patterns that are consistent with emotion regulation (ie, increased right IFG and lowered amygdala activity55-57) and is accompanied by decreased markers of negative emotion.57,80 “Putting feelings into words,” therefore, incidentally recruits PFC resources whose activity can influence the amygdala, thereby regulating those feelings.81
This study used fMRI to investigate the integrity of the PFC-amygdala circuit in MA-dependent and control participants and used self-reported and behavioral measures to relate brain function to aggression and associated traits. Specific objectives of the study were to (1) quantify and compare aggression in MA-dependent and control participants, (2) determine whether the previously observed difference in right-sided IFG activation18 reflects a deficit in emotion regulation, and (3) investigate how these activation patterns relate to aggression.
Participants and study procedure
All procedures were approved by the UCLA Office for the Protection of Research Subjects. Individuals who used MA but were not seeking treatment (MA group) and healthy control individuals (control group) between the ages of 18 and 55 years were recruited using radio, internet, and newspaper advertisements. Following an explanation of the study, participants gave written informed consent and were screened for eligibility using questionnaires, psychiatric diagnostic interviewing (Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders82), and a medical examination. Participants in the MA group were required to meet DSM-IV criteria for current MA dependence and to demonstrate recent MA use by providing a urine sample that tested positive. Exclusion criteria for all participants were any current axis I diagnosis other than MA dependence or substance-induced mood or anxiety disorder in the MA group or nicotine abuse/dependence in both groups; use of psychotropic medications or substances, except some marijuana or alcohol (not qualifying for abuse or dependence); central nervous system, cardiovascular, pulmonary, or systemic disease; human immunodeficiency virus, severe hepatic impairment, hematocrit lower than 32, prostatic hypertrophy, or chronic inflammation; pregnancy; lack of English fluency; and MRI contraindications.
Eligible MA-dependent participants were admitted to the UCLA General Clinical Research Center and participated on a residential basis for 15 to 31 days. They were required to abstain from MA for the duration of the study, verified by urine screening, and no testing occurred in the first 4 days to allow residual MA to clear. Measures presented here were collected over the first 15 days, with the order slightly varied for each participant. Control participants visited the laboratory only on test days and were required to provide urine samples on each test day that tested negative for illicit substances. On completion of the study, participants were compensated with cash, gift certificates, and vouchers.
A total of 76 individuals (39 MA-dependent participants, 37 controls) participated in the study. Owing to subject attrition, late addition of measures to the study protocol, and inconsistencies in data collection, not all participants completed all measures. In addition, fMRI runs were discarded for excessive head movement, problems acquiring behavioral data, chance performance on the task, claustrophobic reaction, or missing structural data. Of the participants with acceptable fMRI data, 21 (11 MA-dependent participants, 10 controls) had participated in a previously described study,18 while 48 (25 MA-dependent participants, 23 controls) performed an updated version of the task (described below).
The Affect Matching/Labeling Task55,56 is a visual match-to-sample task using face stimuli83 and is designed to elicit characteristic PFC and amygdala activation patterns during each of 3 conditions: affect match, affect label, and shape match (Figure 1A).
Competitive Reaction Time Task
The Competitive Reaction Time Task (CRT84) is a measure of perpetrated aggression, operationalized as the amount of aversive noise to which a participant is willing to subject another person. It captures one of the hallmark features of aggression: delivery of a noxious stimulus to a victim with the intent and expectation of harming the victim.85 External, convergent, discriminant, and construct validity of the task have been established in previous studies.85-87
In this task (Figure 1B), participants believed they were competing against another person in a reaction time game (pressing a button faster than their opponent following a “go” cue) and that the loser of each trial would be subjected to a noise blast selected by the winner of the trial. In reality, opponent responses were computer controlled. Noise settings selected by the participant for delivery to the opponent on each trial were the outcome measure. Participants were debriefed immediately following completion of the task.
For this 34-item paper-and-pencil questionnaire,88 participants indicated how much each item reflected their behavior on a 5-point scale.
State Trait Anger Expression Inventory
For the paper-and-pencil State Trait Anger Expression Inventory (STAXI),89 composed of 3 scales (state anger, 15 items; trait anger, 10 items; anger expression, 32 items), participants indicated agreement with each item on a 4-point scale.
Toronto Alexithymia Scale
For the 20-item, paper-and-pencil Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS),90 participants rated their agreement with each item on a 5-point scale, yielding 3 measures: difficulty identifying feelings, difficulty describing feelings, and externally oriented thinking.
Methamphetamine Withdrawal Questionnaire
This 30-item, rater-scored questionnaire, described in detail elsewhere,91 is an adaptation of the Amphetamine Withdrawal Questionnaire.92 Participants in the MA group indicated the severity of functional, emotional, and physical withdrawal symptoms on a 4-point scale.
Visual Analog Scale for Craving
Participants in the MA group completed this measure daily, indicating current levels of MA craving on a 15-cm line marked from 0 to 100 in 10-point increments.
Functional MRI was performed on a 3.0 Tesla Siemens Allegra (Erlangen, Germany) using a single-channel head coil. Functional images were acquired using a standard T2*-weighted gradient-echo echo-planar imaging pulse sequence to collect blood oxygen level–dependent signal. Acquisition parameters were time to repetition, 2500 milliseconds; echo time, 28 milliseconds; flip angle, 80°; and matrix, 64 × 64. Each volume consisted of 36 interleaved slices, parallel to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line, with slice thickness of 2.5 mm and a 25% distance factor. Each of 2 functional runs resulted in 210 volumes. T2-weighted and high-resolution T1-weighted (magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient-echo) structural scans were also acquired.
Stimulus displays for the Affect Matching/Labeling task were generated using MacStim software93 on an Apple MacBook computer (Cupertino, California) and presented through magnet-compatible video goggles.94 Responses were registered using a magnet-compatible button box.94 The CRT was performed using the HyperCard version of the program on an Apple MacBook computer, with noise blasts delivered through TDK headphones (Uniondale, New York).
Preprocessing. Functional MRI data were processed using SPM5.95 To correct for head motion (within 3-mm translation or 5° rotation; movement beyond these parameters was exclusionary), functional images were spatially realigned to the mean image of the time series, using a least squares approach and 6-parameter rigid body spatial transformation. Images were then coregistered to individual structural templates to allow for localization of activation and subsequent spatial normalization.
Amygdala Region-of-Interest Analysis. Amygdala regions of interest (ROIs) were drawn on individual magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient-echo images using FSL FIRST software.96 Functional scans were smoothed with a 5-mm Gaussian kernel and masked with the ROIs. Using the MarsBaR toolbox,97 a general linear model was applied at each voxel within the ROIs, containing a regressor for each condition of the task (affect match, shape match, and affect label for the subset completing this condition), and fixation as an implicit baseline. Condition blocks were modeled as boxcar functions, convolved with a hemodynamic response function provided by SPM. After fitting the general linear model, parameter estimates were averaged across all voxels in the ROI, and the resulting values exported for further analysis.
Whole-Brain Analysis. For individual whole-brain analyses, functional images were smoothed with an 8-mm Gaussian kernel, and the general linear model described above was applied at each voxel across the brain. After fitting the model for each participant, the resulting maps of parameter estimates were spatially normalized to a standard template provided by SPM using a 12-parameter affine transformation and passed to a group-level random-effects analysis. The group-level model combined the previously described18 and added sample, and included sample, group, and sex as factors and age and education as covariates of no interest to account for any potentially confounding effects.
Psychophysiological Interaction Analysis. Effective connectivity between IFG and the amygdala was assessed using the psychophysiological interaction function in SPM5. Psychophysiological interaction analysis uses a multiple regression approach to isolate regions showing a differential relationship with a target region depending on psychological context and can be interpreted as the context-specific influence one brain region exerts over another.98,99 In the present study, individual FIRST-generated amygdala ROIs served as the target, conditions of the affect matching/labeling task as the manipulated context, and IFG as a potential source region for connectivity.
For each participant, regressors that modeled amygdala activity, task conditions, and the amygdala × condition interaction were entered into whole-brain multiple regression analysis. Given our a priori hypotheses,55,56 analyses were restricted to IFG using the PickAtlas toolbox.100 After estimating the model for each participant, a linear contrast was specified for a greater inverse relationship with the amygdala during the affect label than affect match condition. The resulting statistical maps were spatially normalized to the standard SPM template and passed to a group-level random effects analysis, with group and sex as factors and age and education as covariates of no interest.
All whole-brain group analyses were assessed at a statistical threshold of P < .005 with a cluster criterion of at least 30 contiguous voxels, offering a good balance between type I and type II error.101
To account for potentially confounding structural differences,68 we examined volumetric information from individual FIRST- generated amygdala ROIs and IFG gray matter volume using voxel-based morphometry.102 For the voxel-based morphometry analysis, individual magnetization-prepared rapid-acquisition gradient-echo images were manually aligned to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure line, segmented into 3 tissue types, spatially normalized to a standard template, modulated to adjust for nonlinear warping, and smoothed using a 12-mm full-width at half-maximum Gaussian kernel. Signal intensity values, representing an index of regional gray matter volume, were then extracted from voxels of interest for further analysis.
The remaining data were analyzed in SPSS 16.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois), using analysis of variance (ANOVA) and regression models. Because we were unable to match the groups for age and education, and aggression and associated neurocircuitry vary with age and sex,103 demographic variables were entered into all analyses as covariates of no interest.
Participant characteristics
Demographic measures are detailed in the eTable 1. The MA and control groups did not differ in sex composition but, on average, MA-dependent participants were older than controls and had completed fewer years of education. Almost all MA-dependent participants but only about half of the controls smoked cigarettes; however, the number of cigarettes per day did not differ between groups among those who smoked. Current alcohol use was low across participants and did not differ between groups. Methamphetamine use characteristics indicated moderately heavy use in the present sample (Table 1). Withdrawal symptoms and cravings tended to decrease between intake and test days but not all differences reached statistical significance (Table 2). Neither MA use nor abstinence measures correlated with outcome measures.
Aggression and trait characteristics
To compare self-reported aggression between groups, we performed univariate ANOVAs on Aggression Questionnaire, STAXI trait anger, and STAXI anger expression scores, with group as a between-subjects factor and demographic variables as covariates of no interest. All tests showed significant differences between groups, with higher scores in MA-dependent than control participants (Table 3).
To compare perpetrated aggression between groups, we examined CRT performance. Noise intensity and duration settings correlated during all blocks (all r > 0.66; all P < .001) and were summed to form a composite score. Repeated-measures ANOVA of these scores, with group as a between-subjects factor and block as a within-subjects factor, showed a significant block × group interaction. Follow-up tests revealed higher scores in MA-dependent than control participants during block 4 (peak provocation) but no significant group differences for trial 1, block 2, or block 3 (Figure 2).
To evaluate group differences in alexithymia, we performed univariate ANOVAs on TAS subscales, with group as a between-subjects factor and demographic variables as covariates. The MA-dependent participants reported more difficulty identifying feelings than controls but no differences in describing feelings or externally oriented thinking (Table 3). Across control participants, TAS difficulty identifying feelings correlated with STAXI trait anger (r = 0.57; P = .009) and anger expression (r = 0.47; P = .04). Across MA-dependent participants, TAS total scores correlated with STAXI anger expression (r = 0.42; P = .04).
Across all participants, the affect match vs shape match contrast showed widespread activation consistent with the neural system for face processing,103,104 including in the bilateral amygdala and IFG (Table 4). Within these regions, t tests comparing groups revealed lower activation in MA-dependent than control participants in bilateral ventral IFG, predominantly on the right (Figure 3A; Table 4). No regions showed greater activation in MA-dependent than control participants.
To account for potential volumetric differences between groups,68 we examined individual gray matter concentration in these ventral IFG clusters using voxel-based morphometry. An ANOVA testing voxel-based morphometry values for group differences showed a trend toward lower gray matter concentration in the MA group (F1,54 = 2.86; P = .10) (in addition to effects of age and sex). To test whether local gray matter concentration influenced task-related ventral IFG activation, we entered gray matter concentration as a covariate into an ANOVA, comparing activation between groups. Activation values (average parameter estimates in ventral IFG clusters) correlated between left and right clusters (r = 0.57; P < .001) and were combined by calculating a cluster-weighted average. The ANOVA showed no effect of gray matter concentration on these values, while the group difference remained (Table 3).
Finally, we investigated amygdala activation for differences between groups. Volume of amygdala ROIs differed by sex but not group (F1,64 = 1.21; P = .28). Left and right amygdala activation values (average parameter estimates across ROI voxels) correlated with one another (r = 0.68; P < .001) and were combined by calculating their average. An ANOVA of these values, with group as a factor and ROI volume as a covariate, revealed no effect of volume or group difference in activation (Tables 2 and 3).
To test the hypothesis that, owing to IFG dysfunction, MA-dependent participants would fail to lower amygdala activity during affect labeling, we performed a repeated-measures ANOVA on amygdala activation values, with group as a between-subjects factor and condition (affect match, affect label, shape match) as a within-subjects factor. Activation values from left and right amygdala ROIs correlated during all task conditions (all r > 0.61; all P < .001), and were combined by calculating the average. The ANOVA showed a significant effect of condition, and follow-up tests revealed that, as predicted, amygdala activity during the affect label condition was lower than during the affect match condition. Activation during the shape match condition was lower than during both conditions involving faces. Contrary to prediction, however, we found no group difference or group × condition interaction (Figure 3B).
To identify brain regions associated with this reduction in amygdala activation, we tested voxels across IFG for a greater inverse relationship with amygdala activity (ie, greater functional connectivity) during affect label than affect match using psychophysiological interaction analysis. Dorsal IFG showed the expected pattern of connectivity, predominantly on the right (Figure 3C, Table 4). The clusters did not overlap with the ventral IFG clusters that showed a group difference during affect matching. Within the dorsal IFG clusters, no voxels differed between MA-dependent and control participants, suggesting successful IFG recruitment and subsequent amygdala regulation in both groups.
Behavioral correlates of decreased amygdala activity
Individual decreases in amygdala activity were calculated as the difference in activation between affect match and affect label conditions. To determine the functional significance of this decrease, values were entered as independent variables into linear regression models, with aggression scores as the outcome variables and demographic measures as covariates of no interest. Self-reported aggression scores were intercorrelated (all r > 0.47; all P < .01) and were combined into a composite score by calculating their average.
The model examining these scores showed a relationship between decreased amygdala activity and self-reported aggression in control but not MA-dependent participants (Figure 4A). However, MA-dependent participants showed a relationship between decreased amygdala activity and CRT scores (Figure 4B). Control participants showed a similar relationship (Figure 4B) that did not reach statistical significance, possibly because of low statistical power owing to the small subsample. When the 2 groups were combined to increase statistical power, decreased amygdala activity, controlling for group, showed a significant inverse relationship with CRT performance (r = −0.45; P = .03).
Behavioral correlates of ventral ifg activity
Because low ventral IFG activity in the MA group (Figure 3A) did not signify emotion dysregulation, we investigated its functional significance using linear regression. Ventral IFG activation (cluster-weighted average of left and right clusters) was entered as the independent variable, with behavioral measures as outcomes and demographic measures as covariates. In controls, ventral IFG activation did not directly relate to aggression but showed a significant inverse relationship with scores on the difficulty identifying feelings subscale of the TAS (Figure 5), suggesting that ventral IFG contributes to emotional insight. In MA-dependent participants, ventral IFG activation did not relate to TAS scores, suggesting a decoupling owing to their functional deficit in this region.
The results are consistent with the view that emotional insight, in addition to emotion regulation, contributes to aggression,21 and that this capacity involves the ventral IFG.105-107 Low ventral IFG activity and associated alexithymia in MA-dependent individuals may therefore precipitate aggression despite successful emotion regulation. Because results were found in early abstinence and did not relate to MA use history or withdrawal, they also suggest that at least some proportion of MA-related aggression is mediated by personality characteristics rather than acute intoxication, withdrawal, or MA use history.
In this study, MA-dependent participants self-reported higher aggression than controls, replicating previous findings20 and confirming descriptions from community samples.1-9 The MA-dependent participants also perpetrated more aggression on the CRT, where, despite similar initial behavior to controls, they escalated aggression more steeply following provocation. These results provide the first laboratory description of MA-related aggression patterns and suggest that aggression occurs as an increasingly disproportionate response to interpersonal interaction, rather than a preemptive attack.
According to the General Aggression Model,21 failure of either emotion regulation or emotional insight can account for such a pattern. Despite our hypotheses' focus on emotion regulation, however, we found no deficit in this capacity in MA-dependent participants, as affect labeling resulted in dorsal IFG recruitment and lowered amygdala activity across participants. These activation patterns related to self-reported aggression in controls and perpetrated aggression in all participants, suggesting that they represent a neural signature for successful emotion regulation55,57 and that this capacity is relevant to the restraint of aggression in both groups.
Instead, poor emotional insight may underlie MA-related aggression. The General Aggression Model states that even in the presence of sufficient cognitive capacity (ie, emotion regulation), behavior can be aggressive if assessment of internal states is unsuccessful. Alexithymia scores in the MA sample support this view, showing greater difficulty identifying feelings, which, in turn, related to self-reported aggression. This finding is consistent with evidence of impaired introspection and social comprehension in drug addiction31,32 and evidence that MA-related hostility results in part from misinterpretation of the world as a hostile and threatening place.108
Importantly, our imaging data suggest that emotional insight relies on the ventral IFG, a region showing dysfunction in MA-dependent participants. During facial affect matching, MA-dependent participants showed low activity in bilateral ventral IFG (not overlapping with the dorsal IFG region implicated in amygdala regulation), while amygdala activation did not differ between groups. These results replicate and extend our previous findings18 and suggest that, while amygdala-dependent automatic reactions to socioemotional cues are comparable with those of healthy individuals, IFG-dependent deliberative processing is compromised. Ventral IFG is implicated in the recognition, representation, and comprehension of emotionally salient information, including the mental and emotional states of oneself and others,40-42,109 and neurocognitive models suggest that its activity can influence behavioral outcomes by modulating hypothalamic fight-or-flight responses following comprehension of socioemotional cues.110,111 The inverse correlation between ventral IFG activity and alexithymia observed in the controls is consistent with this evidence and suggests that low ventral IFG activity (as exhibited by MA-dependent participants) reflects a limited capacity to identify feelings. In line with a previously described relationship between ventral IFG function and harm avoidance/fear in MA-dependent individuals,76 this deficit could diminish the motivation to temper maladaptive interpersonal behavior, thus escalating aggression.
Beyond increasing the likelihood of aggression, the same deficit could also contribute to the unreliable self-reporting observed among MA-dependent participants. The finding that in the MA group, decreased amygdala activity related to perpetrated aggression but not self-report of this aggression suggests that, owing to limited insight, objective tasks characterize their behavior more reliably than subjective self-report.
Together, the data are consistent with theoretical21 and neurocognitive111 models of aggression and suggest that a deficit in the evaluation of internal states rather than insufficient cognitive capacity precipitates MA-related aggression. Amygdala activation during affect matching suggests appropriate immediate responses, and successful amygdala regulation by dorsal IFG during affect labeling suggests sufficient cognitive capacity; however, low ventral IFG activation and associated alexithymia suggest limited evaluation of internal states, thus favoring aggressive outcomes.
Several limitations of the study should be noted. First, we were unable to match MA-dependent and control participants for age, education, psychiatric history, and smoking status, potentially confounding group differences. Although we included demographic covariates in analyses and performed follow-up tests, we recognize that MA-dependent and control participants likely differ in ways other than MA exposure and that these factors need to be distinguished in future studies. Second, MA-related abnormalities in neurovascular coupling or hemodynamic response could have influenced fMRI results. To minimize such effects, the study used a blocked design but this decreased temporal resolution. The surprising lack of a group difference in amygdala activity or regulation could therefore reflect the low temporal resolution of the design (or the low spatial resolution of fMRI) rather than true equivalence in function. Further, amygdala and ventral PFC are susceptible to signal dropout, potentially obscuring the data. Third, decreased amygdala activation between affect match and label conditions could have resulted from factors other than inhibitory control processes such as differences in stimulus parameters or attention. However, Lieberman et al55 have shown that amygdala activity decreases with affect labeling but not perceptual and attentional control conditions, and other studies57,80 have shown associated decreases in subjective measures of emotion, making incidental emotion regulation a plausible interpretation. Finally, although our data suggest that alexithymia is a crucial contributor to MA-related aggression, the possibility that additional trait characteristics (eg, impulsivity, volatile temper, sensation-seeking) mediate this relationship cannot be excluded.
These limitations notwithstanding, the study adds important neurobiological components to the examination of aggression in MA dependence. The findings suggest that emotion regulation, at least when elicited incidentally, can be successful in MA-dependent individuals but that dysfunction of ventral IFG contributes to heightened aggression by limiting emotional insight. In the continued pursuit of intervention strategies focused on stress-related relapse prevention and improved personal and social function, future studies may therefore benefit from taking these socioemotional considerations into account.
Correspondence: Edythe D. London, PhD, UCLA Semel Institute, 740 Westwood Plaza, Room C8-528, Los Angeles, CA 90095 (elondon@mednet.ucla.edu).
Submitted for Publication: January 19, 2010; final revision received June 16, 2010; accepted August 25, 2010.
Published Online: November 1, 2010. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.154
Author Contributions: Dr London takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of analyses. Drs London and Payer had full access to the data in the study.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by National Institutes of Health grants R01 DA020726, R01 DA015179, P20 DA022539 (Dr London) and R01 MH084116 (Dr Lieberman); individual fellowship F31 DA025422 (Dr Payer); Guggenheim Grant 20070111 (Dr Lieberman); institutional training grants T90 DA022768, T32 DA024635, and M01 RR00865 (UCLA General Clinical Research Center); endowments from the Katherine K. and Thomas P. Pike Chair in Addiction Studies; and the Marjorie Green Family Trust (Dr London).
Disclaimer: The funding sources had no role in the design or conduct of the study, collection, management, analysis, or interpretation of the data, or preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.
Previous Presentations: This study was presented in part at the annual meetings of the Organization for Human Brain Mapping; June 18-23, 2009; San Francisco, California; and the Society for Neuroscience; October 17-21, Chicago, Illinois.
Additional Information: A subset of the sample described in this article was used in a previous publication.18
Additional Contributions: The authors thank Todd Zorick, MD, PhD, for clinical oversight of the study; Catherine Sugar, PhD, for statistical advice; Sarah Wilson, MA, for coordination of the study; Angelica Morales for helpful comments and contribution of voxel-based morphometry data; Kristina Mouzakis and Greg Shipman for database support; and Christine Baker, Clayton Clement, Natalie DeShetler, Bahar Ebrat, Lisa Giragosian, Tom Hanson, MA, Lindsay King, Nathasha Moallem, Brittany Sumerel, and Mary Walker Susselman, CNMT, RT(N)(MR), for participant recruitment, screening, and retention. We also thank 3 anonymous reviewers for their helpful comments.
1.Logan
BKFligner
CLHaddix
T Cause and manner of death in fatalities involving methamphetamine.
J Forensic Sci 1998;43
(1)
28- 34
PubMedGoogle Scholar 2.Maxwell
JC Emerging research on methamphetamine.
Curr Opin Psychiatry 2005;18
(3)
235- 242
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Szuster
RR Methamphetamine in psychiatric emergencies.
Hawaii Med J 1990;49
(10)
389- 391
PubMedGoogle Scholar 4.Swanson
SMSise
CBSise
MJSack
DIHolbrook
TLPaci
GM The scourge of methamphetamine: impact on a level I trauma center.
J Trauma 2007;63
(3)
531- 537
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Tominaga
GTGarcia
GDzierba
AWong
J Toll of methamphetamine on the trauma system.
Arch Surg 2004;139
(8)
844- 847
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Cartier
JFarabee
DPrendergast
ML Methamphetamine use, self-reported violent crime, and recidivism among offenders in California who abuse substances.
J Interpers Violence 2006;21
(4)
435- 445
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Cohen
JBDickow
AHorner
KZweben
JEBalabis
JVandersloot
DReiber
CMethamphetamine Treatment Project, Abuse and violence history of men and women in treatment for methamphetamine dependence.
Am J Addict 2003;12
(5)
377- 385
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.McKetin
RMcLaren
JLubman
DIHides
L Hostility among methamphetamine users experiencing psychotic symptoms.
Am J Addict 2008;17
(3)
235- 240
PubMedGoogle Scholar 9.Zweben
JECohen
JBChristian
DGalloway
GPSalinardi
MParent
DIguchi
MMethamphetamine Treatment Project, Psychiatric symptoms in methamphetamine users.
Am J Addict 2004;13
(2)
181- 190
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.Gonzales
RMooney
LRawson
RA The methamphetamine problem in the United States.
Annu Rev Public Health 2010;31385- 398
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Watanabe-Galloway
SRyan
SHansen
KHullsiek
BMuli
VMalone
AC Effects of methamphetamine abuse beyond individual users.
J Psychoactive Drugs 2009;41
(3)
241- 248
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Boles
SMMiotto
K Substance abuse and violence: a review of the literature.
Aggress Violent Behav 2003;8
(2)
155- 17410.1016/S1359-1789(01)00057-X
Google Scholar 13.Jaffe
APedersen
WCFisher
DG
et al. Drug use, personality and partner violence: a model of separate, additive, contributions in an active drug user sample.
Open Addict J 2009;239- 47
Google Scholar 14.Stretesky
PB National case-control study of homicide offending and methamphetamine use.
J Interpers Violence 2009;24
(6)
911- 924
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.Tyner
EAFremouw
WJ The relation of methamphetamine use and violence: a critical review.
Aggress Violent Behav 2008;13
(4)
285- 297doi:10.1016/j.avb.2008.04.005
Google Scholar 16.Baskin-Sommers
ASommers
I The co-occurrence of substance use and high-risk behaviors.
J Adolesc Health 2006;38
(5)
609- 611
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Sommers
IBaskin
D Methamphetamine use and violence.
J Drug Issues 2006;36
(1)
77- 96
Google Scholar 18.Payer
DELieberman
MDMonterosso
JRXu
JFong
TWLondon
ED Differences in cortical activity between methamphetamine-dependent and healthy individuals performing a facial affect matching task.
Drug Alcohol Depend 2008;93
(1-2)
93- 102
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Henry
JDMazur
MRendell
PG Social-cognitive difficulties in former users of methamphetamine.
Br J Clin Psychol 2009;48
(Pt 3)
323- 327
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Sekine
YOuchi
YTakei
NYoshikawa
ENakamura
KFutatsubashi
MOkada
HMinabe
YSuzuki
KIwata
YTsuchiya
KJTsukada
HIyo
MMori
N Brain serotonin transporter density and aggression in abstinent methamphetamine abusers.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63
(1)
90- 100
PubMedGoogle Scholar 22.Simon
SLDean
ACCordova
XMonterosso
JRLondon
ED Methamphetamine dependence and neuropsychological functioning: evaluating change during early abstinence.
J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2010;71
(3)
335- 344
PubMedGoogle Scholar 23.Scott
JCWoods
SPMatt
GEMeyer
RAHeaton
RKAtkinson
JHGrant
I Neurocognitive effects of methamphetamine: a critical review and meta-analysis.
Neuropsychol Rev 2007;17
(3)
275- 297
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Salo
RNordahl
TEPossin
KLeamon
MGibson
DRGalloway
GPFlynn
NMHenik
APfefferbaum
ASullivan
EV Preliminary evidence of reduced cognitive inhibition in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.
Psychiatry Res 2002;111
(1)
65- 74
PubMedGoogle Scholar 25.Salo
RNordahl
TEMoore
CWaters
CNatsuaki
YGalloway
GPKile
SSullivan
EV A dissociation in attentional control: evidence from methamphetamine dependence.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;57
(3)
310- 313
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.Monterosso
JRAron
ARCordova
XXu
JLondon
ED Deficits in response inhibition associated with chronic methamphetamine abuse.
Drug Alcohol Depend 2005;79
(2)
273- 277
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Salo
RUrsu
SBuonocore
MHLeamon
MHCarter
C Impaired prefrontal cortical function and disrupted adaptive cognitive control in methamphetamine abusers: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry 2009;65
(8)
706- 709
PubMedGoogle Scholar 28.Paulus
MPHozack
NFrank
LBrown
GGSchuckit
MA Decision making by methamphetamine-dependent subjects is associated with error-rate-independent decrease in prefrontal and parietal activation.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;53
(1)
65- 74
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Monterosso
JDomier
CAinslie
GXu
JCordova
XLondon
ED Frontoparietal cortical activity of methamphetamine-dependent and comparison subjects performing a parametric delay-discounting task.
Hum Brain Mapp 2007;28
(5)
383- 393doi:10.1002/hbm.20281
Google Scholar 30.Hoffman
WFMoore
MTemplin
RMcFarland
BHitzemann
RJMitchell
SH Neuropsychological function and delay discounting in methamphetamine-dependent individuals.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2006;188
(2)
162- 170
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Goldstein
RZCraig
ADBechara
AGaravan
HChildress
ARPaulus
MPVolkow
ND The neurocircuitry of impaired insight in drug addiction.
Trends Cogn Sci 2009;13
(9)
372- 380
PubMedGoogle Scholar 32.Homer
BDSolomon
TMMoeller
RWMascia
ADeRaleau
LHalkitis
PN Methamphetamine abuse and impairment of social functioning: a review of the underlying neurophysiological causes and behavioral implications.
Psychol Bull 2008;134
(2)
301- 310
PubMedGoogle Scholar 33.Verdejo-García
APérez-García
M Substance abusers' self-awareness of the neurobehavioral consequences of addiction.
Psychiatry Res 2008;158
(2)
172- 180
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.Davidson
RJPutnam
KMLarson
CL Dysfunction in the neural circuitry of emotion regulation: a possible prelude to violence.
Science 2000;289
(5479)
591- 594
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Adolphs
R Is the human amygdala specialized for processing social information?
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2003;985326- 340
PubMedGoogle Scholar 37.Costafreda
SGBrammer
MJDavid
ASFu
CH Predictors of amygdala activation during the processing of emotional stimuli: a meta-analysis of 385 PET and fMRI studies.
Brain Res Rev 2008;58
(1)
57- 70
PubMedGoogle Scholar 38.Sergerie
KChochol
CArmony
JL The role of the amygdala in emotional processing: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32
(4)
811- 830
PubMedGoogle Scholar 39.Adolphs
R Recognizing emotion from facial expressions: psychological and neurological mechanisms.
Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 2002;1
(1)
21- 62doi:10.1177/1534582302001001003
Google Scholar 40.Sprengelmeyer
RRausch
MEysel
UTPrzuntek
H Neural structures associated with recognition of facial expressions of basic emotions.
Proc Biol Sci 1998;265
(1409)
1927- 1931
PubMedGoogle Scholar 41.Hornak
JRolls
ETWade
D Face and voice expression identification in patients with emotional and behavioural changes following ventral frontal lobe damage.
Neuropsychologia 1996;34
(4)
247- 261
PubMedGoogle Scholar 42.Lange
KWilliams
LMYoung
AWBullmore
ETBrammer
MJWilliams
SCGray
JAPhillips
ML Task instructions modulate neural responses to fearful facial expressions.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;53
(3)
226- 232
PubMedGoogle Scholar 43.Posamentier
MTAbdi
H Processing faces and facial expressions.
Neuropsychol Rev 2003;13
(3)
113- 143
PubMedGoogle Scholar 44.Barbas
H Flow of information for emotions through temporal and orbitofrontal pathways.
J Anat 2007;211
(2)
237- 249
PubMedGoogle Scholar 45.Petrides
MPandya
DN Efferent association pathways from the rostral prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey.
J Neurosci 2007;27
(43)
11573- 11586
PubMedGoogle Scholar 46.Quirk
GJBeer
JS Prefrontal involvement in the regulation of emotion: convergence of rat and human studies.
Curr Opin Neurobiol 2006;16
(6)
723- 727
PubMedGoogle Scholar 47.Woermann
FGvan Elst
LTKoepp
MJFree
SLThompson
PJTrimble
MRDuncan
JS Reduction of frontal neocortical grey matter associated with affective aggression in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: an objective voxel by voxel analysis of automatically segmented MRI.
J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2000;68
(2)
162- 169
PubMedGoogle Scholar 48.Raine
ABuchsbaum
MLaCasse
L Brain abnormalities in murderers indicated by positron emission tomography.
Biol Psychiatry 1997;42
(6)
495- 508
PubMedGoogle Scholar 49.Raine
AMeloy
JRBihrle
SStoddard
JLaCasse
LBuchsbaum
MS Reduced prefrontal and increased subcortical brain functioning assessed using positron emission tomography in predatory and affective murderers.
Behav Sci Law 1998;16
(3)
319- 332
PubMedGoogle Scholar 50.Bufkin
JLLuttrell
VR Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior: current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice.
Trauma Violence Abuse 2005;6
(2)
176- 191
PubMedGoogle Scholar 51.van Elst
LTWoermann
FGLemieux
LThompson
PJTrimble
MR Affective aggression in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy: a quantitative MRI study of the amygdala.
Brain 2000;123
(pt 2)
234- 243
PubMedGoogle Scholar 52.Coccaro
EFMcCloskey
MSFitzgerald
DAPhan
KL Amygdala and orbitofrontal reactivity to social threat in individuals with impulsive aggression.
Biol Psychiatry 2007;62
(2)
168- 178
PubMedGoogle Scholar 53.New
ASHazlett
EABuchsbaum
MSGoodman
MMitelman
SANewmark
RTrisdorfer
RHaznedar
MMKoenigsberg
HWFlory
JSiever
LJ Amygdala-prefrontal disconnection in borderline personality disorder.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2007;32
(7)
1629- 1640
PubMedGoogle Scholar 54.Pietrini
PGuazzelli
MBasso
GJaffe
KGrafman
J Neural correlates of imaginal aggressive behavior assessed by positron emission tomography in healthy subjects.
Am J Psychiatry 2000;157
(11)
1772- 1781
PubMedGoogle Scholar 55.Lieberman
MDEisenberger
NICrockett
MJTom
SMPfeifer
JHWay
BM Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli.
Psychol Sci 2007;18
(5)
421- 428
PubMedGoogle Scholar 56.Hariri
ARBookheimer
SYMazziotta
JC Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system.
Neuroreport 2000;11
(1)
43- 48
PubMedGoogle Scholar 57.Hariri
ARMattay
VSTessitore
AFera
FWeinberger
DR Neocortical modulation of the amygdala response to fearful stimuli.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;53
(6)
494- 501
PubMedGoogle Scholar 58.Goldin
PRMcRae
KRamel
WGross
JJ The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.
Biol Psychiatry 2008;63
(6)
577- 586
PubMedGoogle Scholar 59.Phan
KLFitzgerald
DANathan
PJMoore
GJUhde
TWTancer
ME Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;57
(3)
210- 219
PubMedGoogle Scholar 60.Schaefer
SMJackson
DCDavidson
RJAguirre
GKKimberg
DYThompson-Schill
SL Modulation of amygdalar activity by the conscious regulation of negative emotion.
J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14
(6)
913- 921
PubMedGoogle Scholar 61.Beauregard
MLévesque
JBourgouin
P Neural correlates of conscious self-regulation of emotion.
J Neurosci 2001;21
(18)
RC165
PubMedGoogle Scholar 62.Urry
HLvan Reekum
CMJohnstone
TKalin
NHThurow
MESchaefer
HSJackson
CAFrye
CJGreischar
LLAlexander
ALDavidson
RJ Amygdala and ventromedial prefrontal cortex are inversely coupled during regulation of negative affect and predict the diurnal pattern of cortisol secretion among older adults.
J Neurosci 2006;26
(16)
4415- 4425
PubMedGoogle Scholar 64.Driscoll
DTranel
DAnderson
SW The effects of voluntary regulation of positive and negative emotion on psychophysiological responsiveness.
Int J Psychophysiol 2009;72
(1)
61- 66
PubMedGoogle Scholar 65.Wager
TDDavidson
MLHughes
BLLindquist
MAOchsner
KN Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation.
Neuron 2008;59
(6)
1037- 1050
PubMedGoogle Scholar 66.Ochsner
KNRay
RDCooper
JCRobertson
ERChopra
SGabrieli
JDGross
JJ For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion.
Neuroimage 2004;23
(2)
483- 499
PubMedGoogle Scholar 67.Aron
ARRobbins
TWPoldrack
RA Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex.
Trends Cogn Sci 2004;8
(4)
170- 177
PubMedGoogle Scholar 68.Thompson
PMHayashi
KMSimon
SLGeaga
JAHong
MSSui
YLee
JYToga
AWLing
WLondon
ED Structural abnormalities in the brains of human subjects who use methamphetamine.
J Neurosci 2004;24
(26)
6028- 6036
PubMedGoogle Scholar 69.Payer
DLondon
ED Methamphetamine and the brain: findings from brain imaging studies. JM
RollIn:eds.Methamphetamine Addiction: From Basic Science To Treatment. New York, NY Guildford Press2009;
70.Baicy
KLondon
ED Corticolimbic dysregulation and chronic methamphetamine abuse.
Addiction 2007;102(Suppl 1)5- 15
PubMedGoogle Scholar 71.Aron
JLPaulus
MP Location, location: using functional magnetic resonance imaging to pinpoint brain differences relevant to stimulant use.
Addiction 2007;102(suppl 1)33- 43
PubMedGoogle Scholar 72.Hoffman
WFSchwartz
DLHuckans
MSMcFarland
BHMeiri
GStevens
AAMitchell
SH Cortical activation during delay discounting in abstinent methamphetamine dependent individuals.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2008;201
(2)
183- 193
PubMedGoogle Scholar 73.Kim
YTLee
JJSong
HJKim
JHKwon
DHKim
MNYoo
DSLee
HJKim
HJChang
Y Alterations in cortical activity of male methamphetamine abusers performing an empathy task: fMRI study.
Hum Psychopharmacol 2010;25
(1)
63- 70
PubMedGoogle Scholar 74.Sekine
YMinabe
YOuchi
YTakei
NIyo
MNakamura
KSuzuki
KTsukada
HOkada
HYoshikawa
EFutatsubashi
MMori
N Association of dopamine transporter loss in the orbitofrontal and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices with methamphetamine-related psychiatric symptoms.
Am J Psychiatry 2003;160
(9)
1699- 1701
PubMedGoogle Scholar 75.London
EDSimon
SLBerman
SMMandelkern
MALichtman
AMBramen
JShinn
AKMiotto
KLearn
JDong
YMatochik
JAKurian
VNewton
TWoods
RRawson
RLing
W Mood disturbances and regional cerebral metabolic abnormalities in recently abstinent methamphetamine abusers.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61
(1)
73- 84
PubMedGoogle Scholar 76.Goldstein
RZVolkow
NDChang
LWang
GJFowler
JSDepue
RAGur
RC The orbitofrontal cortex in methamphetamine addiction: involvement in fear.
Neuroreport 2002;13
(17)
2253- 2257
PubMedGoogle Scholar 77.Li
CSSinha
R Inhibitory control and emotional stress regulation: neuroimaging evidence for frontal-limbic dysfunction in psycho-stimulant addiction.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2008;32
(3)
581- 597
PubMedGoogle Scholar 78.Chang
LAlicata
DErnst
TVolkow
N Structural and metabolic brain changes in the striatum associated with methamphetamine abuse.
Addiction 2007;102(suppl 1)16- 32
PubMedGoogle Scholar 79.Berkman
ETLieberman
MD Using neuroscience to broaden emotion regulation: theoretical and methodological considerations.
Soc Pers Psychol Compass 2009;3
(4)
475- 493doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2009.00186.x
Google Scholar 80.Tabibnia
GLieberman
MDCraske
MG The lasting effect of words on feelings: words may facilitate exposure effects to threatening images.
Emotion 2008;8
(3)
307- 317
PubMedGoogle Scholar 81.Esterling
BAL’Abate
LMurray
EJPennebaker
JW Empirical foundations for writing in prevention and psychotherapy: mental and physical health outcomes.
Clin Psychol Rev 1999;19
(1)
79- 96
PubMedGoogle Scholar 82.First
MBSpitzer
RLGibbon
MWilliams
JBW The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-IP). Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1995;
83.Tottenham
NTanaka
JWLeon
ACMcCarry
TNurse
MHare
TAMarcus
DJWesterlund
ACasey
BJNelson
C The NimStim set of facial expressions: judgments from untrained research participants.
Psychiatry Res 2009;168
(3)
242- 249
PubMedGoogle Scholar 84.Bushman
BJBaumeister
RF Threatened egotism, narcissism, self-esteem, and direct and displaced aggression: does self-love or self-hate lead to violence?
J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;75
(1)
219- 229
PubMedGoogle Scholar 85.Anderson
CABushman
BJ External validity of “trivial” experiments: the case of laboratory aggression.
Rev Gen Psychol 1997;1
(1)
19- 41doi:10.1037/1089-2680.1.1.19
Google Scholar 86.Bernstein
SRichardson
DHammock
G Convergent and discriminant validity of the Taylor and Buss measures of physical aggression.
Aggress Behav 1987;13
(1)
15- 24doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1987)13:1<15::AID-AB2480130104>3.0.CO;2-K
Google Scholar 87.Giancola
PRZeichner
A An investigation of gender differences in alcohol-related aggression.
J Stud Alcohol 1995;56
(5)
573- 579
PubMedGoogle Scholar 88.Buss
AHWarren
WL Aggression Questionnaire: Manual. Los Angeles, CA Western Psychological Services2000;
89.Spielberger
CD Manual for the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory. Odessa, FL Psychological Assessment Resources1988;
90.Bagby
RMTaylor
GJParker
JD The Twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale–II: convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity.
J Psychosom Res 1994;38
(1)
33- 40
PubMedGoogle Scholar 91.Zorick
TNestor
LMiotto
KSugar
CHellemann
GScanlon
GRawson
RLondon
ED Withdrawal symptoms in abstinent methamphetamine-dependent subjects.
Addiction 2010;105
(10)
1809- 1818
PubMedGoogle Scholar 92.Srisurapanont
MJarusuraisin
NJittiwutikan
J Amphetamine withdrawal I: reliability, validity and factor structure of a measure.
Aust N Z J Psychiatry 1999;33
(1)
89- 93
PubMedGoogle Scholar 96.Patenaude
B Bayesian Statistical Models of Shape and Appearance for Subcortical Brain Segmentation. Oxford, England University of Oxford2007;
97.Brett
MAnton
JLValabregue
RPoline
JB Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox.
Neuroimage 2002;16
(2)
1140- 1141
Google Scholar 98.Friston
KJBuechel
CFink
GRMorris
JRolls
EDolan
RJ Psychophysiological and modulatory interactions in neuroimaging.
Neuroimage 1997;6
(3)
218- 229
PubMedGoogle Scholar 99.Gitelman
DRPenny
WDAshburner
JFriston
KJ Modeling regional and psychophysiologic interactions in fMRI: the importance of hemodynamic deconvolution.
Neuroimage 2003;19
(1)
200- 207
PubMedGoogle Scholar 100.Maldjian
JALaurienti
PJKraft
RABurdette
JH An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets.
Neuroimage 2003;19
(3)
1233- 1239
PubMedGoogle Scholar 101.Lieberman
MDCunningham
WA Type I and Type II error concerns in fMRI research: re-balancing the scale.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2009;4
(4)
423- 428
PubMedGoogle Scholar 102.Ashburner
JFriston
KJ Voxel-based morphometry: the methods.
Neuroimage 2000;11
(6 Pt 1)
805- 821
PubMedGoogle Scholar 103.Fusar-Poli
PPlacentino
ACarletti
FLandi
PAllen
PSurguladze
SBenedetti
FAbbamonte
MGasparotti
RBarale
FPerez
JMcGuire
PPoliti
P Functional atlas of emotional faces processing: a voxel-based meta-analysis of 105 functional magnetic resonance imaging studies.
J Psychiatry Neurosci 2009;34
(6)
418- 432
PubMedGoogle Scholar 104.Haxby
JVHoffman
EAGobbini
MI Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication.
Biol Psychiatry 2002;51
(1)
59- 67
PubMedGoogle Scholar 105.Reker
MOhrmann
PRauch
AVKugel
HBauer
JDannlowski
UArolt
VHeindel
WSuslow
T Individual differences in alexithymia and brain response to masked emotion faces.
Cortex 2010;46
(5)
658- 667
PubMedGoogle Scholar 106.Kano
MFukudo
SGyoba
JKamachi
MTagawa
MMochizuki
HItoh
MHongo
MYanai
K Specific brain processing of facial expressions in people with alexithymia: an H2 15O-PET study.
Brain 2003;126
(Pt 6)
1474- 1484
PubMedGoogle Scholar 107.Bermond
BVorst
HCMoormann
PP Cognitive neuropsychology of alexithymia: implications for personality typology.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry 2006;11
(3)
332- 360
PubMedGoogle Scholar 108.Lapworth
KDawe
SDavis
PKavanagh
DYoung
RSaunders
J Impulsivity and positive psychotic symptoms influence hostility in methamphetamine users.
Addict Behav 2009;34
(4)
380- 385
PubMedGoogle Scholar 109.Shamay-Tsoory
SGTomer
RBerger
BDGoldsher
DAharon-Peretz
J Impaired “affective theory of mind” is associated with right ventromedial prefrontal damage.
Cogn Behav Neurol 2005;18
(1)
55- 67
PubMedGoogle Scholar 110.Blair
RJCipolotti
L Impaired social response reversal: a case of ‘acquired sociopathy.’
Brain 2000;123
(Pt 6)
1122- 1141
PubMedGoogle Scholar 111.Blair
RJ The roles of orbital frontal cortex in the modulation of antisocial behavior.
Brain Cogn 2004;55
(1)
198- 208
PubMedGoogle Scholar