Importance
Schizophrenia is a highly heritable, polygenic condition characterized by a relatively diverse phenotype and frequent comorbid conditions, such as anxiety and depression. At present, limited evidence explains how genetic risk for schizophrenia is manifest in the general population.
Objective
To investigate the extent to which genetic risk for schizophrenia is associated with different phenotypes during adolescence in a population-based birth cohort.
Design, Setting, and Participants
This cohort study used data from the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC). Of 14 062 children in the birth cohort, genetic data were available for 9912 adolescents. Data were collected periodically from September 6, 1990, and collection is ongoing. Data were analyzed from March 4 to August 13, 2015.
Exposures
Polygenic risk scores (PRSs) for schizophrenia generated for individuals in the ALSPAC cohort using results of the second Psychiatric Genomics Consortium Schizophrenia genome-wide association study as a training set.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Logistic regression was used to assess associations between the schizophrenia PRS and (1) psychotic experiences (Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview at 12 and 18 years of age), (2) negative symptoms (Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences at 16.5 years of age), (3) depressive disorder (Development and Well-Being Assessment at 15.5 years of age), and (4) anxiety disorder (Development and Well-Being Assessment at 15.5 years of age) in adolescence.
Results
Of the 8230 ALSPAC participants whose genetic data passed quality control checks (51.2% male, 48.8% female), 3676 to 5444 participated in assessments from 12 to 18 years of age. The PRSs created using single-nucleotide polymorphisms with a training-set P ≤ .05 threshold were associated with negative symptoms (odds ratio [OR] per SD increase in PRS, 1.21; 95% CI, 1.08-1.36; R2 = 0.007) and anxiety disorder (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.17; 95% CI, 1.06- 1.29; R2 = 0.005). No evidence was found of an association between schizophrenia PRS and psychotic experiences (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.08; 95% CI, 0.98-1.19; R2 = 0.001) or depressive disorder (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.02; 95% CI, 0.91-1.13; R2 = 0.00005). Results were mostly consistent across different training-set P value thresholds and using different cutoffs and measures of the psychopathological outcomes.
Conclusions and Relevance
This study demonstrates polygenic overlaps between common genetic polymorphisms associated with schizophrenia and negative symptoms and anxiety disorder but not with psychotic experiences or depression. Because the genetic risk for schizophrenia appears to be manifest as anxiety and negative symptoms during adolescence, a greater focus on these phenotypes rather than on psychotic experiences might be required for prediction of transition in at-risk samples.
Schizophrenia has a heritability of approximately 80%, and genome-wide association studies (GWASs) indicate that multiple independent loci contribute to its etiology.1 The importance of studying the phenotypic manifestations of increased genetic liability for schizophrenia has been long recognized and originally involved small samples of individuals at high risk as indexed by having a family history.2 Genetic advances now provide the opportunity to extend the power and generalizability of high-risk studies into the general population by examining individuals according to genetic risk. Although individual loci have small effects on risk, multiple-loci approaches show that, cumulatively, alleles on current GWAS platforms explain one-half to one-third of the genetic risk for schizophrenia.3,4 Information from even moderately associated alleles can be collapsed into a single polygenic risk score (PRS) that can be used to explore shared genetic effects with other disorders and to examine how genetic risk is manifest early during development.5
Schizophrenia is defined by psychotic experiences (hallucinations, delusions, and thought disorder) and negative symptoms (eg, blunted affect and apathy), although cognitive deficits are also common as is comorbidity with other diagnoses, particularly affective and anxiety disorders.6 Longitudinal studies show that anxiety, depression, and cognitive deficits often predate schizophrenia,7-11 indicating that these phenotypes might represent early expression of genetic risk for schizophrenia. Although genetic overlap across psychiatric disorders is common,12-15 knowledge of how genetic risk is expressed at different stages of the life course could foster understanding of the etiologic mechanisms and risk prediction and inform targeted interventions.
Three previous studies16-18 have used a schizophrenia PRS generated from a GWAS training set that captured approximately 3% of the proportion of risk variance4 to examine associations with symptoms that characterize schizophrenia. In the first study,16 the schizophrenia PRS was not associated with symptom dimensions characteristic of the disorder in cases with schizophrenia or control individuals (while associations within the whole sample were attributed to confounding by case-control status). Similarly, another study17 reported no association with positive symptom, cognitive, and negative symptom dimensions in a general population sample of adolescents. Finally, the third study18 found no strong evidence that the schizophrenia PRS was associated with psychotic experiences at 12 years of age within the population-based Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) birth cohort. Subsequently, a larger GWAS1 has been completed that explains a much greater proportion of risk variance, thus providing greater power to examine how genetic risk is manifest during development.19 We aimed to examine the psychopathological features associated with the early expression of genetic risk for schizophrenia and, more specifically, whether a schizophrenia PRS derived from the most recent GWAS is associated with (1) psychotic experiences, (2) negative symptoms, (3) anxiety disorders, or (4) depression during adolescence in a large population-based sample.
The sample consisted of young individuals within the ALSPAC cohort. The initial cohort consisted of 14 062 children born to women residing in the former Avon Health Authority area with an expected delivery date from April 1, 1991, to December 31, 1992 (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/; all available data are given at http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/access/).20,21 Data were collected periodically from September 6, 1990, and collection is ongoing. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Committee and the local research ethics committees (listed at http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/researchers/research-ethics/). All participants provided written informed consent.
Genetic data were acquired from 9912 participants using a genome-wide single-nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) genotyping platform (HumanHap550-Quad; Illumina). After quality control assessment, imputation, and restriction to 1 adolescent per family, genetic data were available for 8230 individuals (more detail is found in the eMethods in the Supplement).
The semistructured Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview22,23 was used to assess psychotic experiences (hallucinations, delusions, or experiences of thought interference) at ages 12 and 18 years. To maximize the numbers within our sample, individuals were deemed to have a psychotic experience if rated as having 1 or more definite psychotic experiences at 12 or 18 years of age compared with no experiences or suspected psychotic experiences only at 12 or 18 years of age. More details on the Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview and on the other outcome measures used in this study are given in the eMethods in the Supplement.
Negative symptoms were assessed using 10 questions based on items from the Community Assessment of Psychic Experiences self-report questionnaire24 at 16.5 years of age (eMethods and eTable 1 in the Supplement). The questions measure negative symptoms, such as apathy, anergia, and asociality. Each item was rated on a 4-point scale (0 indicates never; 1, sometimes; 2, often; and 3, always). A total score was constructed based on the sum of responses (minimum score, 0; maximum score, 30). A binary variable was created using a total score of 14 as a cutoff that was chosen to define approximately the top decile (9.18%) of the sample.
Depressive and Anxiety Disorders
Depressive and anxiety disorder outcomes were derived from the semistructured Development and Well-Being Assessment interview at 15.5 years of age. The interview is a valid instrument in community and clinical samples.25International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision and DSM-IV diagnoses of depressive or any anxiety disorder were generated using a computerized diagnostic algorithm that predicts the likelihood of a clinical diagnostic rating (more information is available at http://www.DAWBA.com).26
We defined individuals as having a depressive disorder or an anxiety disorder if they were categorized in the Development and Well-Being Assessment band predicting at least a 15% probability of clinical diagnosis, a cutoff selected to define the approximate top deciles of the sample. We conducted a series of sensitivity analyses using different phenotype score cutoffs to define binary outcomes and using different measures where available to test the robustness of our findings (eMethods in the Supplement).
Construction of the PRS follows the methods described by the International Schizophrenia Consortium.4 The PRS was constructed using results from the second Psychiatric Genomics Consortium (PGC) schizophrenia GWAS (eMethods in the Supplement).1 Polygenic scores were calculated for each ALSPAC individual using the PLINK (version 1.07) score command.27 Scores were created by summing the number of risk alleles present for each SNP (0, 1, or 2) weighted by the logarithm of its odds ratio (OR) for schizophrenia from the PGC.
Our primary analysis used scores generated from a list of SNPs with a GWAS training-set P ≤. 05 threshold, which is the threshold that maximally captures schizophrenia liability.1 Because the composition of a PRS is a balance between true and null effects,28 scores generated using lists of SNPs meeting a series of P value thresholds and using all independent SNPs meeting genome-wide significance as reported by the PGC schizophrenia GWAS1 were used in sensitivity analyses.
Data were analyzed from March 4 to August 13, 2015. We used logistic regression to test the association between outcomes and the schizophrenia PRS. Results are presented as ORs and 95% CIs per SD increase in PRS. Nagelkerke R2 values are also presented as a measure of variance explained. Nonlinear associations between the PRS and outcomes were examined by inclusion of quadratic terms. We examined whether associations with our outcomes were independent by inclusion of all phenotypes within a multivariable model. To correct for multiple testing arising from using different P value thresholds within our sensitivity analyses, we computed permutation-adjusted P values (eMethods in the Supplement).
To test whether the effect size of the schizophrenia PRS was the same or different across phenotypes, we used bivariate probit regression to model pairs of outcomes jointly. We tested equality of regression parameters expressing the effect of the schizophrenia PRS (threshold P = .05) on each outcome using a likelihood ratio test to compare a model that allows effect estimates to differ with a model where the PRS effect was constrained to be equal for both outcomes.29,30 All statistical analyses were performed using STATA statistical software (version 13; StataCorp LP).
Associations Between Schizophrenia PRS and Psychopathological Outcomes
The numbers of individuals who participated in the Psychosis-Like Symptom Interview at 12 and 18 years of age or completed questions relating to negative symptoms at 16.5 years of age and depression and anxiety at 15.5 years of age are shown in Table 1. Of 8230 ALSPAC adolescents whose genetic data passed quality control checks (51.2% male and 48.8% female), 3676 to 5444 participated in assessments from 12 to 18 years of age.
We found no strong evidence that individuals who had a higher PRS, and thus increased genetic risk for schizophrenia, had an increased risk for developing psychotic experiences (OR per SD increase in PRS, 1.08; 95% CI, 0.98-1.19; permutation-adjusted P = .14; R2 = 0.001) (Figure, A, and eTable 2 in the Supplement).
We observed strong evidence of an association between the schizophrenia PRS and negative symptoms at 16.5 years of age (OR, 1.21; 95% CI, 1.08-1.36; permutation-adjusted P = .001; R2 = 0.007) (Figure, B, and eTable 2 in the Supplement). We also observed strong evidence that individuals with a higher schizophrenia PRS were at an increased risk for anxiety disorder at 15.5 years of age (OR per SD increase in polygenic score, 1.17; 95% CI, 1.06-1.29; permutation-adjusted P = .002; R2 = 0.005) (Figure, D, and eTable 2 in the Supplement). We found no strong evidence of an association between the PRS and depressive disorder at 15.5 years of age (OR per SD increase in polygenic score, 1.02; 95% CI, 0.91-1.13; permutation-adjusted P = .77; R2 = 0.00005) (Figure, C, and eTable 2 in the Supplement).
We found no strong evidence to support nonlinear effects of polygenic risk on any of the phenotypes examined. Results per decile of PRS for a training-set threshold of P = .05 are presented in eTable 3 in the Supplement. Associations with negative symptoms and anxiety disorder were independent and persisted when testing all phenotypes within a multivariable model (eTables 4 to 7 in the Supplement).
Common and Specific Associations With Schizophrenia PRS
Tetrachoric correlations between each psychopathological outcome are shown in eTable 8 in the Supplement. The results of the bivariate analyses examining whether the schizophrenia PRS (threshold P = .05) effect sizes are similar or different across phenotypes are summarized in Table 2. We observed some evidence that the strong associations between the schizophrenia PRS and negative symptoms and anxiety are different from the association between the PRS and depression (both P = .02), with weaker evidence that they are different from the associations of the PRS with psychotic experiences (P = .14 and P = .11, respectively). We found no strong evidence that the association between the PRS effect and negative symptoms differed from that for anxiety (P = .92).
Our results were unchanged when we used different cutoff values or different measurement tools to assess our outcomes (eFigures 1-5 in the Supplement), when we adjusted for parental self-reported history of schizophrenia or depression, or when we excluded individuals with a psychotic disorder at 18 years of age. Results were also consistent across all training-set P value thresholds for almost all outcomes. The exception was psychotic experiences, for which we found weak evidence that increased genetic risk was associated with a decreased risk of psychotic experiences at lower training-set P value thresholds (threshold P ≤ 1 × 10−7; OR per SD increase in polygenic score, 0.90; 95% CI, 0.81-0.99; permutation-adjusted P = .04; R2 = 0.002) (Figure, A, and eTable 2 in the Supplement).
In this study, we examined how an increased genetic risk for schizophrenia is manifest phenotypically during adolescence in the general population. We found no strong evidence of an association with the occurrence of psychotic experiences or depressive disorder. However, we found strong evidence that negative symptoms and anxiety disorders were more common in adolescents with a higher genetic risk and that these risks were independent of each other.
Although the absence of an association between a genetic risk for schizophrenia and psychotic experiences in adolescence may seem surprising, these findings are consistent with those of previous studies that have examined this relationship.17,18 The estimates of association and strength of evidence in this study are very similar to those from a previous study using ALSPAC,18 although the power in our study is substantially greater given the use of a much larger training set to generate the risk-scoring algorithm.
At P value thresholds that maximally capture schizophrenia liability, psychotic experiences were more common in those with higher genetic risk, albeit the CIs included the null. At the most stringent P value thresholds, however, we found weak evidence that genetic risk was associated with reduced psychotic experiences. This finding could be caused by random error or could result from attrition bias. Missing data are likely greater for those who develop a psychotic disorder and for those at high genetic risk. Therefore, psychotic experiences may be underrepresented in participants with high compared with low genetic risk included in these analyses, akin to the apparently protective effect of smoking on Alzheimer disease risk seen using risk rather than rate models of analysis.31 Why this would only be observed at P value thresholds that explain less of the variance for schizophrenia is not clear.
A number of potential explanations exist for our findings of strong evidence of an association between genetic risk for schizophrenia and negative symptoms and anxiety disorder but not with psychotic experiences in the general population. First, genetic risk for schizophrenia may be expressed heterotypically during adolescence as anxiety and negative symptoms, and psychotic experiences may develop later. For the minority of individuals who later develop schizophrenia, anxiety and negative symptoms would represent prodromal symptoms of the disorder. This possibility implies that, compared with psychotic experiences arising later in life, hallucinations and paranoid beliefs arising during adolescence might be explained to a greater degree by nongenetic effects, such as childhood trauma32,33 or cannabis use,34 than by genetic risk for schizophrenia. The association between genetic risk for schizophrenia and psychotic experiences might therefore become stronger with increasing age akin to that seen for general cognitive ability.35
Anxiety and negative symptoms might occur as early manifestations of genetic risk for schizophrenia, and we might speculate on possible mechanisms. For example, disturbed biological processes secondary to genetic variation might result in subtle alterations in prediction error processing and attributional salience that might lead to anxiety before the onset of clear-cut psychotic phenomena.36,37 Genetic risk for schizophrenia is also associated with impaired childhood performance IQ in this cohort,38 although we do not know whether variants acting primarily through different brain pathways are differentially related to these phenotypes.
Second, genetic risk for schizophrenia may be expressed during adolescence as increased psychotic experiences, anxiety, and negative symptoms, but psychotic experiences may be observed with greater measurement error; thus, associations would be relatively underpowered for this phenotype. Use of a semistructured interview and similar estimates using questionnaires suggest that this explanation is unlikely but cannot be excluded.
Third, genetic variants identified as showing an association with schizophrenia in the GWAS may only weakly index the risk for hallucinations and delusions and may reflect genetic risk more strongly for other characteristics of the disorder, such as negative symptoms that index severity or chronicity of illness and that might be selected for in clinically ascertained samples. Similarly, such ascertainment might be biased toward those with multiple morbidities, for example, comorbid anxiety disorders. Studies that have examined symptom dimensions within schizophrenia39 and in the general population40,41 show that the heritability of negative and disorganized symptoms is greater than that of positive symptoms and that schizophrenia polygenic risk is more strongly associated with negative and disorganized symptoms than positive symptoms.42
Although evidence exists of genetic overlap between schizophrenia and major depressive disorder in adults,12,43 we found no evidence to support this overlap in our study. Measures of depression in this study may capture more transient disorders in adolescence that obscure a genetic overlap between schizophrenia and a more persistent, chronic form of depression.
Our results indicate that anxiety and negative symptoms are likely to be the best markers of high genetic risk for schizophrenia in population-based samples; however, the variance of these phenotypes explained by schizophrenia genetic risk is small (0.5%-0.7%), and the negative symptoms measure used in the general population might not fully capture the negative symptoms seen in schizophrenia. Our findings have potentially important implications for studies of at-risk samples among whom current approaches for informing prediction of transition rely heavily on psychotic experiences. Our results are consistent with an evolving literature describing anxiety as a common symptom during the prodromal stage of psychosis.44-46
Our study has a number of strengths. First, we used the most recent schizophrenia GWAS from the PGC1—the largest schizophrenia data set available—as a training set, thus minimizing measurement error. Second, we used a large, well-characterized, relatively homogeneous, population-based sample for examining psychosis-related phenotypes during adolescence, a key period of development that closely predates the start of the peak in incidence of schizophrenia. Third, we used a semistructured interview to determine the presence of psychotic experiences, as used clinically. We also used multiple measures of depression and anxiety at different ages during adolescence and different cutoffs of these and of measures of psychotic experiences and negative symptoms as sensitivity analyses to test the robustness of our findings.
Our study also has a number of limitations. First, despite the use of one of the largest population-based birth cohorts worldwide with the required data, our sample may not be adequately powered to identify small effects of cumulative genetic risk on the phenotypes examined,19 especially given potential differences in heritability across the phenotypes.14,40,41 Given that our sample is too small to estimate heritability of the phenotypes accurately, determining whether the absence of evidence of association between a phenotype and genetic risk for schizophrenia reflects an absence of genetic correlation or inadequate power is difficult. Second, missing data in the cohort could lead to bias in our estimates. Genetic risk for schizophrenia47 and presence of mental health problems are associated with reduced retention in cohort studies, which would tend to underestimate associations. Although selective missingness may have led to overestimates of association, this possibility seems unlikely for some of our phenotypes but not others. A further limitation is that rare genetic variants are not captured by a GWAS; therefore, we are only able to examine the effect of common variants (as captured by the current GWAS) on adolescent phenotype expression.
Our results highlight the need for GWAS consortia of schizophrenia to include detailed phenotyping data, to examine the extent to which current GWAS findings relate to specific phenotypes, and to identify genetic variants and pathways that are symptom-domain specific rather than to examine the presence of disorder per se. Furthermore, large population-based longitudinal studies with robust measures of these phenotypic constructs are required to determine how genetic risk for schizophrenia is expressed from childhood through adulthood and whether this expression changes with age, to examine potential mediators and moderators of risk, and to determine the usefulness of genetic risk scores for prediction of transition to psychosis. A better understanding of how genetic risk for schizophrenia manifests during development could inform early recognition of problems in those at greatest risk and potentially inform targeted interventions.
Corresponding Author: Hannah J. Jones, PhD, Centre for Academic Mental Health, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, Oakfield Grove, Bristol BS8 2BN, England (hannah.jones@bristol.ac.uk).
Submitted for Publication: September 25, 2015; final revision received November 20, 2015; accepted November 22, 2015.
Published Online: January 27, 2016. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.3058.
Author Contributions: Drs H. J. Jones and Zammit had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: H. J. Jones, Stergiakouli, Davey Smith, O’Donovan, Owen, Walters, Zammit.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: H. J. Jones, Tansey, Hubbard, Heron, Cannon, Holmans, Lewis, Linden, P. B. Jones, Davey Smith, O’Donovan, Walters, Zammit.
Drafting of the manuscript: H. J. Jones, Cannon, O’Donovan, Zammit.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: H. J. Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, Heron, Holmans.
Obtained funding: Cannon, Lewis, Linden, P. B. Jones, Davey Smith, O’Donovan, Owen, Zammit.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Stergiakouli, Lewis, Davey Smith, O’Donovan.
Study supervision: Stergiakouli, Heron, Davey Smith, O’Donovan, Zammit.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr O’Donovan received a consultancy fee from Roche in July 2015. No other disclosures were reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by grants G0701503, MR/M006727/1, and MR/K004360/1 from the Medical Research Council, grant 102215/2/13/2 from the Wellcome Trust, and the University of Bristol through core support for the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) and by 23andMe, which supported generation of the genome-wide association data by Sample Logistics and genotyping facilities at the Wellcome Trust Sanger Institute and Laboratory Corporation of America. Drs H. J. Jones, Stergiakouli, Tansey, and Davey Smith are part of the Medical Research Council Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol, which is supported by grant MC_UU_12013/1 from the Medical Research Council and the University of Bristol.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: We thank all the families who participated in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting the families, and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists, and nurses.
1.Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci.
Nature. 2014;511(7510):421-427.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Rasic
D, Hajek
T, Alda
M, Uher
R. Risk of mental illness in offspring of parents with schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and major depressive disorder: a meta-analysis of family high-risk studies.
Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(1):28-38.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Ripke
S, O’Dushlaine
C, Chambert
K,
et al; Multicenter Genetic Studies of Schizophrenia Consortium; Psychosis Endophenotypes International Consortium; Wellcome Trust Case Control Consortium 2. Genome-wide association analysis identifies 13 new risk loci for schizophrenia.
Nat Genet. 2013;45(10):1150-1159.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Purcell
SM, Wray
NR, Stone
JL,
et al; International Schizophrenia Consortium. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
Nature. 2009;460(7256):748-752.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 6.Buckley
PF, Miller
BJ, Lehrer
DS, Castle
DJ. Psychiatric comorbidities and schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 2009;35(2):383-402.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Docherty
JP, Van Kammen
DP, Siris
SG, Marder
SR. Stages of onset of schizophrenic psychosis.
Am J Psychiatry. 1978;135(4):420-426.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Tien
AY, Eaton
WW. Psychopathologic precursors and sociodemographic risk factors for the schizophrenia syndrome.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49(1):37-46.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Jones
P, Rodgers
B, Murray
R, Marmot
M. Child development risk factors for adult schizophrenia in the British 1946 birth cohort.
Lancet. 1994;344(8934):1398-1402.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Turnbull
G, Bebbington
P. Anxiety and the schizophrenic process: clinical and epidemiological evidence.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2001;36(5):235-243.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Khandaker
GM, Barnett
JH, White
IR, Jones
PB. A quantitative meta-analysis of population-based studies of premorbid intelligence and schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res. 2011;132(2-3):220-227.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Lee
SH, Ripke
S, Neale
BM,
et al; Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium; International Inflammatory Bowel Disease Genetics Consortium (IIBDGC). Genetic relationship between five psychiatric disorders estimated from genome-wide SNPs.
Nat Genet. 2013;45(9):984-994.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Hamshere
ML, Stergiakouli
E, Langley
K,
et al. Shared polygenic contribution between childhood attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and adult schizophrenia.
Br J Psychiatry. 2013;203(2):107-111.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium. Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis.
Lancet. 2013;381(9875):1371-1379.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Demirkan
A, Penninx
BW, Hek
K,
et al. Genetic risk profiles for depression and anxiety in adult and elderly cohorts.
Mol Psychiatry. 2011;16(7):773-783.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Derks
EM, Vorstman
JAS, Ripke
S, Kahn
RS, Ophoff
RA; Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genomic Consortium. Investigation of the genetic association between quantitative measures of psychosis and schizophrenia: a polygenic risk score analysis.
PLoS One. 2012;7(6):e37852.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Sieradzka
D, Power
RA, Freeman
D,
et al. Are genetic risk factors for psychosis also associated with dimension-specific psychotic experiences in adolescence?
PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e94398.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Zammit
S, Hamshere
M, Dwyer
S,
et al. A population-based study of genetic variation and psychotic experiences in adolescents.
Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(6):1254-1262.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Fraser
A, Macdonald-Wallis
C, Tilling
K,
et al. Cohort profile: the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children: ALSPAC mothers cohort.
Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):97-110.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Boyd
A, Golding
J, Macleod
J,
et al. Cohort profile: the “children of the 90s”: the index offspring of the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children.
Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):111-127.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Horwood
J, Salvi
G, Thomas
K,
et al. IQ and non-clinical psychotic symptoms in 12-year-olds: results from the ALSPAC birth cohort.
Br J Psychiatry. 2008;193(3):185-191.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Zammit
S, Kounali
D, Cannon
M,
et al. Psychotic experiences and psychotic disorders at age 18 in relation to psychotic experiences at age 12 in a longitudinal population-based cohort study.
Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(7):742-750.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Stefanis
NC, Hanssen
M, Smirnis
NK,
et al. Evidence that three dimensions of psychosis have a distribution in the general population.
Psychol Med. 2002;32(2):347-358.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Goodman
R, Ford
T, Richards
H, Gatward
R, Meltzer
H. The Development and Well-Being Assessment: description and initial validation of an integrated assessment of child and adolescent psychopathology.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000;41(5):645-655.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Goodman
A, Heiervang
E, Collishaw
S, Goodman
R. The “DAWBA bands” as an ordered-categorical measure of child mental health: description and validation in British and Norwegian samples.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2011;46(6):521-532.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Purcell
S, Neale
B, Todd-Brown
K,
et al. PLINK: a tool set for whole-genome association and population-based linkage analyses.
Am J Hum Genet. 2007;81(3):559-575.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Iyegbe
C, Campbell
D, Butler
A, Ajnakina
O, Sham
P. The emerging molecular architecture of schizophrenia, polygenic risk scores and the clinical implications for GxE research.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(2):169-182.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Amemiya
T. Qualitative response models: a survey.
J Econ Lit. 1981;19(4):1483-1536.
Google Scholar 30.Kounali
D, Zammit
S, Wiles
N,
et al. Common versus psychopathology-specific risk factors for psychotic experiences and depression during adolescence.
Psychol Med. 2014;44(12):2557-2566.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Fratiglioni
L, Wang
HX. Smoking and Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease: review of the epidemiological studies.
Behav Brain Res. 2000;113(1-2):117-120.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Bendall
S, Jackson
HJ, Hulbert
CA, McGorry
PD. Childhood trauma and psychotic disorders: a systematic, critical review of the evidence.
Schizophr Bull. 2008;34(3):568-579.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Kelleher
I, Keeley
H, Corcoran
P,
et al. Childhood trauma and psychosis in a prospective cohort study: cause, effect, and directionality.
Am J Psychiatry. 2013;170(7):734-741.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Moore
THM, Zammit
S, Lingford-Hughes
A,
et al. Cannabis use and risk of psychotic or affective mental health outcomes: a systematic review.
Lancet. 2007;370(9584):319-328.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.McIntosh
AM, Gow
A, Luciano
M,
et al. Polygenic risk for schizophrenia is associated with cognitive change between childhood and old age.
Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(10):938-943.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Heinz
A. Dopaminergic dysfunction in alcoholism and schizophrenia: psychopathological and behavioral correlates.
Eur Psychiatry. 2002;17(1):9-16.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Heinz
A, Schlagenhauf
F. Dopaminergic dysfunction in schizophrenia: salience attribution revisited.
Schizophr Bull. 2010;36(3):472-485.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Hubbard
L, Tansey
KE, Rai
D,
et al. Evidence of common genetic overlap between schizophrenia and cognition [published online December 16, 2015].
Schizophr Bull.
Google Scholar 39.Dworkin
RH, Lenzenweger
MF, Moldin
SO, Skillings
GF, Levick
SE. A multidimensional approach to the genetics of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145(9):1077-1083.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Zavos
HM, Freeman
D, Haworth
CM,
et al. Consistent etiology of severe, frequent psychotic experiences and milder, less frequent manifestations: a twin study of specific psychotic experiences in adolescence.
JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(9):1049-1057.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Sieradzka
D, Power
RA, Freeman
D, Cardno
AG, Dudbridge
F, Ronald
A. Heritability of individual psychotic experiences captured by common genetic variants in a community sample of adolescents.
Behav Genet. 2015;45(5):493-502.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Fanous
AH, Zhou
B, Aggen
SH,
et al; Schizophrenia Psychiatric Genome-Wide Association Study (GWAS) Consortium. Genome-wide association study of clinical dimensions of schizophrenia: polygenic effect on disorganized symptoms.
Am J Psychiatry. 2012;169(12):1309-1317.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Schulze
TG, Akula
N, Breuer
R,
et al; Bipolar Genome Study. Molecular genetic overlap in bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, and major depressive disorder.
World J Biol Psychiatry. 2014;15(3):200-208.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Fusar-Poli
P, Nelson
B, Valmaggia
L, Yung
AR, McGuire
PK. Comorbid depressive and anxiety disorders in 509 individuals with an at-risk mental state: impact on psychopathology and transition to psychosis.
Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(1):120-131.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Fusar-Poli
P, Yung
AR, McGorry
P, van Os
J. Lessons learned from the psychosis high-risk state: towards a general staging model of prodromal intervention.
Psychol Med. 2014;44(1):17-24.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Martin
J, Tilling
K, Hubbard
L,
et al. Genetic risk for schizophrenia associated with non-participation over time in a population-based cohort study.
Am J Epidemiol. In press.
Google Scholar