[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Hypothetical Causal and Predispositional Effects
Hypothetical Causal and Predispositional Effects

Three hypothetical patterns of results from the linear mixed model analyses are presented here. A, The hypothesis that cannabis causes alterations in brain volumes (depicted as reductions in volume) was tested with contrast 1. As denoted by the bar labeled contrast 1, this contrast tested for differences between exposed and unexposed members of pairs discordant for cannabis use. Finding smaller volumes among exposed members of these pairs compared with their siblings would support this causal hypothesis (pending replication with monozygotic twin pairs). B, Contrast 2 compares volumes for concordant exposed pairs with discordant pairs to test the hypothesis that brain volumes and cannabis use share familial/predispositional factors where concordant exposed pairs are at increased liability (both siblings are exposed vs only one), ie, graded liability. C, Contrast 3 compares volumes for the concordant unexposed pairs with all other groups to test the hypothesis that brain volumes and cannabis use share familial/predispositional factors but that liability does not differ by concordance vs discordance for use.

Figure 2.  Left Amygdala Volume by Cannabis Exposure Group
Left Amygdala Volume by Cannabis Exposure Group

Estimated marginal means (error bars indicate standard errors) for left amygdala volumes by cannabis exposure group are presented. The significant fixed effect of contrast 3 is denoted: concordant unexposed pairs showed larger volumes than all other groups.

aP < .05.

Table 1.  Sample Characteristics of 483 Participants Included From the September 2014 Data Release of the Human Connectome Projecta
Sample Characteristics of 483 Participants Included From the September 2014 Data Release of the Human Connectome Projecta
Table 2.  Regression Results for the Association Between Cannabis-Related Measures and Brain Volumesa
Regression Results for the Association Between Cannabis-Related Measures and Brain Volumesa
Table 3.  Results of SOLAR Analysesa
Results of SOLAR Analysesa
1.
Degenhardt  L, Whiteford  HA, Ferrari  AJ,  et al.  Global burden of disease attributable to illicit drug use and dependence: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010.  Lancet. 2013;382(9904):1564-1574.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Hall  W, Lynskey  M.  The challenges in developing a rational cannabis policy.  Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2009;22(3):258-262.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Room  R.  Cannabis legalization and public health: legal niceties, commercialization and countercultures.  Addiction. 2014;109(3):358-359.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Volkow  ND, Baler  RD, Compton  WM, Weiss  SRB.  Adverse health effects of marijuana use.  N Engl J Med. 2014;370(23):2219-2227.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Lorenzetti  V, Lubman  DI, Whittle  S, Solowij  N, Yücel  M.  Structural MRI findings in long-term cannabis users: what do we know?  Subst Use Misuse. 2010;45(11):1787-1808.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Rocchetti  M, Crescini  A, Borgwardt  S,  et al.  Is cannabis neurotoxic for the healthy brain? a meta-analytical review of structural brain alterations in non-psychotic users.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;67(7):483-492.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Gilman  JM, Kuster  JK, Lee  S,  et al.  Cannabis use is quantitatively associated with nucleus accumbens and amygdala abnormalities in young adult recreational users.  J Neurosci. 2014;34(16):5529-5538.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Cheetham  A, Allen  NB, Whittle  S, Simmons  JG, Yücel  M, Lubman  DI.  Orbitofrontal volumes in early adolescence predict initiation of cannabis use: a 4-year longitudinal and prospective study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(8):684-692.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Jacobus  J, Squeglia  LM, Infante  MA, Bava  S, Tapert  SF.  White matter integrity pre- and post marijuana and alcohol initiation in adolescence.  Brain Sci. 2013;3(1):396-414.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Kendler  KS, Neale  MC, MacLean  CJ, Heath  AC, Eaves  LJ, Kessler  RC.  Smoking and major depression: a causal analysis.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50(1):36-43.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Lynskey  MT, Heath  AC, Bucholz  KK,  et al.  Escalation of drug use in early-onset cannabis users vs co-twin controls.  JAMA. 2003;289(4):427-433.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Gilbertson  MW, Shenton  ME, Ciszewski  A,  et al.  Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma.  Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(11):1242-1247.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Lessov-Schlaggar  CN, Lepore  RL, Kristjansson  SD,  et al.  Functional neuroimaging study in identical twin pairs discordant for regular cigarette smoking.  Addict Biol. 2013;18(1):98-108.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Van Essen  DC, Ugurbil  K, Auerbach  E,  et al; WU-Minn HCP Consortium.  The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective.  Neuroimage. 2012;62(4):2222-2231.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Glasser  MF, Sotiropoulos  SN, Wilson  JA,  et al; WU-Minn HCP Consortium.  The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project.  Neuroimage. 2013;80(C):105-124.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Dale  AM, Fischl  B, Sereno  MI.  Cortical surface-based analysis, I: segmentation and surface reconstruction.  Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):179-194.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Fischl  B, van der Kouwe  A, Destrieux  C,  et al.  Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex.  Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(1):11-22. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Desikan  RS, Ségonne  F, Fischl  B,  et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest.  Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968-980.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Bucholz  KK, Cadoret  R, Cloninger  CR,  et al.  A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA.  J Stud Alcohol. 1994;55(2):149-158.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Gershon  RC, Slotkin  J, Manly  JJ,  et al.  IV, NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring language (vocabulary comprehension and reading decoding).  Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013;78(4):49-69.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
R Development Core Team.  R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.
22.
Canty  A, Ripley  B.  boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus) Functions [computer program]. R package version 1.3-17. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012.
23.
Blangero  J, Almasy  L.  Solar: Sequential Oligogenic Linkage Analysis Routines: Population Genetics Laboratory Technical Report No. 6. San Antonio, TX: Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research; 1996.
24.
Evans  DM, Gillespie  NA, Martin  NG.  Biometrical genetics.  Biol Psychol. 2002;61(1-2):33-51.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Bates  D, Maechler  M, Bolker  B.  lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using S4 Classes. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012.
26.
Cousijn  J, Wiers  RW, Ridderinkhof  KR, van den Brink  W, Veltman  DJ, Goudriaan  AE.  Grey matter alterations associated with cannabis use: results of a VBM study in heavy cannabis users and healthy controls.  Neuroimage. 2012;59(4):3845-3851.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Schacht  JP, Hutchison  KE, Filbey  FM.  Associations between cannabinoid receptor-1 (CNR1) variation and hippocampus and amygdala volumes in heavy cannabis users.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(11):2368-2376.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Yücel  M, Solowij  N, Respondek  C,  et al.  Regional brain abnormalities associated with long-term heavy cannabis use.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(6):694-701.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Agrawal  A, Neale  MC, Prescott  CA, Kendler  KS.  A twin study of early cannabis use and subsequent use and abuse/dependence of other illicit drugs.  Psychol Med. 2004;34(7):1227-1237.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Weiland  BJ, Thayer  RE, Depue  BE, Sabbineni  A, Bryan  AD, Hutchison  KE.  Daily marijuana use is not associated with brain morphometric measures in adolescents or adults.  J Neurosci. 2015;35(4):1505-1512.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Hall  W.  What has research over the past two decades revealed about the adverse health effects of recreational cannabis use?  Addiction. 2015;110(1):19-35.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Battistella  G, Fornari  E, Annoni  J-M,  et al.  Long-term effects of cannabis on brain structure.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(9):2041-2048.PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
 Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. http://addictionresearch.nih.gov/adolescent-brain-cognitive-development-study. Accessed March 14, 2015.
34.
Hanson  JL, Nacewicz  BM, Sutterer  MJ,  et al.  Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala.  Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(4):314-323.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Sartor  CE, Waldron  M, Duncan  AE,  et al.  Childhood sexual abuse and early substance use in adolescent girls: the role of familial influences.  Addiction. 2013;108(5):993-1000.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Burston  JJ, Wiley  JL, Craig  AA, Selley  DE, Sim-Selley  LJ.  Regional enhancement of cannabinoid CB&#8321: receptor desensitization in female adolescent rats following repeated Δ-tetrahydrocannabinol exposure.  Br J Pharmacol. 2010;161(1):103-112.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Rubino  T, Vigano’  D, Realini  N,  et al.  Chronic delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol during adolescence provokes sex-dependent changes in the emotional profile in adult rats: behavioral and biochemical correlates.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(11):2760-2771.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Scallet  AC.  Neurotoxicology of cannabis and THC: a review of chronic exposure studies in animals.  Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;40(3):671-676.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Landfield  PW, Cadwallader  LB, Vinsant  S.  Quantitative changes in hippocampal structure following long-term exposure to delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol: possible mediation by glucocorticoid systems.  Brain Res. 1988;443(1-2):47-62.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
October 2015

Shared Predisposition in the Association Between Cannabis Use and Subcortical Brain Structure

Author Affiliations
  • 1The Program in Neuroscience, Washington University in St Louis, St Louis, Missouri
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Washington University in St Louis, St Louis, Missouri
  • 3Department of Psychology, Washington University in St Louis, St Louis, Missouri
  • 4Department of Radiology, Washington University in St Louis, St Louis, Missouri
  • 5Department of PsychologicalSciences, University of Missouri, Columbia
  • 6Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, Department of Addictions, King’s College London, London, England
JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(10):994-1001. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1054
Abstract

Importance  Prior neuroimaging studies have suggested that alterations in brain structure may be a consequence of cannabis use. Siblings discordant for cannabis use offer an opportunity to use cross-sectional data to disentangle such causal hypotheses from shared effects of genetics and familial environment on brain structure and cannabis use.

Objectives  To determine whether cannabis use is associated with differences in brain structure in a large sample of twins/siblings and to examine sibling pairs discordant for cannabis use to separate potential causal and predispositional factors linking lifetime cannabis exposure to volumetric alterations.

Design, Setting, and Participants  Cross-sectional diagnostic interview, behavioral, and neuroimaging data were collected from community sampling and established family registries from August 2012 to September 2014. This study included data from 483 participants (22-35 years old) enrolled in the ongoing Human Connectome Project, with 262 participants reporting cannabis exposure (ie, ever used cannabis in their lifetime).

Main Outcomes and Measures  Cannabis exposure was measured with the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism. Whole-brain, hippocampus, amygdala, ventral striatum, and orbitofrontal cortex volumes were related to lifetime cannabis use (ever used, age at onset, and frequency of use) using linear regressions. Genetic (ρg) and environmental (ρe) correlations between cannabis use and brain volumes were estimated. Linear mixed models were used to examine volume differences in sex-matched concordant unexposed (n = 71 pairs), exposed (n = 81 pairs), or exposure discordant (n = 89 pairs) sibling pairs.

Results  Among 483 study participants, cannabis exposure was related to smaller left amygdala (approximately 2.3%; P = .007) and right ventral striatum (approximately 3.5%; P < .005) volumes. These volumetric differences were within the range of normal variation. The association between left amygdala volume and cannabis use was largely owing to shared genetic factors (ρg = −0.43; P = .004), while the origin of the association with right ventral striatum volumes was unclear. Importantly, brain volumes did not differ between sex-matched siblings discordant for use (fixed effect = −7.43; t = −0.93, P = .35). Both the exposed and unexposed siblings in pairs discordant for cannabis exposure showed reduced amygdala volumes relative to members of concordant unexposed pairs (fixed effect = 12.56; t = 2.97; P = .003).

Conclusions and Relevance  In this study, differences in amygdala volume in cannabis users were attributable to common predispositional factors, genetic or environmental in origin, with little support for causal influences. Causal influences, in isolation or in conjunction with predispositional factors, may exist for other brain regions (eg, ventral striatum) or at more severe levels of cannabis involvement and deserve further study.

Introduction

Cannabis is the most widely used recreational drug in developed nations.1 Its legal status has been a source of enduring controversy,2 gaining particular momentum in the United States.3 Yet, concerns about putative influences of cannabis on brain structure/function remain salient.4 Neuroimaging studies have found inconsistent evidence linking cannabis to brain structure where previous meta-analyses noted possible associations between cannabis and hippocampus (and potentially amygdala) structure.5,6

Small sample sizes in prior studies (generally N < 100) and varied definitions of cannabis exposure may have contributed to these inconsistent findings. Additionally, studies have implied that cannabis causes volumetric alterations, despite typically not controlling for potential confounding effects of shared predispositional factors (eg, genes/rearing environment that contribute to both volumetric differences and cannabis use). For instance, Gilman et al7 compared recreational cannabis users (n = 20) with matched nonusers (n = 20) and posited, based on observed dose-dependent relationships, that larger left ventral striatal (VS) gray matter density was likely a consequence of cannabis use. However, such cross-sectional case-control designs cannot account for the possibility that volumetric variations might predate cannabis use and/or might relate to cannabis use via predispositional factors, even in a dose-dependent manner.

Longitudinal studies, particularly with assessments preceding onset of cannabis use, are ideally suited to disentangle the effects of cannabis on the developing brain from preexisting differences. Such study designs have shown that smaller orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) volumes were associated with later cannabis initiation in adolescents8 yet also find emerging deficits in white matter as a consequence of heavy alcohol and cannabis use.9 However, even in cross-sectional studies, twins/siblings discordant for cannabis exposure provide a unique opportunity for differentiating predispositional/familial factors from causal effects of cannabis on the brain. As monozygotic (MZ) twins share all genetic material identical by descent, neural differences between MZ twins discordant for cannabis exposure can be potentially attributed to the causal effects of cannabis.10,11 In contrast, if no differences are found, then large causal effects of cannabis on the brain are unlikely and, instead, the association might be attributed to genetic factors/familial environment. For instance, Gilbertson and colleagues12 found that posttraumatic stress disorder severity in combat-exposed veterans negatively correlated with their hippocampal volume and the volumes of their noncombat-exposed MZ cotwins, implicating predispositional rather than causal mechanisms. In contrast, Lessov-Schlaggar et al13 found functional differences in VS response to reward and punishment in regular tobacco smokers when compared with their nonregular-smoking MZ cotwin, implying potential causal mechanisms. Importantly, although data from MZ twins are essential to demonstrate causality in this manner, the absence of neural differences within discordant dizygotic (DZ) twin or nontwin sibling pairs is still compelling evidence against a causal hypothesis. If volume differences are not observed among discordant pairs who share only 50% of their genes, then finding an association with further genetic matching (ie, MZ pairs) would be unlikely.10

The goal of the current study was to test previously observed relationships between cannabis and brain volumes in a large normative sample of twins/siblings from the Human Connectome Project (HCP; N = 483). First, we examined whether cannabis exposure, age at onset of use, and lifetime frequency of use were associated with whole-brain volume (WBV) and amygdala, hippocampus, VS, or OFC volumes. Second, we quantified the degree to which shared genetic and individual-specific environmental factors contributed to these associations. Finally, to test whether any significant volumetric differences could be attributed to predispositional/familial or causal factors, we compared volumes across sex-matched twin/sibling pairs (henceforth referred to as sibling pairs) discordant for cannabis exposure (n = 89 pairs), concordant for exposure (n = 81 pairs), or concordantly unexposed (n = 71 pairs).

Methods
Participants

Participants were drawn from the September 2014 public data release from the HCP (N = 527), which aims to recruit 1200 individuals (3-4 siblings per family, most including a twin pair).14 All participants were aged 22 to 35 years; for all inclusion/exclusion criteria, see the study by Van Essen et al.14 Participants were not excluded for recreational drug use (unless they reported being hospitalized for ≥2 days for substance abuse or being treated by a medical specialist for ≥12 months for substance abuse [or any psychiatric or neurological condition]). Institutional review board approval for this study was obtained from Washington University in St Louis, and all patients provided written informed consent that included permission for public release of the data.

Participants were excluded from the current analyses if they lacked good-quality structural magnetic resonance imaging data (n = 17), were missing relevant interview/questionnaire data (n = 11), or if they screened positive for tetrahydrocannabinol on a urine screen but reported not using cannabis within the last 12 months (n = 16). This resulted in a final sample size of 483 individuals (262 having ever used cannabis). In analyses involving sibling pairs (eAppendix 1 in the Supplement), 36 individuals who did not have a full sibling or twin in the present data release were excluded. Because sex is significantly related to both cannabis use and brain volumes, the inclusion of discordant opposite-sex nontwin sibling pairs (all twin pairs were of the same sex) can result in statistical confounds. Therefore, we excluded 145 opposite-sex pairs, resulting in 241 sibling pairs (50 MZ, 45 DZ, and 146 nontwin siblings [mean sibling age difference, 3.69 years]), including 89 same-sex pairs discordant for cannabis exposure, 81 concordant for cannabis exposure, and 71 concordantly unexposed pairs (eTable 1 in the Supplement).

Brain Volume Data

High-resolution (0.7-mm isotropic voxels) anatomical images were acquired using a customized Siemens Skyra 3-T scanner with a 32-channel head coil. For details on data acquisition and preprocessing, see the study by Glasser et al15 (see eAppendix 2 in the Supplement for relevant preprocessing steps). Volume estimates for the regions of interest were extracted using FreeSurfer version 5.3.0 (http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/).16,17 This included WBV (total gray + cortical white matter volume) and left and right amygdala, hippocampus, and VS volumes from subcortical segmentation and OFC volumes (lateral + medial) from cortical parcellation using the Desikan et al18 atlas.

Questionnaire, Interview, and Behavioral Data
Cannabis Use

Cannabis-related measures were assessed using the Semi-Structured Assessment for the Genetics of Alcoholism (SSAGA).19 Cannabis exposure was a dichotomous variable representing whether an individual reported ever using cannabis during their lifetime. Age at onset of cannabis use (reported age at first cannabis use) and lifetime frequency of use (reported number of times using cannabis across one’s lifetime) were also examined (both coded ordinally by the HCP; eAppendix 3 in the Supplement).

Covariates

The main analyses controlled for demographic factors of sex (female > male), race/ethnicity (white > not; African American > not), age, zygosity (MZ > not; DZ > not), total household income (eAppendix 3 in the Supplement), and age-adjusted picture vocabulary scores as a proxy for crystallized intelligence20 (eAppendix 4 in the Supplement)—henceforth, these are referred to as primary covariates. Follow-up regression analyses controlled for a variety of other potential confounds including personality, impulsivity, other substance use, and comorbid psychopathology (eTable 2 in the Supplement). See eAppendix 5 in the Supplement for rationale and assessment details.

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics version 20 (IBM Corp) and R version 3.1 (The R Project for Statistical Computing).21 One outlier (>3 × the interquartile range away from the 25th/75th percentile) for right amygdala and right hippocampal volume was Winsorized to the next most extreme value. Independent-sample t tests and χ2 tests were used to test for differences in the covariates between cannabis-exposed and unexposed individuals.

Regression Analyses

Linear regressions were used to test whether cannabis exposure (in the full sample, N = 483) and age at onset or frequency of use (among exposed individuals, n = 262) were associated with WBV or subcortical (left and right amygdala, hippocampus, and VS) or OFC volumes, controlling for the primary covariates and WBV (when predicting regional volumes). Bootstrapped 95% CIs were calculated to account for familial clustering (R boot package22). False-discovery rate was used to control for the 9 regression analyses with each cannabis measure. Only brain regions with q < 0.05 were examined in the sibling analysis.

Sources of Variance and Covariance

Sequential oligogenic linkage analysis routines (SOLAR)23 were used to attribute phenotypic correlations (ρp) between cannabis exposure and brain volumes to overlapping genetic (ρg) or individual-specific environmental (ρe) factors.24 All models controlled for the primary covariates and WBV (when examining regional volumes).

Discordant Sibling Analyses

All possible same-sex sibling pairs were drawn from the data (n = 241 pairs from 146 families; details in eAppendix 1 in the Supplement). Linear mixed models (lmer function in R package lme425) were used, which nested individuals within sibling pairs and nested pairs within families. The mixed models included fixed effects for the primary covariates and WBV (when predicting regional volumes). The main effect of interest was membership in a sibling pair concordant or discordant for cannabis exposure. To test this, participants were divided into 4 groups: individuals from concordant unexposed pairs, individuals from concordant exposed pairs, exposed members of discordant pairs, and unexposed members of discordant pairs. This 4-group factor was tested as a fixed effect using Helmert contrast coding (eTable 3 in the Supplement).

Figure 1 displays hypothetical patterns of results from the linear mixed model analyses that could be found by the 3 contrasts examined. Contrast 1 compared exposed and unexposed siblings from discordant pairs to test a causal hypothesis, ie, cannabis causes altered brain volumes (depicted as smaller volumes for exposed vs unexposed individuals in Figure 1A). Siblings share 50% of their genes and much of their rearing environment. Therefore, within-pair volumetric differences would be preliminary evidence for causation, pending replication in MZ pairs. Contrasts 2 and 3 both hypothesize no volumetric differences between the exposed and unexposed members of the discordant pairs and thus tested facets of the hypothesis that cannabis use and brain volumes share predispositional factors. This would suggest that differences in volume likely predate (or co-occur with) cannabis use and that other variables, such as genetic liability or rearing environment, may lead to both neural differences and liability to cannabis use. Alternatively, these other factors could contribute to neural differences, in turn increasing liability to cannabis use. Contrast 2 compared brain volumes from concordant exposed pairs with both members of discordant pairs to test exposure-related differences by concordance/discordance (Figure 1B). A significant effect might indicate that concordantly exposed pairs are at greater liability for cannabis use and altered brain volumes (because both siblings have used cannabis) than discordant pairs (ie, graded liability). Contrast 3 compared volumes from concordant unexposed pairs with all other groups to test whether altered brain volumes and cannabis exposure were associated with a shared predisposition (Figure 1C). A significant effect here implies that both concordant exposed and discordant pairs are at the same genetic liability regardless of whether one or both siblings use cannabis.

Control Analysis

To confirm that differences in brain volumes among discordant pairs in the linear mixed models (contrast 1) were nonsignificant owing to familial matching rather than a reduction in sample size, we randomly paired each cannabis user from a same-sex discordant pair (n = 89) with a sex-matched unrelated unexposed individual. Paired t tests were performed to compare volumes for these unrelated pairs, ie, to remove the familial control while matching for the sample size.

Results
Sample Characteristics

Of the 483 participants, 262 (54.2%) reported ever using cannabis. Forty-nine percent of the exposed individuals reported first using cannabis by age 17 years and 29.4% reported using cannabis more than 100 times (eFigure in the Supplement). Cannabis users were significantly more likely to be male; be nonwhite; report lower income; report greater alcohol, cigarette, and other illicit drug use; report more childhood conduct problems; be less agreeable and more impulsive; and show steeper delay discounting compared with never users (Table 1). Similar relationships were also observed with increasing frequency of use and decreasing age at onset (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Descriptive statistics and intercorrelations among brain volumes are presented in eTable 5 in the Supplement and intercorrelations among all covariates are in eTable 6 in the Supplement.

Regression Analyses

Table 2 summarizes the linear regressions results, controlling for the primary covariates (full results in eTables 7-9 in the Supplement). Relative to unexposed individuals, cannabis users had smaller left amygdala (approximately 2.3%) and right VS (approximately 3.5%) volumes. Post hoc comparisons showed similar volumetric differences in those who used fewer than 100 or 100 or more times, with no statistical difference between the regression coefficients across these levels of use (eTable 10 in the Supplement). Among cannabis users, heavier use was associated with smaller left hippocampus volumes (although this effect did not pass false-discovery rate correction). There were no significant associations with age at onset of use (eTable 9 in the Supplement). These results remained significant when also controlling for a variety of personality factors, impulsivity, other substance use, and psychiatric history (eTable 2 in the Supplement).

Sources of Variance and Covariance

The heritability of cannabis exposure (ever vs never used) was estimated at 67.2% (standard error, 13.6%; P < .001; Table 3). All brain volumes of interest were also significantly heritable. There was no evidence for contributions of shared rearing environment. As previously noted, only the left amygdala (ρp = −0.175; P = .005) and right VS (ρp = −0.154; P = .02) showed significant phenotypic correlations with cannabis use. Decomposing these correlations, we found a significant genetic correlation between left amygdala volume and cannabis use (ρg = −0.433; P = .004) but not a significant environmental correlation (ρe = 0.280; P = .19). Neither genetic factors nor environmental factors were significant for right VS volume.

Discordant Sibling Analyses

Given these results, we focused on the left amygdala and right VS for the sibling analyses. Contrast 3 was significantly associated with left amygdala volumes (β = 12.56; t302.80 = 2.97; P = .003), supporting the predispositional hypothesis. We did not find evidence for either the causal (contrast 1) or graded liability (contrast 2) hypotheses. Concordant unexposed pairs had larger amygdala volumes than all other groups, even unexposed members of discordant pairs (Figure 2). The estimated marginal means (SEs) for each group were as follows: 1534.502 (14.829) for concordant unexposed, 1487.364 (15.500) for discordant unexposed, 1472.501 (15.449) for discordant exposed, and 1492.870 (13.957) for concordant exposed (Figure 2).

Despite not using cannabis, these unexposed individuals from discordant pairs had amygdala volumes resembling cannabis-exposed individuals, including their exposed siblings. None of the 3 contrasts significantly were associated with right VS volumes (fixed effects predicting all volumes are presented in eTable 11 in the Supplement).

Results including opposite-sex sibling pairs are presented in eTable 12 in the Supplement. We observed significant effects of contrast 3 (predisposition), contrast 1 (causal), and an interaction between contrast 1 and pair-sex concordance, underscoring the confounding effect of sex, within pairs, with respect to cannabis use and brain volume.

Control Analyses

Finally, the exposed members of the same-sex discordant pairs (n = 89) were compared with unrelated but sex-matched unexposed individuals. Significantly lower volumes for the left amygdala (approximately 5.1%) were observed for the exposed vs unexposed members of these unrelated pairs (eTable 13 in the Supplement). This confirmed that the lack of volumetric differences between exposed and unexposed members of discordant pairs (contrast 1) was attributable to familial matching and not a reduced sample size.

Discussion
Cannabis Exposure Findings

To our knowledge, this is the largest study to date examining the association between cannabis exposure (ever vs never used) and brain volumes. Cannabis exposure was associated with smaller left amygdala and right VS volumes; these findings persisted even after controlling for a host of covariates. While other studies have noted reductions in amygdala volumes,26-28 the right VS finding is somewhat unique to this study. It contradicts the increased VS volume in occasional cannabis users reported by Gilman and colleagues.7 It remains to be explored whether this is owing to the cannabis measures explored (eg, cannabis exposed vs joints per week), sample size (483 vs 40), or other sample characteristics (eg, race/ethnicity; 74.5% white in our sample but unreported by Gilman et al7).

Importantly, by leveraging the familial design of the HCP, we demonstrated that amygdala volumetric reductions among cannabis users are primarily attributed to familial factors shared by twins/siblings. Overlapping genetic factors (ρg) were the only significant source of covariance; a significant correlation between individual-specific environmental factors (ρe) would be expected if the causal hypothesis were supported.29 The discordant sibling analyses further confirmed this; even in the absence of cannabis exposure, smaller amygdala volumes were observed among individuals with heightened familial liability, given their sibling’s cannabis use. However, interpretations from our sibling analyses should be tempered by our limited sample size to examine discordant MZ pairs only. This predisposition to smaller brain volumes, even in the absence of manifest cannabis use, casts considerable doubt on hypotheses that cannabis use, at least at the levels noted in this sample, causes reductions in amygdala volumes. Instead, both the exposed and unexposed siblings in discordant pairs and concordant exposed pairs tended to have smaller amygdala volumes than concordant unexposed individuals.

Limitations and Future Directions

First, while the normative sampling of the HCP is a strength, we were limited by sample size from examining heavy/problematic cannabis use, which has been inconsistently associated with volumetric alterations.27,30 Furthermore, because heavy/problem cannabis use is often associated with psychiatric problems,31 severity and chronicity of use may have been limited by the HCP’s exclusion of individuals receiving extended psychiatric treatment or hospitalization. Also, data were not available on recent duration/frequency of use, which has been linked to structural changes.32 Thus, although we noted similar evidence of association with lighter (<100 times) and heavier (≥100 times) lifetime cannabis exposure, we were underpowered to test this in our sibling analyses and could not exclude the role of causal factors at higher levels of cannabis exposure. Second, although right VS volume and cannabis use were significantly related, we were unable to disentangle the etiology of this relationship. Neither genetic nor environmental correlations were significant and within-pair differences could be equated across all groups. Thus, causal and/or predispositional influences may link cannabis exposure and VS volume. Third, while the family structure in the HCP data are powerful, longitudinal data that were collected from prior to cannabis onset through later development is critical for substantiating causal claims (eg, National Institutes of Health efforts33). Fourth, the small sample size of discordant MZ twin pairs (n = 9 pairs) is a limitation. Fifth, the role of additional covariates cannot be excluded (eg, childhood trauma is linked to both amygdala volumes34 and cannabis use35). Sixth, while we selected a priori regions of interest based on prior studies, other regions should be examined in future work (eg, using whole-brain voxelwise analysis). Seventh, exploring other brain-related measures, such as white matter integrity and task-related activity, might reveal different findings. Eighth, evidence for potential causal effects in opposite-sex sibling pairs was driven by a small number (n = 14) of discordant pairs where the female sibling was the exposed member. A thorough examination of phenotypic data failed to distinguish these discordant exposed females from others in the sample. However, based on evidence for sex differences in the endocannabinoid system,36,37 such differences in related individuals may reflect qualitatively different pathways and warrants further study. Finally, future work should further explore the graded liability hypothesis.

Conclusions

Despite speculation regarding the neurotoxic effects of tetrahydrocannabinol based on preclinical research (eg, studies by Scallet38 and Landfield et al39), the observed cannabis-related volumetric differences were well within the range of normal variation. When using a simple index of exposure (ie, ever vs never use), we found no evidence for the causal influence of cannabis exposure on amygdala volume. Future work characterizing the roles of causal and predispositional factors underpinning neural changes at various degrees of cannabis involvement may provide targets for substance abuse policy and prevention programs.

Back to top
Article Information

Corresponding Author: David Pagliaccio, PhD, The Program in Neuroscience, Washington University in St Louis, Campus Box 1125, 1 Brookings Dr, St Louis, MO 63130 (david.pagliaccio@wustl.edu).

Submitted for Publication: December 20, 2014; final revision received May 9, 2015; accepted May 15, 2015.

Published Online: August 26, 2015. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.1054.

Author Contributions: Dr Pagliaccio had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Study concept and design: Pagliaccio, Barch, Bogdan, Heath, Agrawal.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Pagliaccio, Agrawal.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.

Statistical analysis: Pagliaccio, Barch, Wood, Agrawal.

Obtained funding: Barch, Heath, Agrawal.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Bogdan, Agrawal.

Study supervision: Barch, Bogdan, Agrawal.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: Data for this study were provided by the Human Connectome Project, WU-Minn Consortium (principal investigators: David Van Essen, PhD, and Kamil Ugurbil, PhD; grant 1U54MH091657) funded by the 16 National Institutes of Health institutes and centers that support the National Institutes of Health Blueprint for Neuroscience Research, as well as by the McDonnell Center for Systems Neuroscience at Washington University. Dr Agrawal’s work was supported by a grant from the National Institute on Drug Abuse (5K02DA32573). Dr Pagliaccio’s work was supported by a grant from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (5T32GM081739). Dr Bogdan receives support from the Klingenstein Third Generation Foundation.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: We thank all participants and their families who provided time and effort to making this study possible.

References
1.
Degenhardt  L, Whiteford  HA, Ferrari  AJ,  et al.  Global burden of disease attributable to illicit drug use and dependence: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010.  Lancet. 2013;382(9904):1564-1574.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Hall  W, Lynskey  M.  The challenges in developing a rational cannabis policy.  Curr Opin Psychiatry. 2009;22(3):258-262.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Room  R.  Cannabis legalization and public health: legal niceties, commercialization and countercultures.  Addiction. 2014;109(3):358-359.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Volkow  ND, Baler  RD, Compton  WM, Weiss  SRB.  Adverse health effects of marijuana use.  N Engl J Med. 2014;370(23):2219-2227.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Lorenzetti  V, Lubman  DI, Whittle  S, Solowij  N, Yücel  M.  Structural MRI findings in long-term cannabis users: what do we know?  Subst Use Misuse. 2010;45(11):1787-1808.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Rocchetti  M, Crescini  A, Borgwardt  S,  et al.  Is cannabis neurotoxic for the healthy brain? a meta-analytical review of structural brain alterations in non-psychotic users.  Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013;67(7):483-492.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Gilman  JM, Kuster  JK, Lee  S,  et al.  Cannabis use is quantitatively associated with nucleus accumbens and amygdala abnormalities in young adult recreational users.  J Neurosci. 2014;34(16):5529-5538.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Cheetham  A, Allen  NB, Whittle  S, Simmons  JG, Yücel  M, Lubman  DI.  Orbitofrontal volumes in early adolescence predict initiation of cannabis use: a 4-year longitudinal and prospective study.  Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71(8):684-692.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Jacobus  J, Squeglia  LM, Infante  MA, Bava  S, Tapert  SF.  White matter integrity pre- and post marijuana and alcohol initiation in adolescence.  Brain Sci. 2013;3(1):396-414.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Kendler  KS, Neale  MC, MacLean  CJ, Heath  AC, Eaves  LJ, Kessler  RC.  Smoking and major depression: a causal analysis.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1993;50(1):36-43.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Lynskey  MT, Heath  AC, Bucholz  KK,  et al.  Escalation of drug use in early-onset cannabis users vs co-twin controls.  JAMA. 2003;289(4):427-433.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Gilbertson  MW, Shenton  ME, Ciszewski  A,  et al.  Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma.  Nat Neurosci. 2002;5(11):1242-1247.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Lessov-Schlaggar  CN, Lepore  RL, Kristjansson  SD,  et al.  Functional neuroimaging study in identical twin pairs discordant for regular cigarette smoking.  Addict Biol. 2013;18(1):98-108.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Van Essen  DC, Ugurbil  K, Auerbach  E,  et al; WU-Minn HCP Consortium.  The Human Connectome Project: a data acquisition perspective.  Neuroimage. 2012;62(4):2222-2231.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Glasser  MF, Sotiropoulos  SN, Wilson  JA,  et al; WU-Minn HCP Consortium.  The minimal preprocessing pipelines for the Human Connectome Project.  Neuroimage. 2013;80(C):105-124.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Dale  AM, Fischl  B, Sereno  MI.  Cortical surface-based analysis, I: segmentation and surface reconstruction.  Neuroimage. 1999;9(2):179-194.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Fischl  B, van der Kouwe  A, Destrieux  C,  et al.  Automatically parcellating the human cerebral cortex.  Cereb Cortex. 2004;14(1):11-22. PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Desikan  RS, Ségonne  F, Fischl  B,  et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest.  Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968-980.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Bucholz  KK, Cadoret  R, Cloninger  CR,  et al.  A new, semi-structured psychiatric interview for use in genetic linkage studies: a report on the reliability of the SSAGA.  J Stud Alcohol. 1994;55(2):149-158.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Gershon  RC, Slotkin  J, Manly  JJ,  et al.  IV, NIH Toolbox Cognition Battery (CB): measuring language (vocabulary comprehension and reading decoding).  Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. 2013;78(4):49-69.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
R Development Core Team.  R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.
22.
Canty  A, Ripley  B.  boot: Bootstrap R (S-Plus) Functions [computer program]. R package version 1.3-17. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012.
23.
Blangero  J, Almasy  L.  Solar: Sequential Oligogenic Linkage Analysis Routines: Population Genetics Laboratory Technical Report No. 6. San Antonio, TX: Southwest Foundation for Biomedical Research; 1996.
24.
Evans  DM, Gillespie  NA, Martin  NG.  Biometrical genetics.  Biol Psychol. 2002;61(1-2):33-51.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Bates  D, Maechler  M, Bolker  B.  lme4: Linear Mixed-Effects Models Using S4 Classes. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012.
26.
Cousijn  J, Wiers  RW, Ridderinkhof  KR, van den Brink  W, Veltman  DJ, Goudriaan  AE.  Grey matter alterations associated with cannabis use: results of a VBM study in heavy cannabis users and healthy controls.  Neuroimage. 2012;59(4):3845-3851.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Schacht  JP, Hutchison  KE, Filbey  FM.  Associations between cannabinoid receptor-1 (CNR1) variation and hippocampus and amygdala volumes in heavy cannabis users.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(11):2368-2376.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Yücel  M, Solowij  N, Respondek  C,  et al.  Regional brain abnormalities associated with long-term heavy cannabis use.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(6):694-701.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Agrawal  A, Neale  MC, Prescott  CA, Kendler  KS.  A twin study of early cannabis use and subsequent use and abuse/dependence of other illicit drugs.  Psychol Med. 2004;34(7):1227-1237.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Weiland  BJ, Thayer  RE, Depue  BE, Sabbineni  A, Bryan  AD, Hutchison  KE.  Daily marijuana use is not associated with brain morphometric measures in adolescents or adults.  J Neurosci. 2015;35(4):1505-1512.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Hall  W.  What has research over the past two decades revealed about the adverse health effects of recreational cannabis use?  Addiction. 2015;110(1):19-35.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Battistella  G, Fornari  E, Annoni  J-M,  et al.  Long-term effects of cannabis on brain structure.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(9):2041-2048.PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
 Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study. http://addictionresearch.nih.gov/adolescent-brain-cognitive-development-study. Accessed March 14, 2015.
34.
Hanson  JL, Nacewicz  BM, Sutterer  MJ,  et al.  Behavioral problems after early life stress: contributions of the hippocampus and amygdala.  Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(4):314-323.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Sartor  CE, Waldron  M, Duncan  AE,  et al.  Childhood sexual abuse and early substance use in adolescent girls: the role of familial influences.  Addiction. 2013;108(5):993-1000.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Burston  JJ, Wiley  JL, Craig  AA, Selley  DE, Sim-Selley  LJ.  Regional enhancement of cannabinoid CB&#8321: receptor desensitization in female adolescent rats following repeated Δ-tetrahydrocannabinol exposure.  Br J Pharmacol. 2010;161(1):103-112.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Rubino  T, Vigano’  D, Realini  N,  et al.  Chronic delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol during adolescence provokes sex-dependent changes in the emotional profile in adult rats: behavioral and biochemical correlates.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(11):2760-2771.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Scallet  AC.  Neurotoxicology of cannabis and THC: a review of chronic exposure studies in animals.  Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 1991;40(3):671-676.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Landfield  PW, Cadwallader  LB, Vinsant  S.  Quantitative changes in hippocampal structure following long-term exposure to delta 9-tetrahydrocannabinol: possible mediation by glucocorticoid systems.  Brain Res. 1988;443(1-2):47-62.PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×