Key PointsQuestion
What is the association between fluid intelligence and psychiatric disorders among adolescents?
Findings
Fluid intelligence was lower among adolescents who met criteria for the following psychiatric disorders at the time of intelligence testing: bipolar disorder, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, oppositional disorder, conduct disorder, substance use disorders, and specific phobia. Those with severe disorders had the strongest association with IQ.
Meaning
Active symptoms of psychiatric disorders may interfere with cognitive functioning, and early identification and treatment of children with mental disorders in school settings are critical to promote academic achievement and long-term success.
Importance
Despite long-standing interest in the association of psychiatric disorders with intelligence, few population-based studies of psychiatric disorders have assessed intelligence.
Objective
To investigate the association of fluid intelligence with past-year and lifetime psychiatric disorders, disorder age at onset, and disorder severity in a nationally representative sample of US adolescents.
Design, Setting, and Participants
National sample of adolescents ascertained from schools and households from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement, collected 2001 through 2004. Face-to-face household interviews with adolescents and questionnaires from parents were obtained. The data were analyzed from February to December 2016. DSM-IV mental disorders were assessed with the World Health Organization Composite International Diagnostic Interview, and included a broad range of fear, distress, behavior, substance use, and other disorders. Disorder severity was measured with the Sheehan Disability Scale.
Main Outcomes and Measures
Fluid IQ measured with the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, normed within the sample by 6-month age groups.
Results
The sample included 10 073 adolescents (mean [SD] age, 15.2 [1.50] years; 49.0% female) with valid data on fluid intelligence. Lower mean (SE) IQ was observed among adolescents with past-year bipolar disorder (94.2 [1.69]; P = .004), attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (96.3 [0.91]; P = .002), oppositional defiant disorder (97.3 [0.66]; P = .007), conduct disorder (97.1 [0.82]; P = .02), substance use disorders (alcohol abuse, 96.5 [0.67]; P < .001; drug abuse, 97.6 [0.64]; P = .02), and specific phobia (97.1 [0.39]; P = .001) after adjustment for a wide range of potential confounders. Intelligence was not associated with posttraumatic stress disorder, eating disorders, and anxiety disorders other than specific phobia, and was positively associated with past-year major depression (mean [SE], 100 [0.5]; P = .01). Associations of fluid intelligence with lifetime disorders that had remitted were attenuated compared with past-year disorders, with the exception of separation anxiety disorder. Multiple past-year disorders had a larger proportion of adolescents less than 1 SD below the mean IQ range than those without a disorder. Across disorders, higher disorder severity was associated with lower fluid intelligence. For example, among adolescents with specific phobia, those with severe disorder had a mean (SE) of 4.4 (0.72) points lower IQ than those without severe disorder (P < .001), and those with alcohol abuse had a mean (SE) of 5.6 (1.2) points lower IQ than those without severe disorder (P < .001).
Conclusions and Relevance
Numerous psychiatric disorders were associated with reductions in fluid intelligence; associations were generally small in magnitude. Stronger associations of current than past disorders with intelligence suggest that active symptoms of psychiatric disorders interfere with cognitive functioning. Early identification and treatment of children with mental disorders in school settings is critical to promote academic achievement and long-term success.
Many psychiatric disorders involve disruptions in cognitive functioning. These encompass attention, memory, language processing, and executive functions.1-7 Given these patterns, there has been long-standing interest in the association of psychiatric disorders with intelligence.
Intelligence is a complex construct that has inspired a voluminous literature regarding its definition, measurement, and implications. Modern conceptualizations typically acknowledge a general intelligence factor (often referred to as g) as well as narrower, more specific abilities (eg, processing speed, visuospatial reasoning, working memory).8(pp34-52)9 The specific abilities encompassing intelligence continue to be debated,10,11 but a widely accepted model of cognitive abilities distinguishes between fluid and crystallized intelligence as 2 primary components.12 Fluid intelligence reflects reasoning and the ability to solve novel problems; crystallized intelligence reflects knowledge and skills that are the result of experience and learning.13(pp87-120) Analysis of the structure of cognitive abilities underlying intelligence suggests that fluid reasoning loads most strongly onto the generalized intelligence factor14(pp115-142) and is indistinguishable from g.15
To what extent are psychiatric disorders associated with fluid intelligence? Modern examination of intelligence and psychiatric disorders has been primarily limited to relatively small, clinical samples. Poor performance on intelligence tests has been documented in individuals with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD),16-20 conduct disorder and oppositional defiant disorder (ODD),21-28 and posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).5,6,29 Associations of intelligence with depression and anxiety disorders are inconsistent across studies.21,30-33 The degree to which intelligence is associated with most psychiatric disorders remains an open question, given inherent biases in studies composed of clinical samples and the lack of population-based studies that measure intelligence.
One important question is whether associations of intelligence with psychiatric disorders reflect that low intelligence is a risk factor for psychiatric disorders or that changes in cognitive functioning are a consequence of developing a psychiatric disorder. While prospective data are optimal to adjudicate between these possibilities, to date such evidence exists only for disruptive behavior problems, indicating that low intelligence prospectively predicts life-course–persistent antisocial behavior, particularly for males.24,34 If low intelligence associated with other psychiatric disorders reflects a consequence rather than risk factor for psychiatric disorders, we would expect associations of intelligence to be stronger among individuals who currently meet criteria for a disorder as compared with those who have met criteria in the past but do not currently. In contrast, if low IQ is a risk factor for psychiatric disorders, we should observe associations of similar magnitude for both current and past disorders with IQ.
In the present report, we investigate the association of fluid intelligence with a wide range of psychiatric disorders in a nationally representative sample of US adolescents. We present intelligence estimates for adolescents who currently meet criteria for fear, distress, behavior, and substance use disorders, as well as for those who met criteria in the past but not currently, and further examine associations between fluid intelligence and psychiatric disorders by age at onset and severity of disorder.
Data were drawn from the National Comorbidity Survey Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A), a nationally representative, face-to-face survey of 13- to 18-year-olds sampled from the continental United States in 2001 to 2004.35 The sample was selected through a dual-frame design, with adolescents recruited from both schools and households.36-38 The sample included 10 148 English-speaking adolescents, 10 073 (99.3%) with valid outcome data that were analyzed in the present study. Sample weights were created based on the 2000 Census. More details on NCS-A sampling and weighting procedures are available elsewhere.37-39
Written informed consent from adults and assent from adolescents were obtained. Each participant received $50 for participation. The Human Subjects Committees of Harvard Medical School and the University of Michigan approved recruitment and consent procedures; the Institutional Review Board of Columbia University approved the present analysis.
Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test
Adolescents completed the fluid intelligence portion of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT),40,41 which assesses fluid reasoning with 48 items. This task uses abstract matrices similar to those developed by Raven,42 which are prototypical measures of fluid reasoning and general intelligence.43 The K-BIT Matrices test involves a series of progressively more challenging items. Test administration was discontinued when an adolescent responded incorrectly to all items in a set (sets include 5 items initially and 4 items for the last 2 sets). The K-BIT (and its revision, the KBIT-2) is widely used among children,40,44-50 adolescents,51-53 and adults54-56; the items on the K-BIT have well-documented reliability across these samples, and results across samples correlate with reassessments, suggesting that the interpretation of results across samples has strong validity. Hereafter, we refer to fluid intelligence on the K-BIT as IQ.
The K-BIT norms were created specifically for the NCS-A by the test developer (A.S.K.), as the NCS-A is considerably larger than the original normative sample for the K-BIT; in addition, the K-BIT was published in 1990, so its norms were outdated. Raw scores were generated based on the K-BIT manual for 91.3% of tests, which were administered and scored exactly as prescribed. An additional 8.4% of tests could be scored despite deviations in test administration. For example, some respondents were only asked the most difficult item in each set. In these cases, the K-BIT score was imputed based on the number of correct items and the level at which they met discontinuation criteria. A small percentage of cases (0.3%) were excluded because of invalid test administration. No items were scored for 397 participants; their score was imputed as the mean of the 6-month age group. Scores were normed within 6-month age groups to a mean of 100 and standard deviation of 15. The K-BIT Matrices test demonstrated good internal consistency (Cronbach α = .90), comparable to the value of 0.88 reported in the K-BIT manual for ages 13 to 19 years.40 Exploratory factor analyses indicated that a 1-factor model adequately fit the data.
An adolescent version of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview for DSM-IV was used to assess psychiatric disorders.35,36,39 Disorders were grouped into 5 empirically defined clusters57: (1) fear disorders (specific phobia, agoraphobia, social phobia, panic disorder); (2) distress disorders (separation anxiety disorder, PTSD, major depressive episode/dysthymia, generalized anxiety disorder); (3) behavior disorders (ADHD, ODD, conduct disorder, eating disorders); (4) substance use disorders (alcohol and drug abuse, with or without dependence); and (5) bipolar disorder. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is based on parent-report only. Oppositional defiant disorder and depression combined parent- and child-report of symptoms using an “or” rule.58,59 Children and parents who endorsed symptoms of each psychiatric disorder were asked about the age symptoms began. Clinical reappraisal of children comparing Composite International Diagnostic Interview diagnoses to those assessed with a clinical interview showed good concordance.39
Respondents who met criteria for a diagnosis completed the Sheehan Disability Scales60 assessing the extent to which symptoms of the disorder interfered with home life, school or work, family relationships, and social life on a 0-to-10 Likert scale. Consistent with prior research,61,62 severe impairment was operationalized as a score of 7 or higher in any 1 of the 4 domains.
Parental education (less than high school, high school graduate, some college, college degree or more), parental income (<1.5, 1.5-3.0, 3.1-6.0, >6.0 times the poverty level), race/ethnicity (non-Hispanic white, non-Hispanic black, Asian, other), age, nativity (US born vs not), number of siblings, and birth order were adjusted for in all models. The mean (SE) K-BIT score when all covariates were at their reference level was 102.2 (1.75). In addition, lifetime disorders other than the focal disorder being examined were adjusted for using dichotomous indicators of any fear disorder, any distress disorder, any behavior disorder, any substance use disorder, and bipolar disorder.
We examined mean levels of fluid intelligence among those meeting criteria for past-year and lifetime psychiatric disorders using linear regression. Effect sizes were estimated using the Cohen d. We examined the distribution of low (<1 SD below the mean), average (within 1 SD of the mean), and high (>1 SD above the mean) fluid intelligence across disorder groups, and estimated associations with past-year and lifetime psychiatric disorders using generalized logit models. Sample sizes for each disorder group (past year, lifetime but not current, and by age at onset), as well as the no-disorder group, are provided in Table 1; cells with insufficient sample size (<10) were not analyzed. In eTable 1 in the Supplement, we provide cell sizes for those with a current disorder that began in the past year and for those with a current disorder that began prior to the past year. Finally, we examined whether sex and parent income moderated the associations of mental disorders with fluid intelligence and found no evidence of effect modification. All analyses were estimated with survey design weights; standard errors, estimated with Taylor series linearization implemented in SAS, version 9.4 for Windows (IBM). A false discovery rate correction for multiple comparisons was applied to all analyses given the large number of statistical tests.63
Fluid Intelligence and Past-Year Psychiatric Disorders
Table 2 presents adjusted means and standardized β for the association between fluid intelligence and past-year psychiatric disorder, as well as lifetime (but not past-year) disorder (eTable 2 in the Supplement presents unadjusted means). Past-year bipolar disorder was associated with the lowest average fluid intelligence (mean [SE], 94.2 [1.69]; P = .004) followed by behavior disorders, with ADHD (mean [SE], 96.3 [0.91]; P = .002), conduct disorder (mean [SE], 97.1 [0.82]; P = .02), and ODD (mean [SE], 97.3 [0.66]; P = .007) each falling significantly below the population mean. Past-year substance use disorders were also associated with low IQ (mean [SE] for alcohol abuse, 96.5 [0.67]; P < .001; drug abuse, 97.6 [0.64]; P = .02). Of the fear and distress disorders, only past-year specific phobia (mean [SE], 97.1 [0.39]; P = .001) was associated with low fluid intelligence. Past-year major depression was associated with slightly higher fluid intelligence (mean [SE], 100 [0.5]; P = .01) compared with those with no distress disorders. Fluid intelligence decreased as the number of current disorders increased. Effect sizes for these associations are provided in eTable 3 in the Supplement. In eTable 4 in the Supplement, we separate current disorders into those that began in the past 12 months vs those that began earlier. There were no significant associations between IQ and psychiatric disorders for disorders that began in the past 12 months (however, sample sizes were small).
Fluid Intelligence and Lifetime Psychiatric Disorders
Adjusted means of fluid intelligence for those meeting criteria for a lifetime but not current disorder are in Table 2 (eTable 5 in the Supplement presents adjusted means for lifetime disorders, regardless of past-year status). Associations with fluid intelligence were uniformly attenuated compared with past-year disorders, with 1 exception: past separation anxiety disorder was associated with low IQ (mean [SE], 97.2 [0.61]; P = .01). No association was observed between fluid intelligence and number of lifetime disorders.
Distribution of Fluid Intelligence by Psychiatric Disorder
Table 3 describes the proportion of adolescents with high, medium, and low IQ by psychiatric disorder status. Adjusted multinomial odds ratios for these distributions are in Table 4. Multiple past-year disorders had a larger proportion of adolescents in the low IQ range than those without a disorder, including bipolar disorder, all behavior disorders, alcohol abuse, separation anxiety disorder, specific phobia, and agoraphobia. The pattern was largely similar for lifetime but not past-year disorders, but was significant only for separation anxiety disorder, conduct disorder, and drug abuse. In eTable 6 in the Supplement, we provide distributions of high, middle, and low IQ separating current disorders into those beginning in the past year vs earlier.
Fluid Intelligence by Psychiatric Disorder Severity
Table 5 shows associations between disorder severity and fluid intelligence. Greater disorder severity was associated with lower IQ across a wide range of disorders including all fear disorders, generalized anxiety disorder, ODD, eating disorders, alcohol abuse, and bipolar disorder.
Fluid Intelligence by Psychiatric Disorder Age at Onset
eTables 7 to 9 in the Supplement provide unadjusted mean differences in IQ, IQ distributions, and adjusted associations as a function of disorder age at onset. Few differences emerged by disorder age at onset.
To our knowledge, the present study represents the first population-based study examining association of fluid intelligence with psychiatric disorders in US youth. Our analysis generates 3 central conclusions.
First, past-year bipolar disorder, disruptive behavior disorders, and substance abuse were most strongly associated with low fluid intelligence. Lower IQ has been documented among youths with these disorders in clinical samples.16-28,64,65 Our population estimates indicate that mean IQ was approximately one-third of a standard deviation (approximately 5 points) lower than average among youths with bipolar disorder, behavior disorders, and substance abuse.
The associations of behavior disorders with IQ were stronger for current disorders than for disorders that had remitted. This could reflect either that behavior disorder symptomatology interferes with cognitive functioning, producing low IQ primarily for those with active symptoms, or that low IQ is observed among adolescents with behavior disorders that are chronic and involve more severe symptoms. Few adolescents had behavior disorder onsets in the past year, indicating that current disorders primarily reflect chronic cases, and low IQ was most consistently observed for adolescents with the most severe disorders. Prospective studies have documented low IQ as a precursor of behavior disorder onset.24,34 Our finding that adolescents with more chronic, severe forms of behavior disorder are most likely to have lower IQ is in line with these findings, although it does not rule out the possibility that IQ changes after onset of disorder explain at least a portion of the observed associations.
Second, most fear and distress disorders were not associated with low IQ, with the exception of specific phobia and separation anxiety disorder, which are among the earliest-onset fear and distress disorders.57 Specific phobia, in particular, has been shown to explain a meaningful proportion of later-onset mental disorders.66 These disorders thus appear to represent an early liability to internalizing psychiatric disorders; our results suggest that this liability may be associated with low IQ. Past-year specific phobia was associated with IQ, but lifetime disorder was not. Specific phobia is often a persistent condition,66,67 and this pattern could reflect an association of low IQ with persistent, but not transient, phobia. Alternatively, it may be that current symptoms of phobia interfered with performance due to test anxiety. In contrast, separation anxiety was related to IQ when experienced prior to the past year but not currently. Given the high prevalence of these disorders,66,68 greater research is needed on neuropsychological correlates of early-onset fear and distress disorders.
We found no association between PTSD and IQ. This diverges from prior research, which has consistently demonstrated that low IQ is a risk factor for PTSD onset after trauma.29,69-71 However, most prior work has been conducted in military samples returning from active combat. Military samples are not representative of the general population, nor are they composed of adolescents in our age range. Furthermore, considerable disagreement exists regarding the validity of the association between IQ and PTSD in military samples72,73 because IQ may select service members into degree of combat exposure. Our results are not consistent with theories that low IQ is a vulnerability factor for the development of PTSD after trauma, at least among youth.
Third, past-year depression was associated with slightly higher mean IQ, although we should note that the effect size was small, but statistically significant due to the high prevalence of major depression in adolescence.74 It has frequently been argued that children with very high IQ may exhibit higher rates of bipolar disorder,75-77 as well as social withdrawal and avoidance.78 We find no support for a link between high IQ and bipolar disorder at the population level, but the observed association with depression warrants further exploration because children with higher IQ may present with unique mental health concerns.
Intelligence quotient was ascertained at the time of the interview, precluding an assessment of the reciprocal relation between mental disorders and cognitive ability. Although some of the variance in IQ is stable over early development,79,80 there is also substantial plasticity in IQ.42,79,81,82 While we cannot establish temporality, the associations of IQ with past-year disorders were consistently stronger than for lifetime disorders that had remitted. Although this could reflect a stronger influence of current symptoms on IQ than the reverse, the most plausible interpretation of this pattern is that current symptoms reflect the most persistent disorders, suggesting that lower IQ is associated with chronic psychiatric disorders rather than transient disorders. Future studies should examine this possibility, as measures of disorder duration were substantially co-linear with age at onset, given the young age of NCS-A participants.
Taken together, these findings indicate that children and adolescents with psychiatric disorders face challenges in learning, memory, and reasoning. This underscores the need for early identification of children with mental disorders to provide academic accommodations and treatment to promote long-term success. Although accommodations are often made for children with ADHD and behavior problems, our findings suggest that children with early-onset fear and distress disorders and adolescents with substance use disorders may also require individualized education plans and support. These findings also provide fruitful hypotheses for future research. For example, children with psychiatric disorders face lower educational and occupational functioning; these results suggest that fluid intelligence may be a mechanism in this pathway, given that higher IQ is associated with better school performance.82,83 This remains to be examined in future studies.
In addition to the limitation of a single time point of measurement of IQ, other limitations should be considered. The K-BIT was administered by lay interviewers, which may have increased the frequency of protocol deviations in test administration. Such deviations could have led to worse performance among children with test-taking difficulties (eg, ADHD or test anxiety). However, the K-BIT has been validated in children with intellectual disability and other challenges,49,52,53 and the reliability of K-BIT Matrices was comparable for the present sample and the standardization sample. Furthermore, the K-BIT is a “Level B” test, which permits examiners without high qualifications to administer and interpret it. Psychosis was not assessed in NCS-A given low prevalence in this age group, precluding evaluation of associations with IQ. Finally, given the cross-sectional assessment, recall bias in reports of past disorders likely contributed to underreporting of past disorders, particularly those that were low in severity. This would make the IQ associations with lifetime disorders overestimates because they reflect more severe cases. Longitudinal data are needed to determine the extent to which early-onset psychiatric disorders that remit influence IQ.
To our knowledge, the present study is the largest assessment of IQ in US children ever conducted, and results demonstrate robust associations of IQ with a broad range of psychiatric disorders, most notably for bipolar disorder and behavior disorders—including ADHD, ODD, and conduct disorder, as well as specific phobia, separation anxiety, and substance use disorders. Although associations of IQ with bipolar disorder and behavior disorders are consistent with prior research from clinical samples, those with fear and distress disorders reveal novel relationships not observed in prior studies and call into question others, including the lack of association with PTSD. Together, these findings reflect the potential role of cognitive factors in the etiology of diverse forms of psychiatric disorders, as well as how mental disorders may influence cognitive ability. Most importantly, this work highlights the critical importance of early identification and treatment of mental disorders in youth and the potential utility of accommodations in school settings for children with a wide range of psychiatric disorders to promote long-term success.
Corresponding Author: Katherine M. Keyes, PhD, Department of Epidemiology, Columbia University, Mailman School of Public Health, 722 W 168th St, Ste 724, New York, NY 10032 (kmk2104@columbia.edu).
Accepted for Publication: November 6, 2016.
Published Online: December 28, 2016. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3723
Author Contributions: Dr Keyes and Mr Platt had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Study concept and design: Keyes, McLaughlin.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.
Drafting of the manuscript: All authors.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Platt, Kaufman.
Statistical analysis: Keyes, Platt, Kaufman.
Obtained funding: McLaughlin.
Administrative, technical, or material support: McLaughlin.
Study supervision: Keyes, McLaughlin.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Kaufman earns royalties from Pearson on other Kaufman tests, but the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test is no longer published or available for purchase. No other disclosures are reported.
Funding/Support: The present study was funded by the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (K01AA021511 to Dr Keyes), the National Institute of Mental Health (R01-MH103291 and R01-MH106482 to Dr McLaughlin; T32 MH013043 to Mr Platt), and a Jacobs Foundation Early Career Research Fellowship (to Dr McLaughlin).
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: Dahsan Gary, MPH, Department of Epidemiology, Columbia University, provided assistance with manuscript preparation, and Seth Prins, PhD, Department of Sociomedical Sciences, Columbia University, provided comments on a draft version of the manuscript. These contributions were made without additional funding or payment.
1.Willcutt
EG, Doyle
AE, Nigg
JT, Faraone
SV, Pennington
BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review.
Biol Psychiatry. 2005;57(11):1336-1346.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Martinussen
R, Hayden
J, Hogg-Johnson
S, Tannock
R. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2005;44(4):377-384.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Yehuda
R, Keefe
RS, Harvey
PD,
et al. Learning and memory in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152(1):137-139.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Vasterling
JJ, Duke
LM, Brailey
K, Constans
JI, Allain
AN
Jr, Sutker
PB. Attention, learning, and memory performances and intellectual resources in Vietnam veterans: PTSD and no disorder comparisons.
Neuropsychology. 2002;16(1):5-14.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Bremner
JD, Vermetten
E, Afzal
N, Vythilingam
M. Deficits in verbal declarative memory function in women with childhood sexual abuse-related posttraumatic stress disorder.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 2004;192(10):643-649.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Vasterling
JJ, Brailey
K. Neuropsychological findings in adults with PTSD. In: Vasterling
JJ, Brewin
CR, eds. Neuropsychology of PTSD: Biological, Cognitive, and Clinical Perspectives. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2005.
7.Moradi
AR, Doost
HTN, Taghavi
MR, Yule
W, Dalgleish
T. Everyday memory deficits in children and adolescents with PTSD: performance on the Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40(3):357-361.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Sternberg
RJ. Handbook of Intelligence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press; 2000.
9.Neisser
U, Boodoo
G, Bouchard
TJ,
et al. Intelligence: knowns and unknowns.
Am Psychol. 1996;51(2):77-101.
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Sternberg
RJ. Beyond IQ: a Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press Archive; 1985.
11.Gardner
HE. Intelligence Reframed: Multiple Intelligences for the 21st Century. New York, NY: Perseus Books Group; 2000.
12.Schneider
WJ, McGrew
KS. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll model of intelligence. In: Flanagar
DP, Harrison
PL, eds. Contemporary Intellectual Assessment: Theories, Tests, and Issues. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Guilford Press; 2012:99-144.
13.Catell
RB. Intelligence: Its Structure Growth and Action. Amsterdam, Netherlands: North-Holland; 1971.
14.Caroll
JB. Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 1993.
15.Benson
NF, Kranzler
JH, Floyd
RG. Examining the integrity of measurement of cognitive abilities in the prediction of achievement: comparisons and contrasts across variables from higher-order and bifactor models.
J Sch Psychol. 2016;58(1):1-19.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Rapport
MD, Scanlan
SW, Denney
CB. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and scholastic achievement: a model of dual developmental pathways.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40(8):1169-1183.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Mariani
MA, Barkley
RA. Neuropsychological and academic functioning in preschool boys with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Dev Neuropsychol. 1997;13:111-129.
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Rucklidge
JJ, Tannock
R. Psychiatric, psychosocial, and cognitive functioning of female adolescents with ADHD.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2001;40(5):530-540.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Kuntsi
J, Eley
TC, Taylor
A,
et al. Co-occurrence of ADHD and low IQ has genetic origins.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004;124B(1):41-47.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Cook
ET, Greenberg
MT, Kusche
CA. The relations between emotional understanding, intellectual functioning, and disruptive behavior problems in elementary-school-aged children.
J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1994;22(2):205-219.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Dietz
KR, Lavigne
JV, Arend
R, Rosenbaum
D. Relation between intelligence and psychopathology among preschoolers.
J Clin Child Psychol. 1997;26(1):99-107.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Kusche
CA, Cook
ET, Greenberg
MT. Neuropsychological and cognitive functioning in children with anxiety, externalizing, and comorbid psychopathology.
J Clin Child Psychol. 1993;22(2):172-195.
Google ScholarCrossref 24.White
JL, Moffitt
TE, Silva
PA. A prospective replication of the protective effects of IQ in subjects at high risk for juvenile delinquency.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 1989;57(6):719-724.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Fergusson
DM, Horwood
LJ, Lynskey
MT. The effects of conduct disorder and attention deficit in middle childhood on offending and scholastic ability at age 13.
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993;34(6):899-916.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Gendreau
PCG, Little
T. Predicting Adult Offender Recidivism: What Works. Ottawa, Ontario, Canada: Public Works and Government Services Canada; 1997.
27.Vitacco
MJ, Neumann
CS, Jackson
RL. Testing a four-factor model of psychopathy and its association with ethnicity, gender, intelligence, and violence.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 2005;73(3):466-476.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Kandel
E, Mednick
SA, Kirkegaard-Sorensen
L,
et al. IQ as a protective factor for subjects at high risk for antisocial behavior.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 1988;56(2):224-226.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Koenen
KC, Moffitt
TE, Poulton
R, Martin
J, Caspi
A. Early childhood factors associated with the development of post-traumatic stress disorder: results from a longitudinal birth cohort.
Psychol Med. 2007;37(2):181-192.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Grossman
I, Kaufman
AS, Mednitsky
S, Scharff
L, Dennis
B. Neurocognitive abilities for a clinically depressed sample versus a matched control group of normal individuals.
Psychiatry Res. 1994;51(3):231-244.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Werry
JS, Elkind
GS, Reeves
JC. Attention deficit, conduct, oppositional, and anxiety disorders in children: III. laboratory differences.
J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1987;15(3):409-428.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Shaffer
D, Schonfeld
I, O’Connor
PA,
et al. Neurological soft signs: their relationship to psychiatric disorder and intelligence in childhood and adolescence.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42(4):342-351.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Gorlyn
M, Keilp
JG, Oquendo
MA, Burke
AK, Sackeim
HA, John Mann
J. The WAIS-III and major depression: absence of VIQ/PIQ differences.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2006;28(7):1145-1157.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Moffitt
TE, Caspi
A. Childhood predictors differentiate life-course persistent and adolescence-limited antisocial pathways among males and females.
Dev Psychopathol. 2001;13(2):355-375.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Merikangas
K, Avenevoli
S, Costello
J, Koretz
D, Kessler
RC. National comorbidity survey replication adolescent supplement (NCS-A): I. background and measures.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):367-369.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Kessler
RC, Avenevoli
S, Costello
EJ,
et al. National comorbidity survey replication adolescent supplement (NCS-A): II. overview and design.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):380-385.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Kessler
RC, Avenevoli
S, Costello
EJ,
et al. Design and field procedures in the US National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A).
Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2009;18(2):69-83.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Kessler
RC, Berglund
P, Chiu
WT,
et al. The US National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R): design and field procedures.
Int J Methods Psychiatr Res. 2004;13(2):69-92.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Kessler
RC, Avenevoli
S, Green
J,
et al. National comorbidity survey replication adolescent supplement (NCS-A): III. concordance of DSM-IV/CIDI diagnoses with clinical reassessments.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2009;48(4):386-399.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Kaufman
AS, Kaufman
NL. Manual for the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service; 1990.
41.Kaufman
AS, Wang
JJ. Gender, race, and education differences on the K-BIT at ages 4 to 90 years.
J Psychoed Assess. 1992;10(3):219-229.
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Raven
JC. Mental Tests Used in Genetic Studies: the Performance of Related Individuals on Tests Mainly Educative and Mainly Reproductive [master's thesis]. London, England: University of London; 1936.
43.Kaufman
AS. IQ Testing 101. New York, NY: Springer; 2009.
44.Prewett
PN. A comparison of two screening tests (the Matrix Analogies Test—Short Form and the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test) with the WISC-III.
Psychol Assess. 1995;7(1):69-72.
Google ScholarCrossref 45.Canivez
GL. Validity of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test: comparisons with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children–Third Edition.
Assessment. 1995;2(2):101-111.
Google Scholar 46.Childers
JS, Durham
TW, Wilson
S. Relation of performance on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test with the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test—Revised among preschool children.
Percept Mot Skills. 1994;79(3, pt 1):1195-1199.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Canivez
GL, Neitzel
R, Martin
BE. Construct validity of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition, and adjustment scales for children and adolescents.
J Psychoed Assess. 2005;23(1):15-34.
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Prewett
PN. The relationship between the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT) and the WISC-R with referred students.
Educ Psychol Meas. 1992;29(1):25-27.
Google Scholar 49.Canivez
GL. Validity and diagnostic efficiency of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test in reevaluating students with learning disability.
J Psychoed Assess. 1996;14(1):4-19.
Google ScholarCrossref 50.Levinson
EM, Folino
L. Correlations of scores on the Gifted Evaluation Scale with those on WISC-III and Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test for students referred for gifted evaluation.
Psychol Rep. 1994;74(2):419-424.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Grados
JJ, Russo-Garcia
KA. Comparison of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third Edition in economically disadvantaged African American youth.
J Clin Psychol. 1999;55(9):1063-1071.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Mervis
CB, Kistler
DJ, John
AE, Morris
CA. Longitudinal assessment of intellectual abilities of children with Williams syndrome: multilevel modeling of performance on the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test-Second Edition.
Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2012;117(2):134-155.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Webber
LS, McGillivray
JA. An Australian validation of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT) with adolescents with an intellectual disability.
Aust Psychol. 1998;33(3):234-237.
Google ScholarCrossref 54.Hays
JR, Reas
DL, Shaw
JB. Concurrent validity of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence and the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test among psychiatric inpatients.
Psychol Rep. 2002;90(2):355-359.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Naugle
RI, Chelune
GJ, Tucker
DG. Validity of the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test.
Psychol Assess. 1993;5(2):182-186.
Google ScholarCrossref 56.Walters
SO, Weaver
KA. Relationships between the Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Third Edition.
Psychol Rep. 2003;92(3, pt 2):1111-1115.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Kessler
RC, Chiu
WT, Demler
O, Merikangas
KR, Walters
EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617-627.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Cantwell
DP, Lewinsohn
PM, Rohde
P, Seeley
JR. Correspondence between adolescent report and parent report of psychiatric diagnostic data.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36(5):610-619.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Grills
AE, Ollendick
TH. Issues in parent-child agreement: the case of structured diagnostic interviews.
Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2002;5(1):57-83.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Leon
AC, Olfson
M, Portera
L, Farber
L, Sheehan
DV. Assessing psychiatric impairment in primary care with the Sheehan Disability Scale.
Int J Psychiatry Med. 1997;27(2):93-105.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.McLaughlin
KA, Green
JG, Hwang
I, Sampson
NA, Zaslavsky
AM, Kessler
RC. Intermittent explosive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(11):1131-1139.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 62.Kessler
RC, Coccaro
EF, Fava
M, Jaeger
S, Jin
R, Walters
E. The prevalence and correlates of DSM-IV intermittent explosive disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(6):669-678.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Thaler
NS, Sutton
GP, Allen
DN. Social cognition and functional capacity in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res. 2014;220(1-2):309-314.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Buchy
L, Seidman
LJ, Cadenhead
KS,
et al. Evaluating the relationship between cannabis use and IQ in youth and young adults at clinical high risk of psychosis.
Psychiatry Res. 2015;230(3):878-884.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Kessler
RC, Avenevoli
S, Costello
EJ,
et al. Prevalence, persistence, and sociodemographic correlates of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication Adolescent Supplement.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(4):372-380.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 67.Becker
ES, Rinck
M, Türke
V,
et al. Epidemiology of specific phobia subtypes: findings from the Dresden Mental Health Study.
Eur Psychiatry. 2007;22(2):69-74.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Merikangas
KR, Nakamura
EF, Kessler
RC. Epidemiology of mental disorders in children and adolescents.
Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2009;11(1):7-20.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 69.Pitman
RK, Orr
SP, Lowenhagen
MJ, Macklin
ML, Altman
B. Pre-Vietnam contents of posttraumatic stress disorder veterans’ service medical and personnel records.
Compr Psychiatry. 1991;32(5):416-422.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Macklin
ML, Metzger
LJ, Litz
BT,
et al. Lower precombat intelligence is a risk factor for posttraumatic stress disorder.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 1998;66(2):323-326.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Kremen
WS, Koenen
KC, Boake
C,
et al. Pretrauma cognitive ability and risk for posttraumatic stress disorder: a twin study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(3):361-368.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 72.Dohrenwend
BP, Yager
TJ, Wall
MM, Adams
BG. The roles of combat exposure, personal vulnerability, and involvement in harm to civilians or prisoners in Vietnam War-related posttraumatic stress disorder.
Clin Psychol Sci. 2013;1(3):223-238.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 73.Breslau
N, Chen
Q, Luo
Z. The role of intelligence in posttraumatic stress disorder: does it vary by trauma severity?
PLoS One. 2013;8(6):e65391.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 74.Merikangas
KR, He
JP, Burstein
M,
et al. Lifetime prevalence of mental disorders in US adolescents: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication–Adolescent Supplement (NCS-A).
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49(10):980-989.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 75.MacCabe
JH, Lambe
MP, Cnattingius
S,
et al. Excellent school performance at age 16 and risk of adult bipolar disorder: national cohort study.
Br J Psychiatry. 2010;196(2):109-115.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 76.Kyaga
S, Lichtenstein
P, Boman
M, Hultman
C, Långström
N, Landén
M. Creativity and mental disorder: family study of 300,000 people with severe mental disorder.
Br J Psychiatry. 2011;199(5):373-379.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 77.Smith
DJ, Anderson
J, Zammit
S, Meyer
TD, Pell
JP, Mackay
D. Childhood IQ and risk of bipolar disorder in adulthood: prospective birth cohort study.
BJPsych Open. 2015;1(1):74-80.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 78.Swiatek
M. An empirical investigation of the social coping strategies used by gifted adolescents.
Gift Child Q. 1995;39(3):154-160.
Google ScholarCrossref 79.Moffitt
TE, Caspi
A, Harkness
AR, Silva
PA. The natural history of change in intellectual performance: who changes? how much? is it meaningful?
J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1993;34(4):455-506.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.Price
CJ, Ramsden
S, Hope
TM, Friston
KJ, Seghier
ML. Predicting IQ change from brain structure: a cross-validation study.
Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2013;5:172-184.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 81.Breslau
N, Chilcoat
HD, Susser
ES, Matte
T, Liang
KY, Peterson
EL. Stability and change in children’s intelligence quotient scores: a comparison of two socioeconomically disparate communities.
Am J Epidemiol. 2001;154(8):711-717.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 83.Kaufman
AS, Raiford
SE, Coalson
DL. Intelligent Testing With the WISC-V. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley; 2016.