Context
To our knowledge, there is no published information on the epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) in adults. If the prevalence of autism is increasing, rates in older adults would be expected to be lower than rates among younger adults.
Objective
To estimate the prevalence and characteristics of adults with ASD living in the community in England.
Design
A stratified, multiphase random sample was used in the third national survey of psychiatric morbidity in adults in England in 2007. Survey data were weighted to take account of study design and nonresponse so that the results were representative of the household population.
Setting
General community (ie, private households) in England.
Participants
Adults (people 16 years or older).
Main Outcome Measures
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Module 4 in phase 2 validated against the Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised and Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders in phase 3. A 20-item subset of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient self-completion questionnaire was used in phase 1 to select respondents for phase 2. Respondents also provided information on sociodemographics and their use of mental health services.
Results
Of 7461 adult participants who provided a complete phase 1 interview, 618 completed phase 2 diagnostic assessments. The weighted prevalence of ASD in adults was estimated to be 9.8 per 1000 (95% confidence interval, 3.0-16.5). Prevalence was not related to the respondent's age. Rates were higher in men, those without educational qualifications, and those living in rented social (government-financed) housing. There was no evidence of increased use of services for mental health problems.
Conclusions
Conducting epidemiologic research on ASD in adults is feasible. The prevalence of ASD in this population is similar to that found in children. The lack of an association with age is consistent with there having been no increase in prevalence and with its causes being temporally constant. Adults with ASD living in the community are socially disadvantaged and tend to be unrecognized.
Autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) are neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by impairment of reciprocal social interaction and communication and restricted repetitive behaviors.1 They have persisting negative effects on learning and development of independence in adulthood.2 In 2007, the yearly cost to society of each adult with ASD in Great Britain was estimated to be £90 000.3 Adults with ASDs are more likely to be recognized and supported if they also have severe intellectual disability; those with higher levels of functioning tend to be overlooked in the community.4
In childhood, ASDs are associated with intellectual disability and male sex. More recent surveys5-7 report higher prevalence estimates. In children, the median rate in 16 surveys published between 1966 and 1991 was 4.4 per 10 000 population; the median rate in 16 surveys published between 1992 and 2001 was 12.7 per 10 000.6 In 3 recent large regionwide or national community surveys8-10 of children and adolescents in England, the prevalence of ASD was approximately 10 per 1000. It is not known whether this reported increased prevalence reflects case finding changes or increasing incidence due to newly emerging causes. Among intellectually disabled adults (<0.5% of the overall adult population), a rate of 75 per 1000 was obtained from an intellectual disability case register11 that incorporated identification from direct observation, detailed case records, and interviews with caregivers. Although adults with ASD have been studied across a range of age groups,12
there is no information about community prevalence across the age range in that population. Adults in Great Britain who have responded to postal and online surveys13 stating that they have ASD are more often male (2:1), rarely 65 years or older, and rarely in full-time employment. They tend to have been given a diagnosis of high-functioning autism or Asperger syndrome, with only 1 in 5 in receipt of psychological or psychiatric services.
Cases of ASD in surveys of children have been identified using direct observation and collateral descriptions of behavior (from parents and teachers),5-7 techniques that are less feasible in adults. We used a multiple-phase design: an initial screening phase of adults in the community, a direct observation second phase, and developmental interviews of collateral informants in a third phase.
We hypothesized that, in the community, the rate of ASDs in early adulthood would be similar to that reported in older children but that far fewer cases would be found among older adults, particularly those in the retirement age range. We also hypothesized that adults with ASDs would be more likely to be male and disadvantaged socioeconomically and less likely to be receiving support from mental health services than adults with other mental disorders.
The third national Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey in England, carried out in 2007,14 used a stratified 2-phase design based on a random probability sample. Postcode sectors (on average, 2550 households) were stratified on the basis of a measure of socioeconomic status within a regional breakdown (Figure 1). All primary sampling units were then further stratified on the basis of the proportion of adults employed in non–manual labor jobs and sorted by the proportion of households without a motor vehicle based on 2001 UK Census data (http://www.statistics.gov.uk/about/data/methodology/specific/population/PEMethodology/). Next, postal sectors were sampled from each stratum with a probability proportional to size; 519 postal sectors were selected, with 28 delivery points randomly selected in each (yielding 14 532 delivery points). Interviewers visited these to identify private households in England containing at least 1 adult (16 years or older) (Figure 1). Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Royal Free Medical School Research Ethics Committee, London, England.
To estimate the prevalence of ASD among adults in the community, a subset of questions from a self-report autism trait screening questionnaire, the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ),15 was used in phase 1. This tool was used to select participants for a second-phase evaluation using detailed clinical assessments based on module 4 of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Module 4 (ADOS-4).16 The 50-item AQ and a recently published17 factor analytically derived 28-item AQ have been validated cross-culturally.18 For each phase 1 respondent, the probability of selection for a phase 2 diagnostic assessment was calculated from respondents' answers to screening questions in the phase 1 questionnaire14,19 as the highest of 4 disorder-specific probabilities (ASD, psychosis, borderline personality disorder, and antisocial personality disorder).
The higher the score on the phase 1 screens, the greater the likelihood of being selected for phase 2. The selection process for a third validation phase is described in the next section.
We used stepwise logistic regression to develop a 20-item subset of the 50-item AQ, using previously gathered diagnostic clinic and control data.15,19 The 20-item version (AQ-20) was administered to all phase 1 respondents to generate a probability of selection for the detailed phase 2 diagnostic assessment. Phase 1 respondents also completed questionnaires covering physical and mental health, socioeconomic factors, use of mental health services,14 and willingness to take part in further interviews. Predicted verbal IQ (V-IQ; range estimate, 70-130) was derived using the National Adult Reading Test20; V-IQ was the only method for estimating IQ available in the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey, although its validity in community cases of ASD is untested. National Centre for Social Research interviewers conducted fieldwork from October 2006 through December 2007. A few of the selected respondents were incapable of undertaking the interview alone because of mental or physical incapacity. For them, the option of a proxy interview with another member of the household or someone else who knew them well was available (Figure 1). The information collected was insufficient for selection probabilities to be calculated, and respondents interviewed by proxy were therefore ineligible for phase 2 interviews.
Phase 2 ASD assessments were based on the ADOS-4, a face-to-face clinical assessment of current behavior consistent with a diagnosis of ASD. It consists of tests termed presses that evaluate communication, reciprocal social interaction, creativity, imagination, and stereotyped and restricted interests. The ADOS-4 incorporates algorithms for ASD,21 applied to selected ADOS-4 ratings corresponding to DSM-IV22 criteria for pervasive developmental disorder. Phase 2 interviewers were experienced in psychological research and received an induction and training program, run by a senior research psychologist (J.S.), a psychiatrist (T.S.B.), and a qualified ADOS-4 trainer (F.S.). Training experience was gained through assessing adults living in the community, including students, and working age and older adults who had a clinician-determined diagnosis of an ASD such as Asperger syndrome. Interviewing in the field did not commence until the 4 interviewers achieved at least 90% agreement on ratings of jointly observed ADOS-4 examinations. During fieldwork, interviewers received supervision sessions and prepared ASD case vignette reports. They took part in debriefing after the interviews to add further contextual information.
A threshold of 10 or greater on the ADOS-4 total score is recommended for identifying cases of autism,21 provided it includes scores of at least 3 on the Communication domain and at least 6 on Reciprocal Social Interaction. To our knowledge, for the first time in a community sample and in older adults,19 we validated this threshold for determining a case of ASD using the ADOS-4, with 54 assessments carried out by the senior research psychologist in a third study phase (Figure 1). These assessments included developmental interviews with family members or other suitable informants of second-phase respondents, half of whom had a high probability of having ASD. The Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised23 and the Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders24 were administered. The appropriateness of using a threshold of 10 or greater was tested using models based on predicting cases of ASD from these 2 tools (details of these assessments and of clinical consensus diagnoses will be reported separately).
Survey data were weighted to take account of study design and nonresponse so that the results were representative in terms of age, sex, region, and area characteristics of the household population 16 years or older in England.14 First, sample weights were applied to take account of the different probabilities of selecting respondents in different-sized households. Second, to reduce household nonresponse bias, a household-level weight was calculated from a logistic regression model using interviewer observation and area-level variables (using 2001 UK Census data) available for responding and nonresponding households. The nonresponse weight for each household was calculated as the inverse of the probability of response estimated from the model multiplied by the household's selection weight. Finally, weights were applied using the techniques of calibration weighting based on age, sex, and region to weight the data to represent the structure of the national population, by taking account of differential nonresponse between regions and age × sex groups. The population control totals used were the Office for National Statistics 2006 midyear household population estimates.25 Phase 2 weights were designed to generate condition-specific phase 2 data sets representative of the population eligible for phase 2 by virtue of that particular condition. Combining the phase 2 weighted data with the phase 1 weighted data for the noneligible group thus gives data representative of the whole adult population. Because incomplete response could bias survey estimates, we investigated possible nonresponse bias: we compared the probability of ASD (phase 1 AQ-20 score) between geographic regions by regional response rate and by each respondent's willingness to take further part in the survey.
Weighted prevalence estimates were determined using an ADOS-4 threshold of 10 or greater (PROC SURVEYFREQ, version 9.1; SAS Institute, Inc, Cary, North Carolina) and for comparison a range of thresholds from 7 or greater to 13 or greater. The distribution of AQ-20 and ADOS-4 scores was examined using histograms. The small number of people identified as having ASD (in terms of the number of ADOS-4 cases found) means that caution is required in interpreting the population distribution of ASD. Variation in prevalence of ASD by other characteristics was examined in univariable and multivariable weighted logistic regression models (PROC SURVEYLOGISTIC, version 9.1; SAS Institute, Inc) and by examining the predicted probability of being a case as a function of continuously measured characteristics (age and V-IQ). The sample for these analyses consisted of phase 1 respondents with a near zero probability of ASD (AQ-20 score <5, classified as non-ASD cases) together with all respondents who completed a full ADOS-4 in phase 2. Phase 1 respondents with an AQ-20 score of 5 to 20 who were not administered a phase 2 assessment were excluded as unknowns and accounted for in the weighting.
Of the 13 171 households identified as potentially eligible in the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey, 4075 refused; 7461 individuals in the households (56.6%) provided a complete phase 1 interview. Of these, 849 people were selected for phase 2 interviews; 62 refused and no contact was made with 157, leaving 630 participants (74.2%) who completed phase 2 assessments (Figure 1). Full ADOS-4 assessments were carried out in 618 participants. Nonresponse bias was investigated by comparing the proportion scoring 13 or greater on the AQ-20 in the 5 regions with the highest response rate and the 6 regions with the lowest response rate in phase 1: it was 1.1% and 1.2%, respectively; using weighted data, it was 1.1% and 1.3%. Refusal to take part by participants selected for a phase 2 interview was 24% overall: the proportion of those who refused and scored 13 or greater on the AQ-20 was 24.3% among individuals with an AQ-20 score of 0 to 12 and 23.3% among those with an AQ-20 score of 13 to 20. Neither comparison gave any indication that survey participation was associated with having ASD. The ADOS-4 threshold of 10 or greater for ASD was supported by models based on predicting cases of ASD from the phase 4 Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders and Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised developmental assessments (details will be reported separately).
Using the validated threshold of a score of 10 or greater on the ADOS-4 to indicate a case of ASD, we determined that the overall prevalence of ASD in the English population of individuals 16 years or older was 9.8 per 1000 population (95% confidence interval [CI], 3.0-16.5). Table 1 includes the unweighted and weighted numbers of cases and weighted estimates for ADOS-4 cut-offs from 7 or greater to 12 or greater. The estimated V-IQ of our ASD cases ranged from 70 (and less) to 100.
The rate of ASD among men (18.2 per 1000; 95% CI, 4.2-32.2) was much greater than among women (2.0 per 1000; 95% CI, 00.0-4.3). This difference was statistically significant in a weighted univariable logistic model (Table 2). Figure 2 depicts the association with age using the weighted prevalence of ASD according to the diagnostic ADOS-4 threshold of 10 or greater. The predicted probability of ASD suggested a gradual decrease with increasing years, although the trend was not significant (odds ratio, 0.99; 95% CI, 0.94-1.04; P = .55); assuming that this is the true odds ratio, for every extra year of age, the odds of ASD would decrease by 1%. The lower limit of the 95% CI suggests a decrease in the odds of 6% with every increasing year of age. In a univariable model, the predicted probability of ASD (ADOS-4 ≥10)
was inversely related to V-IQ score (odds ratio, 0.94; 95% CI, 0.87-0.99; P = .04); for every unit increase in V-IQ score, the odds of ASD decreased by 6.0%.
Having ASD was significantly associated with living in accommodation rented from a social landlord (local authority or housing association) rather than from a private landlord or owned by the participant or other member of the household (Table 2). The presence of ASD was higher in participants without any educational qualifications than in those with university-level qualifications (Table 2).
The presence of ASD was significantly associated with being single (Table 2). A relative preponderance of ASD in more deprived local areas (wards) was close to being statistically significant (P = .06; Table 2). There was no overall association between equivalized household income and the presence of ASD; in participants living in households with the highest income, ASD was less frequent than among those with the lowest incomes, which was close to being statistically significant (P = .06; Table 2). No significant association was identified between household composition and ASD. People who did not know whether they received state benefits had higher odds of having ASD (Table 2); there was insufficient information to determine whether receiving benefits was associated with ASD. Among working-age participants (aged 16-64 years), we found no significant association with economic activity. We had insufficient data to evaluate associations with ethnic groups. Each analysis using the ADOS-4 threshold of 10 or greater in Table 2 was repeated with the cut-off of 7 or greater. The statistically significant associations for sex and tenancy for an ADOS-4 threshold of 10 or greater were also found when the threshold was 7 or greater; those for other associations were also in the same direction but no longer statistically significant.
Because most (15 of 19) of the phase 2 cases with an ADOS-4 threshold of 10 or greater were in male participants, the weighted univariable logistic model (Table 2, males and females combined) was repeated in males only. The findings were unaltered.
Multivariable backward stepwise logistic modeling was performed using the 8 most significant and borderline significant univariable predictors listed in Table 2 (analysis conducted on both sexes), producing 3 significant predictors after removal of nonsignificant variables with the use of backward selection (Table 3). Men had nearly 9 times the odds of having an ASD diagnosis compared with women (P = .002). For every unit increase in the level of education, the odds of ASD decreased by 55% (P = .049). Those in social housing had a significantly higher odds of having ASD compared with those who owned their home (P = .03). Index of Multiple Deprivation, single marital status, receipt of welfare benefits, household income, and V-IQ were not associated (Table 3). Finally, age (continuous) was added to the model. However, its inclusion did not improve the fit of the model (odds ratio, 0.98; 95% CI, 0.94-1.02; P = .36).
Service use for mental or emotional problems was compared between participants with and without ASD. These data are not shown in a table because the base number for people with ASD was too small to make meaningful statistical comparisons (19 cases were identified in the 2007 Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey sample). Nevertheless, there was no evidence of increased use of services for mental or emotional problems by people with ASD in the preceding month or year. None of the cases found in this study had previously been given a formal autism assessment or diagnosis.
Autism spectrum disorder affects approximately 1% of the adult English household population. There was no evidence of a statistically significant reduction in prevalence of ASDs as a function of age. Prevalence was greatest in men, in those living in social housing, and in those with the lowest educational qualifications. Adults with ASD appear to be largely unrecognized. To our knowledge, there are no previous systematic community surveys of adults with which to compare these findings.
The survey was based on the most validated investigator-rated diagnostic instruments available for studying
the prevalence of ASD in adults. However, a number of study limitations should be considered. The number of cases we identified was small, but our sampling strategy, as well as our approach to weighting, means that we could account for the low specificity and sensitivity of our phase 1 screening measure. Because we screened people from across a wide range of AQ-20 score strata, our prevalence estimate is probably reliable. The cost and the burden of including ADOS-4 assessments during phase 1 would have been unsustainable. There is thus a need to develop improved phase 1 screening. Sampling excluded institutional residents and adults with intellectual disability severe enough to prevent them from participating in the assessment. A definitive estimate of prevalence in the whole adult population would require additional sampling in those populations. The association with low intellectual ability in our household sample suggests that our sampling and interviewing methods did not exclude any but the most severely intellectually disabled adults. The 57% response rate in phase 1 was disappointing. Response rates in surveys have been declining in most countries during the past decade.26 However, our estimates were weighted to take account of differences between census data and the profile of the participants. Sensitivity analyses showed no evidence of a nonresponse bias; reluctance to cooperate was not associated with a higher phase 1 AQ-20 score. This finding is supported by recent research showing that nonresponse rate alone is a weak predictor of nonresponse bias and by recent studies suggesting that changes in nonresponse rates do not necessarily alter survey estimates.27 Furthermore, the phase 2 weighting took account of variation in response rate by phase 1 AQ-20 score, age, and sex. Bias in prevalence estimation can be eliminated if an assumption of missingness at random within these adjustment cells is justified.28 In a missingness-at-random situation, missingness depends on demographic or other characteristics that can be adjusted for using weighting. If, after such adjustment has been made, missingness is not related to the survey outcome, missing data can be said to be missing at random. Given our sensitivity analyses and the absence of any other information to the contrary, it is plausible that within age, sex, region, and AQ-20 score groups (the variables upon which our weighting was based), our missing data situation approximates reasonably well to missingness at random. We found nothing in the literature on nonresponse bias pertaining to ASD specifically; nonresponse has been linked to psychopathologic factors in some studies,29 but it is not clear in which direction the overall burden of bias lies.
The small number of cases identified (n = 19) limited our ability to identify possible associations. For example, although the prevalence of ASD appeared greater in adults living in the most deprived localities and in those in the lowest household income groups (Table 2), these associations were not statistically significant; however, such associations cannot be ruled out given the wide confidence intervals. Nevertheless, the study was powered to generate a prevalence estimate within a prespecified 95% CI, rather than to test hypotheses associating ASD status with particular attributes.
For practical reasons, large surveys of adults usually preclude the involvement of collateral informants capable of describing the respondent's behavior over time, including in childhood. Our principal in-depth outcome measure, the ADOS-4, is limited to direct observation in an artificial context. However, detailed assessments in a supplementary third phase sample (to be reported separately) and using well-established developmental interviews showed good agreement with the ADOS-4. Intellectual ability is an important factor in determining the outcome of ASD in adulthood2; our measure of V-IQ was limited and has not been validated for community-living adults with ASD. However, educational level achieved is regarded as a good index of ability and was more strongly associated with ASD in our multivariable model. Our assessment methods provided insufficient information for subtyping ASD, but our cases were likely to be Asperger syndrome, pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified, and autistic disorder.22
The weighted prevalence of 9.8 per 1000 population in adults is essentially the same as recently reported8-10 in systematic surveys of children up to age 15 years. Two of these childhood surveys also used age-standardized versions of the same diagnostic instrument, the ADOS.8,10 This overall rate can be contrasted with that of other mental disorders contributing significantly to the global burden of disease in adulthood. Thus, the prevalence of ASD is approximately twice that of psychosis and half that of current depressive episodes assessed in the same survey.14 Some research and public health purposes may require the use of cut-off points or an ordinal scale or dimensional description of the burden of a disorder. The distribution of ADOS-4 cases (Table 1) suggests continuity rather than discontinuity above and below the diagnostic threshold as found in psychiatric disorders such as depression.30 Our validation study does not recommend using a cut-off point of 7 or greater (Table 1) on the ADOS-4, although the developers21 have suggested that this might be acceptable in combination with other detailed clinical information. The higher estimated prevalence in men vs women was based on the ADOS-4. The self-report AQ-20 used in phase 1 suggested a smaller sex difference in those with high scores. This variability is in line with childhood studies7 showing that the sex difference is less when subthreshold forms of the condition are studied. It could also indicate differences between instruments in the identification of the less common female form of autism, although this seems less likely. Some degree of heterogeneity is likely to underlie the behavioral presentation of our autism prevalence cases; such heterogeneity may cause problems in the design and interpretation of community treatment trials.31
Our survey lacked the statistical power to examine small but possibly clinically and etiologically important differences between cohorts in rates of ASD according to age. Our CI for age is consistent with a percentage increase in prevalence of 4% or a percentage decrease of 6% per year of age. Small reductions of prevalence with age could be explained by increased mortality in autism32 or by older respondents being less available to participate in a household survey because of loss of family support or increased use of institutionalization. However, based on the literature reviewed earlier, we would expect substantially lower rates in earlier birth cohorts (older respondents) if rates of ASD have been rising considerably during recent decades, and we would also expect lower rates in younger adults than in recent childhood surveys using the ADOS-4. We did not find this. Overall, our findings suggest that prevalence is neither rising nor falling significantly. This favors the interpretation that methods of ascertainment have changed in more recent surveys of children compared with the earliest surveys in which the rates reported were considerably lower. Our data suggest that the causes of autism appear to be temporally constant and that recent apparent rises in rates of diagnosis must therefore reflect better survey case finding rather than some new environmental toxin. However, we urge caution and the need for independent replication of this first set of adult community survey findings. Whether using cross-sectional survey or cohort33 sampling designs, further work with adults is needed to improve the specificity and sensitivity of self-report measures of ASD.
The finding that adults with ASD are socially disadvantaged, at a lower educational level, less able intellectually, and apparently unrecognized by mental health services is of potential public health importance. This is in a country with well-established social, educational, welfare, and health care services funded from taxation. Research is also needed in lower-income countries where clinical experience suggests that ASD is prevalent and disabling. Participants with ASDs were less likely to know about a key aspect of their financial circumstances, namely, whether they are receiving state financial benefits (Table 2). This is in line with the clinical observation that many people with ASD are ill equipped to manage their finances. Previous work34,35 shows that adults with mental health disorders receive more attention from services because these problems are recognized needs. However, we found no statistically significant increase in use of the services among adults with ASD. The cases identified in this survey were apparently not clinically recognized; the underrecognition of ASDs was also apparent in a recent national audit4 of support for more able adults with autism from local government and the national health service in England. This audit also makes a reasoned economic argument for increasing the identification of adults with ASD and for supporting them to obtain and maintain appropriate paid employment. The characteristics of our community survey cases appear to differ from those of adults who have responded to postal and online surveys stating that they have an ASD,13 emphasizing the importance of systematic case finding for public health information. Formal comparison of community and clinically diagnosed cases is not yet possible. However, it would appear that cases are often underdiagnosed in elderly individuals, those with lower levels of functioning, those in paid employment, and men.
There are no effective medical treatments for ASD, particularly in adulthood. Adults with ASD have enduring problems with communication and social understanding. However, social care services are being developed to support them, based on the principle that staff recognize and accept the presence of the condition and learn how to understand and communicate with those who have it. This might, for example, improve access to sustained paid employment.36 In our clinical experience, providing this sort of social care to adults with a diagnosis of ASD leads to improvements in quality of life and reductions in the inappropriate use of high-cost hospital services.
To our knowledge, we have demonstrated for the first time in the general population that the rate of ASD is not significantly associated with age, suggesting that the causes of autism are temporally constant. A great deal more research should be directed at the epidemiology and care of adults with this condition.
Correspondence: Traolach S. Brugha, MD(NUI), FRCPsych, Department of Health Sciences, University of Leicester, Leicester General Hospital, Gwendolen Rd, Leicester LE5 4PW, England (tsb@le.ac.uk).
Submitted for Publication: November 12, 2010; accepted November 30, 2010.
Author Contributions: Dr Brugha had full access to all the data in the study and had final responsibility for the decision to submit for publication.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by the National Health Service Information Centre for Health and Social Care and Department of Health; autism follow-up was supported by the National Institute for Health Research and Department of Health Policy Research Programme.
Role of the Sponsor: The scope of the topics covered in the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey was determined in discussion with the funders before fieldwork commenced. The funders took no part in determining the results reported in this article. The authors are solely responsible for the article and its contents.
Additional Contributions: Acknowledgment for the advice and support of groups and individuals can be found elsewhere.19
2.Howlin
PGoode
SHutton
JRutter
M Adult outcome for children with autism.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2004;45
(2)
212- 229
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Knapp
MRomeo
RBeecham
J Economic Consequences of Autism in the UK. London, England Mental Health Foundation and Autism Speaks2007;
4.National Audit Office, Supporting People With Autism Through Adulthood. London, England National Audit Office2009;
5.Fombonne
E Epidemiological surveys of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders: an update.
J Autism Dev Disord 2003;33
(4)
365- 382
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Fombonne
E Epidemiology of autistic disorder and other pervasive developmental disorders.
J Clin Psychiatry 2005;66(suppl 10)3- 8
PubMedGoogle Scholar 7.Newschaffer
CJCroen
LADaniels
JGiarelli
EGrether
JKLevy
SEMandell
DSMiller
LAPinto-Martin
JReaven
JReynolds
AMRice
CESchendel
DWindham
GC The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders.
Annu Rev Public Health 2007;28235- 258
PubMedGoogle Scholar 8.Baird
GSimonoff
EPickles
AChandler
SLoucas
TMeldrum
DCharman
T Prevalence of disorders of the autism spectrum in a population cohort of children in South Thames: the Special Needs and Autism Project (SNAP).
Lancet 2006;368
(9531)
210- 215
PubMedGoogle Scholar 9.Green
HMcGinnity
AMeltzer
HFord
TGoodman
R Mental Health of Children and Young People in Great Britain, 2004. Basingstoke, England Palgrave Macmillan September2005;
10.Baron-Cohen
SScott
FJAllison
CWilliams
JBolton
PMatthews
FEBrayne
C Prevalence of autism-spectrum conditions: UK school-based population study.
Br J Psychiatry 2009;194
(6)
500- 509
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Cooper
SASmiley
EMorrison
JWilliamson
AAllan
L Mental ill-health in adults with intellectual disabilities: prevalence and associated factors.
Br J Psychiatry 2007;19027- 35
PubMedGoogle Scholar 12.Risi
SLord
CGotham
KCorsello
CChrysler
CSzatmari
PCook
EH
JrLeventhal
BLPickles
A Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2006;45
(9)
1094- 1103
PubMedGoogle Scholar 13.National Autistic Society, I Exist: The Message From Adults With Autism. London, England National Autistic Society2008;
14.McManus
SMeltzer
HBrugha
TBebbington
PJenkins
R Adult Psychiatric Morbidity in England, 2007: Results of a Household Survey. London, England NHS Information Centre for Health and Social Care2009;
15.Baron-Cohen
SWheelwright
SSkinner
RMartin
JClubley
E The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians.
J Autism Dev Disord 2001;31
(1)
5- 17
PubMedGoogle Scholar 16.Lord
CRisi
SLambrecht
LCook
EH
JrLeventhal
BLDiLavore
PCPickles
ARutter
M The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism.
J Autism Dev Disord 2000;30
(3)
205- 223
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Hoekstra
RAVinkhuyzen
AAEWheelwright
SBartels
MBoomsma
DIBaron-Cohen
SPosthuma
Dvan der Sluis
S The construction and validation of an abridged version of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ-short) [published online August 10, 2010].
J Autism Dev Disord PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Wakabayashi
ABaron-Cohen
SWheelwright
STojo
Y The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in Japan: a cross-cultural comparison.
J Autism Dev Disord 2006;36
(2)
263- 270
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Brugha
TMcManus
SMeltzer
HSmith
JScott
FJPurdon
SHarris
JBankart
J Autism Spectrum Disorders in Adults Living in Households Throughout England—Report From the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey 2007. Leeds, England NHS Information Centre2009;
20.Nelson
HEWillison
J National Adult Reading Test (NART). 2nd Windsor, England NFER-Nelson1991;
21.Lord
CRutter
MDiLavore
PCRisi
S Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: ADOS Manual. Los Angeles, CA Western Psychological Services2002;
22.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
23.Lord
CRutter
MLe Couteur
A Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders.
J Autism Dev Disord 1994;24
(5)
659- 685
PubMedGoogle Scholar 24.Wing
LLeekam
SRLibby
SJGould
JLarcombe
M The Diagnostic Interview for Social and Communication Disorders: background, inter-rater reliability and clinical use.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2002;43
(3)
307- 325
PubMedGoogle Scholar 26.de Leeuw
EDde Heer
W Trends in Household Survey Nonresponse: A Longitudinal and International Comparison: Survey Nonresponse. Hoboken, NJ Wiley & Sons2002;44- 51
27.Groves
RM Nonresponse rates and nonresponse bias in household surveys.
Public Opin Q 2006;70
(5)
646- 67510.1093/poq/nfl033
Google Scholar 28.Groves
RMedDillman
DedEltinge
JedLittle
RJAed Survey Nonresponse. New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc2002;
29.Eaton
WWAnthony
JCTepper
SDryman
A Psychopathology and attrition in the epidemiologic catchment area surveys.
Am J Epidemiol 1992;135
(9)
1051- 1059
PubMedGoogle Scholar 30.Melzer
DTom
BDBrugha
TSFryers
TFMeltzer
H Common mental disorder symptom counts in populations: are there distinct case groups above epidemiological cut-offs?
Psychol Med 2002;32
(7)
1195- 1201
PubMedGoogle Scholar 31.Spence
SJThurm
A Testing autism interventions: trials and tribulations.
Lancet 2010;375
(9732)
2124- 2125
PubMedGoogle Scholar 32.Pickett
JAPaculdo
DRShavelle
RMStrauss
DJ 1998-2002 Update on “causes of death in autism.”
J Autism Dev Disord 2006;36
(2)
287- 288
PubMedGoogle Scholar 33.Susser
EBresnahan
M Epidemiologic approaches to neurodevelopmental disorders.
Mol Psychiatry 2002;7(suppl 2)S2- S3
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.Brugha
TSBebbington
PESingleton
NMelzer
DJenkins
RLewis
GFarrell
MBhugra
DLee
AMeltzer
H Trends in service use and treatment for mental disorders in adults throughout Great Britain.
Br J Psychiatry 2004;185378- 384
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Bebbington
PEMeltzer
HBrugha
TSFarrell
MJenkins
RCeresa
CLewis
G Unequal access and unmet need: neurotic disorders and the use of primary care services.
Psychol Med 2000;30
(6)
1359- 1367
PubMedGoogle Scholar 36.Howlin
PAlcock
JBurkin
C An 8 year follow-up of a specialist supported employment service for high-ability adults with autism or Asperger syndrome.
Autism 2005;9
(5)
533- 549
PubMedGoogle Scholar