Importance
Marijuana use may alter ventral striatal response to reward, which might heighten susceptibility to substance use disorder. Longitudinal research is needed to determine the effects of marijuana use on neural function involved in reward response.
Objective
To determine whether marijuana use among young adults prospectively affects nucleus accumbens (NAcc) activation during reward anticipation.
Design, Setting, and Participants
One hundred eight young adults were recruited from the Michigan Longitudinal Study, an ongoing study of youth at high risk for substance use disorder and a contrast sample of control families. Participants underwent 3 consecutive functional magnetic resonance imaging scans at approximate ages of 20 (time 1), 22 (time 2), and 24 (time 3) years. Self-report data on marijuana and other drug use occasions were collected annually since age 11 years.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Cross-lagged models were used to test the association of marijuana use with neural response in the NAcc to reward anticipation during a monetary incentive delay task controlling for sex, age, other substance use, and family history of substance use disorder.
Results
Of 108 participants, 39 (36.1%) were female and mean (SD) age at baseline was 20.1 (1.4) years. Greater marijuana use was associated with later blunted activation in the NAcc during reward anticipation (time 1 to time 2: β = −0.26, P = .04; time 2 to time 3: β = −0.25, P = .01). When the cross-lagged model was tested with the inclusion of previous and concurrent cigarette use, the effect of marijuana use from time 2 to time 3 remained significant (β = −0.29; P = .005) and the effect of cigarette use was nonsignificant.
Conclusions and Relevance
The findings of this study indicate that marijuana use is associated with decreased neural response in the NAcc during the anticipation of nondrug rewards. Over time, marijuana use may alter anticipatory reward processing in the NAcc, which may increase the risk for continued drug use and later addiction.
Marijuana is the most commonly used illicit substance in the United States and is especially prevalent among young adults.1 Marijuana use typically emerges during adolescence, peaking in the early 20s, with 35% of 21- and 22- year-old individuals reporting past year use.2 This pervasiveness may be due to low perceptions of harm,2 despite the short- and long-term negative consequences3 of marijuana use. Short-term consequences include acute anxiety, paranoia, and altered perception,4 as well as impaired motor coordination while driving.5 Deficits in academic achievement6 and disrupted brain structure and function7-10 suggest long-term negative consequences of marijuana use. Of particular relevance to the present study is evidence that earlier onset of marijuana use leads to a faster transition to marijuana dependence11 and increases the risk of developing other substance use disorders (SUDs).12
Marijuana’s psychoactive properties are due largely to Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol, which binds to CB1 cannabinoid neural receptors and stimulates the transmission of dopamine from the ventral tegmental area to the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) in the ventral striatum. The ventral striatum is a region involved in reward-driven behavior, including substance use.13-15 A recent positron emission tomography study16 demonstrated reduced reactivity of this system in long-term marijuana users during extracellular dopamine stimulation, which was correlated with greater addiction severity and craving. This finding is consistent with theories positing that, with long-term use, the incentive salience of cues that predict receipt of the drug strengthens, likely at the expense of rewards not associated with that drug. Such an effect is believed to contribute to the development and maintenance of SUD.17-19 Thus, marijuana use may cause a perturbation of the dopaminergic reward system, thereby increasing risk for developing SUDs.
A number of cross-sectional studies20-22 have investigated the effect of marijuana use on nondrug reward response during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) with the monetary incentive delay task. Monetary reward anticipation during the monetary incentive delay task reliably and robustly activates the ventral striatum, including the NAcc. Some studies23,24 report that marijuana users show increased activation of the ventral striatum to monetary reward anticipation versus nonusers, whereas other investigations25-27 have found no differences. However, cross-sectional studies may be unable to disentangle differences in activation due to preexisting neural susceptibilities compared with altered neural function as a result of marijuana use. Increased NAcc activation to reward anticipation has been linked to behavioral traits associated with substance use risk22,28-30 and prospectively associated with later substance use problems.21 Thus, longitudinal studies that account for preexisting susceptibilities are needed to more clearly identify the cumulative effect of marijuana use on neural mechanisms underlying reward responsivity.
To address this gap, the present study examined cross-lagged prospective associations between marijuana use and NAcc activation to monetary reward anticipation. Longitudinal fMRI during the monetary incentive delay task was conducted in young adults at the approximate ages of 20, 22, and 24 years, coinciding with the normative peak age of marijuana use.2 Consistent with prior evidence supporting attenuated dopamine release in the ventral striatum among heavy marijuana users16 and reduced incentive salience for nondrug rewards in long-term substance users,17,18 we hypothesized that greater marijuana use would be associated with later decreases in NAcc activation to monetary reward anticipation beyond possible cofounding influences.
Box Section Ref IDKey Points
Question Does marijuana use alter nucleus accumbens (NAcc) responsivity to monetary reward anticipation?
Findings In this longitudinal study that included 108 young adults, cross-lagged analyses indicated an association between marijuana use and decreased neural response in the NAcc during anticipation of monetary rewards.
Meaning Over time, marijuana use may blunt the responsivity of the reward system to nondrug incentives; this may be a factor underlying continued drug use and addiction.
Participants were 108 young adults recruited from the Michigan Longitudinal Study, an ongoing prospective study of youth from families with high levels of SUD and a contrast sample of families without SUD.31 A total of 84 participants (77.8%) were categorized as high risk based on parental history of SUD, which was ascertained by a clinical psychologist using the Diagnostic Interview Schedule–Version 4.32 Exclusion criteria are presented in the eMethods in the Supplement. No minimum levels of marijuana use were required for inclusion in the present study. All eligible participants were assessed for psychosocial functioning at 3-year intervals and substance use and problems beginning annually at age 11 years. Additional details on Michigan Longitudinal Study assessment and data collection protocol are provided elsewhere.31,33 Participants completed 3 consecutive fMRI scans at approximately 2-year intervals. The present study was reviewed and approved by the University of Michigan Medical School Institutional Review Board, and all participants provided written informed consent and received financial compensation. The Table displays participant characteristics, including race/ethnicity and sex.
Substance use was assessed annually with the Drinking and Drug History questionnaire.34 Past-year marijuana use was measured by the number of days during the past year that participants reported using marijuana or hashish. Past-year binge drinking was measured by the number of days during the past year that participants reported drinking 5 or more standard alcoholic drinks. Past-year cigarette use was measured by the number of days participants reported smoking cigarettes during the past year. For each substance, previous use was the number of days of use from age 11 through the year before time 1. Substance use data are reported in the Table.
To assess neural response during anticipation of monetary reward, participants performed a modified version of the monetary incentive delay task.20-22 Each trial began with an incentive cue indicating whether they could win or lose a small ($0.20) or large ($5.00) monetary reward or whether no money was at stake (cue). A fixation cross then appeared (anticipation) and was followed by a variable-duration target. Pressing the button while the target was on the screen signified a correct response. After each trial, participants were shown feedback indicating whether they pressed the button quickly enough (outcome). Additional details are provided in the eMethods in the Supplement.
fMRI Data Acquisition and Analysis
Whole-brain blood oxygen level–dependent images were acquired on a 3-T scanner (Signa; GE Healthcare) using a T2*-weighted single-shot combined spiral in/out sequence35 (repetition time [TR], 2000 milliseconds; echo time [TE], 30 milliseconds; flip angle, 90°; field of view [FOV], 200 mm; 64 × 64 matrix; in-plane resolution, 3.12 × 3.12 mm; section thickness, 4 mm).34 In addition, a high-resolution, anatomic, T1-weighted scan was obtained (TR, 25 milliseconds; minimum TE; FOV, 25 cm; 256 × 256 matrix; section thickness, 1.4 mm). Image processing and individual-level analyses are described in the eMethods in the Supplement.
The present report focuses on the contrast between anticipation during monetary gain trials and neutral trials (ie, reward anticipation). Contrasts for small and large gains were estimated separately. Given the problems associated with the circularity of statistical inference when defining volumes of interest based on observed contrast activation36 and considering the present study’s a priori interest in the NAcc, anatomic masks of the left and right NAcc were created as described previously22,28 using the Build ROI function in MarsBaR37 (eFigure 1 in the Supplement). Contrasts for small and large gains were then linearly combined with the mean determined across hemispheres. Associations between incentive amount and hemisphere in NAcc volume are described in the eMethods in the Supplement. Acceptable reliability of NAcc activation across sessions has been reported.21
Cross-Lagged Model Analysis
To examine longitudinal associations between marijuana use and NAcc activation during monetary reward anticipation, cross-lagged analyses were conducted using robust maximum likelihood estimation in Mplus, version 7.2.38 Log transformations were performed to improve the normality of the marijuana use variable (transformed variable skewness: time 1, 1.56; time 2, 1.62; and time 3, 1.83; and transformed variable kurtosis: time 1, 1.42; time 2, 1.30; and time 3, 1.82). Cross-lagged analyses included across-time stability coefficients for marijuana use and NAcc activation, within-time associations between marijuana use and NAcc activation, and across-time cross-lagged paths between marijuana use and NAcc activation.
Covariates were sex, age at time 1, parental history of SUD, previous marijuana use and binge drinking, and past-year binge drinking. Age at time 1 was included to control for the range in ages (18-24 years) at the initial scan. Parental history of SUD was included to control for heightened risk of substance use.39 Because of evidence3,7 suggesting an effect of onset and duration of marijuana use on brain structure and function, we accounted for previous marijuana use. We also accounted for previous and past-year binge drinking, given the high comorbidity between marijuana and alcohol use.40-42 Because marijuana use is also frequently seen with cigarette smoking41 and some studies43,44 suggest that nicotine use may reduce the influence of Δ9-tetrahydrocannabinol on NAcc response to nondrug reward anticipation, we tested an additional analytic model that included previous and past-year cigarette use as covariates. The following parameters determined adequate model fit: nonsignificant χ2, root mean square error of approximation below 0.06, and a comparative fit index and Tucker-Lewis Index of 0.95 or higher.45 Significant pathways were established based on α = .05. To allow comparison with earlier cross-sectional studies,23-25,27 we also investigated associations between time 1 NAcc activation and previous marijuana use and age at first use with partial correlations, controlling for previous binge drinking and cigarette use.
Reaction times and accuracy data are provided in the eResults and eTable 1 in the Supplement. Partial correlations were conducted to test associations between marijuana use and task performance, controlling for sex, age at time 1, parental history of SUD, previous marijuana use and binge drinking, and concurrent past-year binge drinking. No significant (α = .05) partial correlations were found between marijuana use and task performance (eTable 2 in the Supplement). Participants showed robust ventral striatum activation at each time during reward anticipation (eFigure 2 in the Supplement).
Marijuana-NAcc Cross-Lagged Associations
Partial correlations found no association between NAcc activation at time 1 and previous marijuana use (r = 0.10; P = .48) and a marginally significant negative correlation between age of first marijuana use and NAcc activation at time 1 (r = −0.23; P = .07). The cross-lagged model showed excellent model fit and significant cross-lagged paths between marijuana use and blunted NAcc activation during monetary reward anticipation (Figure 1). Past-year marijuana use at time 1 was negatively associated with NAcc activation at time 2 (β = −0.26; P = .04), with no covariates reaching significance. Past-year marijuana use at time 2 was negatively associated with NAcc activation at time 3 (β = −0.25; P = .01), with significant covariates of previous marijuana use (β = 0.31; P = .007) and previous binge drinking (β = −0.17; P = .01). Stability coefficients were significant for past-year marijuana use (time 1-2: β = 0.48, P < .001; time 2-3: β = 0.76, P < .001) and for NAcc activation (time 1-2: β = 0.26, P = .003; time 2-3: β = 0.35, P < .001). A graphical representation of cross-lagged results is shown in Figure 2. Testing positive for marijuana use but reporting abstinence within 48 hours before each scan did not greatly affect the findings (eResults in the Supplement).
A cross-lagged model including previous and past-year cigarette use also fit well (χ2 = 14.53; P = .27; root mean square error of approximation, 0.04; comparative fit index, 0.99; Tucker-Lewis Index, 0.94). Stability coefficients were significant for past-year marijuana use (time 1-2: β = 0.44, P = .003; time 2-3: β = 0.73, P < .001) and NAcc activation (time 1-2: β = 0.27, P = .002; time 2-3: β = 0.36, P < .001). The negative association between time 2 marijuana use and time 3 NAcc activation remained significant (β = −0.29; P = .005) with significant covariates of previous marijuana use (β = 0.42; P = .001) and previous binge drinking (β = −0.17; P = .008); however, the association between time 1 marijuana use and time 2 NAcc activation was only marginally significant (β = −0.24; P = .07), with no covariates reaching significance. Pearson correlation coefficients were examined between marijuana use and other substances of interest included in cross-lagged analyses (eTable 3 in the Supplement).
To our knowledge, the present study is the first to report longitudinal associations between marijuana use and NAcc activation during a nondrug reward anticipation task. We found a significant prospective association between marijuana use and decreased NAcc activation to monetary reward anticipation. Our findings indicate that continued marijuana use may result in a blunted NAcc response to nondrug rewards, even when controlling for previous and concurrent substance use. The model also controlled for potential risk factors, such as familial risk for SUD and baseline differences in NAcc activation. This work provides robust evidence that marijuana use has long-term associations with anticipatory reward processing.
Although previous studies have shown increased NAcc activation to monetary reward anticipation among marijuana users23,24 or no significant differences from controls,25-27 we found decreased NAcc activation over time. Prior cross-sectional studies23-27 may not have been able to account for preexisting differences in NAcc activation that predisposed certain individuals toward engaging in reward-seeking behavior, such as substance use. At time 1, we found a marginal association between greater NAcc activation and earlier age at onset of marijuana use, but no association with the amount, supporting the view that heightened reward system activation represents a risk factor for use rather than a consequence. Using prospective, cross-lagged analyses, we isolated the association between marijuana use and later NAcc function more directly while accounting for baseline NAcc activation at time 1. These findings indicate that continued marijuana use affects anticipatory reward processing, whereas preexisting differences in NAcc function were not associated with later marijuana use. Given that the baseline measurement in this study was conducted during young adulthood, future prospective studies at earlier ages may more clearly detect preexisting striatal risk associated with marijuana use.
Finding a prospective association between marijuana use and decreased NAcc activation to monetary reward supports a mechanism through which marijuana use may lead to enhanced vulnerability to SUD. Although early substance use involves voluntary decisions, our findings support the view that continued consumption produces long-term alterations in neural circuits involved in reward processing, which may in turn contribute to drug-seeking behavior and compulsive use.19 One possible mechanism is that the effects of long-term marijuana use on these neural systems results in a general blunting of reward response. This blunting may lead to further drug use in an attempt to counteract insufficient reward responsivity, which is consistent with the reward deficiency theory of addiction.46 We did not observe an association between NAcc response to monetary reward and later marijuana use. Although a direct robust association may not exist between these factors, repeated use may result in a bias toward marijuana and marijuana-associated cues relative to unrelated cues. Continued stimulation of mesolimbic neural circuits during marijuana use may sensitize this circuitry to attribute greater salience to stimuli paired with marijuana use.47 This bias toward marijuana cues may contribute to long-term use of the substance and an increased risk for addiction. To test this theory, it will be necessary for future studies to prospectively examine neural response to both marijuana-associated and nondrug reward cues over time.
An alternative hypothesis is that blunted reward system response in marijuana users is associated with general anhedonia, which may maintain substance use. This association may be more specific to marijuana, given that endogenous cannabinoids have been shown48 to be involved in regulating emotional responses, and animal work49,50 has demonstrated a link between exposure to exogenous cannabinoids and depressive phenotypes. Marijuana may be used to cope with dysregulated mood, and continued use may alter neural systems involved in mood and emotion (eg, dopaminergic reward system). Attenuated striatal reactivity to dopamine stimulation in long-term marijuana users has been shown16 to correlate with negative emotionality. This is also supported by prior work51 demonstrating a prospective association between marijuana use and greater negative emotionality.
Given the high incidence of polysubstance use41 and the fact that all drugs of abuse affect dopamine levels in the NAcc,13-15 we accounted for potentially confounding substance use. For example, there is evidence52,53 that heavy alcohol use may blunt NAcc activation to reward anticipation. We found an effect of long-term marijuana use on anticipatory reward processing when controlling for potential effects of previous and concurrent binge drinking. Thus, marijuana use may have a specific influence on NAcc response beyond that of heavy alcohol use. Cumulative marijuana use and binge drinking before time 1 had a significant effect on NAcc activation at time 3 but not at time 2. This finding was likely due to the more proximal influence of marijuana use at time 1 accounting for a large degree of the variance of time 2 NAcc activation, resulting in less unexplained variance to be accounted for by previous marijuana use and binge drinking. In contrast, there may have been more unexplained variance to be accounted for by previous marijuana use and binge drinking at time 2 given the weaker correlation (eTable 3 in the Supplement).
Owing to earlier work43,44 showing blunted NAcc during monetary reward anticipation among marijuana users who also smoke cigarettes, we tested a cross-lagged model that included both variables. The association between marijuana use at time 1 and later NAcc activation was reduced to marginal significance, whereas the association between marijuana use at time 2 and later NAcc activation remained significant. This difference in the influence of cigarette use on the model over time is likely due to a higher correlation between marijuana and cigarette use at time 1 than at time 2 (r = 0.42 and r = 0.25, respectively) (eTable 3 in the Supplement). However, no significant effects of previous or concurrent cigarette use on NAcc activation were observed at any time. Our findings suggest that reduced NAcc activation was not being driven by nicotine effects on endocannabinoid modulation of the NAcc.43 More specific cross-lagged group comparisons between marijuana, cigarette, and combined marijuana and cigarette users may further disentangle the association between marijuana and nicotine on anticipatory reward processing.
One limitation of our study is that the binge drinking threshold was 5 or more standard alcoholic drinks for both men and women, which is greater than the typical cutoff of 4 or more drinks for women.54 Our findings also may have limited generalizability, given that the sample consisted predominately of white and high-risk (defined by family history of SUD) young adults. However, in many ways this was an appropriate sample, given that both white youth2 and youth with a family history of SUD39 tend to show high levels of substance use. Finally, early vs late onset of marijuana use could be an important moderator, but we did not have an adequate number of early-onset users to test for such differences. Future studies with larger sample sizes should examine whether these associations differ across the age of onset of marijuana use.
The present study provides evidence for the longitudinal effect of marijuana use on NAcc functioning during monetary reward anticipation. Because of the rise in marijuana use rates coupled with decreasing perceptions of harm,2 this study provides important and what we believe to be novel information pertaining to the long-term influence of marijuana use on brain mechanisms underlying addiction. Educating youth about the influence of marijuana on this neural circuitry and its potential downstream consequences on addiction vulnerability may help improve efforts to prevent the initiation and escalation of marijuana use. Such efforts may dispel the notion that marijuana use produces no lasting neural influences and may elucidate the potential harms associated with this substance.
Corresponding Author: Mary M. Heitzeg, PhD, Addiction Research Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Michigan, 4250 Plymouth Rd, Ann Arbor, MI 48109 (mheitzeg@umich.edu).
Submitted for Publication: December 20, 2015; final revision received April 15, 2016; accepted April 17, 2016.
Published Online: July 6, 2016. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.1161.
Author Contributions: Dr Heitzeg had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: Martz, Jester, Zucker, Heitzeg.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: Martz, Cope, Hardee, Heitzeg.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: All authors.
Statistical analysis: Martz, Trucco, Cope, Hardee, Jester.
Obtained funding: Zucker, Heitzeg.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Cope, Zucker.
Study supervision: Cope, Heitzeg.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This research was funded by grants R01 DA027261 (Drs Zucker and Heitzeg), T32 DA007267, and T32 DA007268 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse; and grants R01 AA12217 and R01 AA07065 (Drs Zucker and Heitzeg), T32 AA07477, and K08 AA023290 (Dr Trucco) from the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
1.Center for Behavioral Health Statistics and Quality. Behavioral Health Trends in the United States: Results From the 2014 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: HHS Publication No. SMA 15-4927, NSDUH Series H-50. Rockville, MD: Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration; 2015.
2.Johnston
LD, O’Malley
PM, Bachman
JG, Schulenberg
J, Miech
RA. Monitoring the Future: National Survey Results on Drug Use, 1975-2014, Volume II: College Students and Adults Ages 19-55. Ann Arbor: Institute for Social Research, The University of Michigan; 2015.
3.Volkow
ND, Baler
RD, Compton
WM, Weiss
SRB. Adverse health effects of marijuana use.
N Engl J Med. 2014;370(23):2219-2227.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.D’Souza
DC, Perry
E, MacDougall
L,
et al. The psychotomimetic effects of intravenous delta-9-tetrahydrocannabinol in healthy individuals: implications for psychosis.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2004;29(8):1558-1572.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Maggs
JL, Staff
J, Kloska
DD, Patrick
ME, O’Malley
PM, Schulenberg
J. Predicting young adult degree attainment by late adolescent marijuana use.
J Adolesc Health. 2015;57(2):205-211.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Filbey
FM, Aslan
S, Calhoun
VD,
et al. Long-term effects of marijuana use on the brain.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(47):16913-16918.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Gilman
JM, Kuster
JK, Lee
S,
et al. Cannabis use is quantitatively associated with nucleus accumbens and amygdala abnormalities in young adult recreational users.
J Neurosci. 2014;34(16):5529-5538.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Meier
MH, Caspi
A, Ambler
A,
et al. Persistent cannabis users show neuropsychological decline from childhood to midlife.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(40):E2657-E2664.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Schweinsburg
AD, Brown
SA, Tapert
SF. The influence of marijuana use on neurocognitive functioning in adolescents.
Curr Drug Abuse Rev. 2008;1(1):99-111.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Chen
CY, Storr
CL, Anthony
JC. Early-onset drug use and risk for drug dependence problems.
Addict Behav. 2009;34(3):319-322.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Lynskey
MT, Heath
AC, Bucholz
KK,
et al. Escalation of drug use in early-onset cannabis users vs co-twin controls.
JAMA. 2003;289(4):427-433.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Ikemoto
S. Dopamine reward circuitry: two projection systems from the ventral midbrain to the nucleus accumbens-olfactory tubercle complex.
Brain Res Rev. 2007;56(1):27-78.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Grace
AA, Floresco
SB, Goto
Y, Lodge
DJ. Regulation of firing of dopaminergic neurons and control of goal-directed behaviors.
Trends Neurosci. 2007;30(5):220-227.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Pierce
RC, Kumaresan
V. The mesolimbic dopamine system: the final common pathway for the reinforcing effect of drugs of abuse?
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2006;30(2):215-238.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Volkow
ND, Wang
GJ, Telang
F,
et al. Decreased dopamine brain reactivity in marijuana abusers is associated with negative emotionality and addiction severity.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(30):E3149-E3156.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Blum
K, Braverman
ER, Holder
JM,
et al. Reward deficiency syndrome: a biogenetic model for the diagnosis and treatment of impulsive, addictive, and compulsive behaviors.
J Psychoactive Drugs. 2000;32(suppl):i-iv, 1-112.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Knutson
B, Adams
CM, Fong
GW, Hommer
D. Anticipation of increasing monetary reward selectively recruits nucleus accumbens.
J Neurosci. 2001;21(16):RC159.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Heitzeg
MM, Villafuerte
S, Weiland
BJ,
et al. Effect of GABRA2 genotype on development of incentive-motivation circuitry in a sample enriched for alcoholism risk.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(13):3077-3086.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Yau
WY, Zubieta
JK, Weiland
BJ, Samudra
PG, Zucker
RA, Heitzeg
MM. Nucleus accumbens response to incentive stimuli anticipation in children of alcoholics: relationships with precursive behavioral risk and lifetime alcohol use.
J Neurosci. 2012;32(7):2544-2551.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Jager
G, Block
RI, Luijten
M, Ramsey
NF. Tentative evidence for striatal hyperactivity in adolescent cannabis-using boys: a cross-sectional multicenter fMRI study.
J Psychoactive Drugs. 2013;45(2):156-167.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Nestor
L, Hester
R, Garavan
H. Increased ventral striatal BOLD activity during non-drug reward anticipation in cannabis users.
Neuroimage. 2010;49(1):1133-1143.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Enzi
B, Lissek
S, Edel
MA,
et al. Alterations of monetary reward and punishment processing in chronic cannabis users: an FMRI study.
PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0119150.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Filbey
FM, Dunlop
J, Myers
US. Neural effects of positive and negative incentives during marijuana withdrawal.
PLoS One. 2013;8(5):e61470.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Karoly
HC, Bryan
AD, Weiland
BJ, Mayer
A, Dodd
A, Feldstein Ewing
SW. Does incentive-elicited nucleus accumbens activation differ by substance of abuse? An examination with adolescents.
Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2015;16:5-15.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Bjork
JM, Smith
AR, Hommer
DW. Striatal sensitivity to reward deliveries and omissions in substance dependent patients.
Neuroimage. 2008;42(4):1609-1621.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Forbes
EE, Brown
SM, Kimak
M, Ferrell
RE, Manuck
SB, Hariri
AR. Genetic variation in components of dopamine neurotransmission impacts ventral striatal reactivity associated with impulsivity.
Mol Psychiatry. 2009;14(1):60-70.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Hahn
T, Dresler
T, Ehlis
AC,
et al. Neural response to reward anticipation is modulated by Gray’s impulsivity.
Neuroimage. 2009;46(4):1148-1153.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Zucker
RA, Fitzgerald
HE, Refior
SK, Puttler
LI, Pallas
DM, Ellis
DA. The clinical and social ecology of childhood for children of alcoholics: description of a study and implications for a differentiated social policy. In: Fitzgerald
HE, Lester
BM, Zucker
RA, eds. Children of Addiction: Research, Health and Policy Issues. New York, NY: Routledge Falmer; 2000:109-141.
32.Robins
L, Cottler
LB, Bucholz
KK, Compton
WM, North
CS, Rourke
K. Diagnostic Interview Schedule for the DSM-IV. St. Louis, MO: Washington University School of Medicine; 2000.
33.Zucker
RA, Ellis
DE, Fitzgerald
HE, Bingham
CR, Sanford
KP. Other evidence for at least two alcoholisms, II: life course variation in antisociality and heterogeneity of alcoholic outcome.
Dev Psychopathol. 1996;8(4):831-848.
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Zucker
RA, Fitzgerald
HE, Noll
RB. Drinking and Drug History. Revised ed, version 4. East Lansing: Michigan State University Department of Psychology; 1990.
35.Glover
GH, Law
CS. Spiral-in/out BOLD fMRI for increased SNR and reduced susceptibility artifacts.
Magn Reson Med. 2001;46(3):515-522.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Kriegeskorte
N, Simmons
WK, Bellgowan
PSF, Baker
CI. Circular analysis in systems neuroscience: the dangers of double dipping.
Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(5):535-540.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Brett
M, Anton
JL, Valabregue
R, Poline
JB. Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox [abstract]. In: Eighth International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain; Sendai, Japan; June 2-6, 2002.
38.Muthén
BO, Muthén
LK. Mplus (Version 7.2). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2014.
39.Zucker
RA. Genes, brain, behavior, and context: the developmental matrix of addictive behavior. In: Stoltenberg
SF, ed. Genes and the Motivation to Use Substances. Vol 61: Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. New York: Springer New York; 2014:51-69.
41.Jackson
KM, Sher
KJ, Schulenberg
JE. Conjoint developmental trajectories of young adult substance use.
Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2008;32(5):723-737.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Neal
DJ, Fromme
K. Event-level covariation of alcohol intoxication and behavioral risks during the first year of college.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 2007;75(2):294-306.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Jansma
JM, van Hell
HH, Vanderschuren
LJMJ,
et al. THC reduces the anticipatory nucleus accumbens response to reward in subjects with a nicotine addiction.
Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3(2):e234.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.van Hell
HH, Vink
M, Ossewaarde
L, Jager
G, Kahn
RS, Ramsey
NF. Chronic effects of cannabis use on the human reward system: an fMRI study.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2010;20(3):153-163.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Hu
L, Bentler
PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria vs new alternatives.
Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):1-55.
Google ScholarCrossref 46.Blum
K, Cull
JG, Braverman
ER, Comings
DE. Reward deficiency syndrome.
Am Sci. 1996;84(2):132-145.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 47.Robinson
TE, Berridge
KC. The neural basis of drug craving: an incentive-sensitization theory of addiction.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1993;18(3):247-291.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Martin
M, Ledent
C, Parmentier
M, Maldonado
R, Valverde
O. Involvement of CB1 cannabinoid receptors in emotional behaviour.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2002;159(4):379-387.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Bambico
FR, Nguyen
NT, Katz
N, Gobbi
G. Chronic exposure to cannabinoids during adolescence but not during adulthood impairs emotional behaviour and monoaminergic neurotransmission.
Neurobiol Dis. 2010;37(3):641-655.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Rubino
T, Vigano’
D, Realini
N,
et al. Chronic Δ
9-tetrahydrocannabinol during adolescence provokes sex-dependent changes in the emotional profile in adult rats: behavioral and biochemical correlates.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2008;33(11):2760-2771.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Heitzeg
MM, Cope
LM, Martz
ME, Hardee
JE, Zucker
RA. Brain activation to negative stimuli mediates a relationship between adolescent marijuana use and later emotional functioning.
Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2015;16:71-83.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Wrase
J, Schlagenhauf
F, Kienast
T,
et al. Dysfunction of reward processing correlates with alcohol craving in detoxified alcoholics.
Neuroimage. 2007;35(2):787-794.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Beck
A, Schlagenhauf
F, Wüstenberg
T,
et al. Ventral striatal activation during reward anticipation correlates with impulsivity in alcoholics.
Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(8):734-742.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. NIAAA council approves definition of binge drinking. NIAAA News. 2004(3):3.