Key PointsQuestion
How can multiple nicotine-associated memories and nicotine craving be selectively inhibited?
Findings
In rat models and a human study, propranolol administered with an unconditioned stimulus-induced memory reactivation-reconsolidation interference procedure inhibited nicotine craving induced by exposure to diverse nicotine-associated cues and nicotine itself. By contrast, the established conditioned stimulus-induced memory reactivation-reconsolidation interference procedure only inhibited nicotine craving induced by the previously reactivated nicotine-associated cues.
Meaning
These findings suggest that an unconditioned stimulus-induced procedure may be a promising method for decreasing nicotine craving and, potentially, relapse among smokers.
Importance
A relapse into nicotine addiction during abstinence often occurs after the reactivation of nicotine reward memories, either by acute exposure to nicotine (a smoking episode) or by smoking-associated conditioned stimuli (CS). Preclinical studies suggest that drug reward memories can undergo memory reconsolidation after being reactivated, during which they can be weakened or erased by pharmacological or behavioral manipulations. However, translational clinical studies using CS-induced memory retrieval-reconsolidation procedures to decrease drug craving reported inconsistent results.
Objective
To develop and test an unconditioned stimulus (UCS)-induced retrieval-reconsolidation procedure to decrease nicotine craving among people who smoke.
Design, Setting, and Participants
A translational rat study and human study in an academic outpatient medical center among 96 male smokers (aged 18- 45 years) to determine the association of propranolol administration within the time window of memory reconsolidation (after retrieval of the nicotine-associated memories by nicotine UCS exposure) with relapse to nicotine-conditioned place preference (CPP) and operant nicotine seeking in rats, and measures of preference to nicotine-associated CS and nicotine craving among people who smoke.
Intervention
The study rats were injected noncontingently with the UCS (nicotine 0.15 mg/kg, subcutaneous) in their home cage, and the human study participants administered a dose of propranolol (40 mg, per os; Zhongnuo Pharma).
Main Outcomes and Measures
Nicotine CPP and operant nicotine seeking in rats, and preference and craving ratings for newly learned and preexisting real-life nicotine-associated CS among people who smoke.
Results
Sixty-nine male smokers completed the experiment and were included for statistical analysis: 24 in the group that received placebo plus 1 hour plus UCS, 23 who received propranolol plus 1 hour plus UCS, and 22 who received UCS plus 6 hours plus propranolol. In rat relapse models, propranolol injections administered immediately after nicotine UCS-induced memory retrieval inhibited subsequent nicotine CPP and operant nicotine seeking after short (CPP, d = 1.72, 95% CI, 0.63-2.77; operant seeking, d = 1.61, 95% CI, 0.59-2.60) or prolonged abstinence (CPP, d = 1.46, 95% CI, 0.42-2.47; operant seeking: d = 1.69, 95% CI, 0.66-2.69), as well as nicotine priming-induced reinstatement of nicotine CPP (d = 1.28, 95% CI, 0.27-2.26) and operant nicotine seeking (d = 1.61, 95% CI, 0.59-2.60) after extinction. Among the smokers, oral propranolol administered prior to nicotine UCS-induced memory retrieval decreased subsequent nicotine preference induced by newly learned nicotine CS (CS1, Cohen d = 0.61, 95% CI, 0.02-1.19 and CS2, d = 0.69, 95% CI, 0.10-1.28, respectively), preexisting nicotine CS (d = 0.57, 95% CI, −0.02 to 1.15), and nicotine priming (CS1, d = 0.82, 95% CI, 0.22-1.41 and CS2, d = 0.78, 95% CI, 0.18-1.37, respectively; preexisting nicotine CS, d = 0.92, 95% CI, 0.31-1.52), as well as nicotine craving induced by the preexisting nicotine CS (d = 0.64, 95% CI, 0.05-1.22), and nicotine priming (d = 1.15, 95% CI, 0.52-1.76).
Conclusions and Relevance
In rat-to-human translational study, a novel UCS-induced memory retrieval-reconsolidation interference procedure inhibited nicotine craving induced by exposure to diverse nicotine-associated CS and nicotine itself. This procedure should be studied further in clinical trials.
A core feature of nicotine addiction is high relapse rates during abstinence.1-3 In human studies4 and in animal models,5,6 key triggers of craving and relapse during abstinence are acute reexposure to nicotine (the unconditioned stimulus, [UCS], a smoking episode for people or noncontingent nicotine priming injections in laboratory animals) or nicotine-associated conditioned stimuli (CS), presumably because of reactivation of nicotine-associated memories.7,8
Nicotine-associated memories may undergo reconsolidation, a time-dependent process in which consolidated memories become unstable for several hours after their reactivation.9-11 Preclinical studies suggest that reconsolidation requires activation of the noradrenergic system.12-14 Human studies have shown that the administration of the β-adrenoceptor blocker propranolol before or after memory reactivation decreases conditioned fear among healthy individuals, fear of spiders among arachnophobic individuals,15 or psychophysiological responses induced by traumatic imagery scripts among individuals with posttraumatic stress disorder.16,17 Propranolol administration after memory retrieval also impairs reconsolidation of drug-related memories in rat models,14,18-23 and among people addicted to heroin.24 However, other studies failed to demonstrate an effect of propranolol on drug craving among people who use drugs.25-27
One potential reason for the limited “translational” utility of propranolol’s effect on drug craving is that previous human studies exposed participants to drug-associated CS, not (UCS), to induce memory retrieval and reconsolidation. Under these experimental conditions, propranolol or other neuropharmacological manipulations interfere with the reconsolidation of memories selectively associated with the reactivated CS.28-31 In the context of nicotine addiction, smoking is associated with multiple CS that vary across individuals.32 It is not feasible to reactivate all of a smoker’s nicotine-associated CS in a clinical or laboratory setting.
Based on these considerations and findings that interference with reconsolidation after retrieval by UCS (aversive shock) prevented subsequent fear expression induced by multiple CS,28,29,33 we recently developed a UCS-induced memory retrieval-reconsolidation interference procedure and demonstrated its efficacy in decreasing relapse to cocaine seeking and craving in rat models.31 In the present study, we introduced a memory retrieval-reconsolidation interference procedure that is based on our rat study31 in which we reactivated nicotine reward memories in both rat models and among people who smoke by acute exposure to nicotine (the UCS) and then interfered with memory reconsolidation using propranolol. In the rat study, we assessed the efficacy of our procedure using animal models of drug relapse6,34 based on the CPP35 and intravenous drug self-administration36 procedures. In the human study, we assessed the efficacy of our new procedure using a laboratory method in which drug craving during abstinence was assessed after exposing the participants to drug-associated cues27,37-39 or drug priming.40
The timelines of the experiments are provided in eFigure 1 and 2 in the Supplement.
We housed male 156 Sprague-Dawley rats (260-280 g; Vitalriver Company) in individual cages in a temperature- (23±2°C) and humidity- (50 ±5%) controlled animal facility with unlimited access to food and water. We performed the experimental procedures in accordance with the National Institutes of Health Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals and the procedures were approved by the Biomedical Ethics Committee for animal use and protection of Peking University. The procedures for training and testing for nicotine CPP are similar to those of our previous studies.41-43 The training and extinction procedures are also similar to those of previous studies by our group and others.44-48 We injected the rats noncontingently with the previously self-administered drug (nicotine 0.15 mg/kg, subcutaneous, the UCS) in their home cage. We based the nicotine UCS retrieval dose on previous studies using the reinstatement procedure,46,48,49 and these studies also guided our choice for the nicotine priming dose during the reinstatement tests described later. We based the propranolol dose (10 mg/kg, intraperitoneal) and the timing of the injections on previous studies of reconsolidation of drug memories in rats.18-20 The UCS and CS memory retrieval manipulations were based on those used in our previous studies.31,45 This information is described in the eMethods in the Supplement.
The timeline of the study is provided in eFigure 3 in the Supplement and a description of the experimental phases is provided in the eMethods in the Supplement.
We recruited 96 healthy young men through posters and online advertisements. Because of the multisession aspect of the study, we enrolled only men to avoid the influence of the menstrual cycle phase on the response to nicotine stimulant effects and craving induced by nicotine-associated cues.50,51 The inclusion criteria were that participants had to be men between the ages of 18 and 45 years who had been smoking 10 or more cigarettes per day continuously for at least 12 months prior to the screening, were willing to refrain from smoking for at least a night, had an afternoon end-exhaled carbon monoxide concentration less than or equal to 10 ppm (Bedfont Mini2 Smokerlyzer; Bedfont Scientific Ltd), and were in general good health as determined by a physician. Participants were excluded if they had used addictive drugs other than nicotine based on self-reporting or had a positive urine test result during either the recruitment screening or testing, had a current or past history of DSM-IV Axis I disorders, were currently using antidepressants or had depression diagnosed, had clinically evident cognitive impairment, or had self-reported treatment with any prescription drugs during the previous 2 weeks or any over-the-counter drugs during the 3 days prior to the experimental sessions.
Eligible candidates were scheduled for a screening interview during which they read the study protocol and signed the informed consent form. The study was approved by the institutional review boards of Peking University Health Center, and all participants were paid for their participation. During the baseline session, we collected demographic data (age, sex, and education) and self-reports of the number of cigarettes per day and the Fagerström Test of Nicotine Dependence Score, and we also assessed depression and anxiety using standard measures.
We chose the dose of propranolol (40 mg, oral; Zhongnuo Pharma) based on previous studies on memory reconsolidation among people with spider phobias15 and people addicted to heroin.24 We administered propranolol 1 hour before the memory retrieval manipulation rather than 1 hour after retrieval as in the rat study, because plasma concentrations of propranolol peak approximately 1 hour after oral administration,52 and we were concerned that postretrieval oral administration might delay effective brain concentrations of propranolol until after the typical 1 to 2 hour “reconsolidation window.”53,54 The delayed 6-hour propranolol administration condition (a standard control condition in rodent and human memory studies) is based on previous human studies on propranolol’s effect on reconsolidation of fear memories.53,54
In a double-blind experimental design, we recruited 96 participants and randomly assigned them to 1 of 3 groups, but it in the statistical analyses, we excluded 27 participants because they did not complete the experiment. The 3 groups of participants included: placebo plus 1 hour plus UCS, propranolol plus 1 hour plus UCS, and UCS plus 6 hours plus propranolol, all in a 1:1:1 ratio. During the randomization period, an independent (blind) team member, who was only involved in the rat experiments, generated a table of randomization codes using Microsoft Excel software. Other team members who were responsible for the random allocation of the patients, drug administration, and the subsequent memory tests during the study were blind to the drug condition. We gave the participants the placebo and propranolol in identical capsules. We used methods from previous studies with some modifications.55,56 There were 6 sessions: screening and baseline tests of preference (liking) and craving for “pre-existing” nicotine CS (day 1), conditioning for 3 different CS to be paired or not paired (CS-) with smoking (day 2-4), postconditioning test of preference and craving for the CS (day 5), UCS memory reactivation and propranolol or placebo administration (day 6), a posttreatment test (day 7), and a priming test (day 8) (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). We asked the participants to abstain from smoking for at least 8 hours (overnight) before the daily experimental sessions, and verified short-term abstinence by examining their carbon monoxide level (less than 8 ppm) before the sessions.57 We instructed the participants to continue their usual daily smoking habits and not to attempt to quit smoking while in the study.
Assessment of Nicotine Craving and Preference (Liking)
Based on previous studies,45,55 we measured subjective ratings of “liking” and craving. For ratings of “liking,” we marked the scale at the left and right ends with -5 (“dislike”) and 5 (“like very much”) and with 0 (“neutral”) at the midpoint. For ratings of craving, we marked the scale at the left and right ends with 0 (“extremely low”) and 10 (“extremely high”). The participants had to rate their current craving level to smoke or their preference for the different CS after exposure to the different experimental manipulations (see earlier).
We reported the results as mean (standard error of the mean) and analyzed the data by analyses of variance (ANOVAs) with the appropriate between- and within-subject factors for each experiment (eTable in the Supplement). We excluded 22 participants because they did not meet the smoking citeria and 5 participants because they decided to quit the study during the conditioning phase and included 69 participants (24 for placebo plus 1 hour plus UCS, 23 for propranolol plus 1 hour plus UCS, and 22 for UCS plus 6 hours plus propranolol) for data analysis. No statistical methods were used to predetermine sample sizes, but they are similar to those reported in our previous studies of rats and humans.14,21,45 We performed post hoc analyses of significant effects in ANOVAs using the Tukey test. We also performed post hoc analyses and report the Cohen d estimate and its 95% CIs using R software (3.3.1 version). Values of P less than or equal to .05, 2-tailed, were considered statistically significant.
Effect of Propranolol on Reconsolidation of Multiple Nicotine Reward Memories After UCS Retrieval in Rats
In the first experiement, we found that propranolol administration (10 mg/kg, intraperitoneal) within the reconsolidation time window after UCS exposure decreased nicotine preference in the CPP procedure and relapse to nicotine seeking in the drug self-administration procedure (eFigures 4 and 5 in the Supplement).
In the second experiement, we developed a modified training procedure in which we trained rats for both nicotine self-administration (14 days) and then nicotine CPP (8 days), resulting in the formation of both Pavlovian and operant reward memories in the same rat (eFigures 1 and 2 in the Supplement). For the nicotine UCS retrieval procedure, the analysis showed a significant main effect of the propranolol dose (0, 10 mg/kg) on both the CPP test (mixed ANOVA, F1,15 = 6.59, P = .02, Figure 1A) and the nicotine-seeking test (F1,15 = 13.65, P = .002, Figure 1B). By contrast, for the operant CS retrieval procedure, the analysis showed a significant main effect of the propranolol dose on the nicotine-seeking test (mixed ANOVA, F1,13 = 13.94, P = .003, Figure 2) but not the CPP test (F1,13 = 0.02, P = .88, Figure 2).
These results indicate that propranolol injections after UCS retrieval disrupted both nicotine CPP and self-administration memories, leading to the inhibition of nicotine CPP and operant nicotine seeking. By contrast, propranolol injections administered after operant CS memory retrieval selectively disrupted nicotine self-administration memories, leading to the inhibition of operant nicotine seeking but not nicotine CPP.
We also examined whether propranolol injections administered after nicotine UCS retrieval affect reward memories for sucrose. We found that propranolol prevented nicotine CPP 1 day later but had no effect on sucrose seeking in the extinction test (eFigure 3 in the Supplement).
Effect of UCS-Induced Memory Interference Procedure on Multiple Nicotine-Associated Memories and Nicotine Craving Among Smokers
In the third experiment, we tested whether the findings from the rat studies would generalize in relation to people who smoke. We used a variation of a cue preference procedure, designed as a human version of the CPP procedure.55,56 We assigned the 96 smokers (69 of whom completed the experiment) to 3 experimental groups: placebo plus 1 hour plus UCS retrieval, propranolol plus 1 hour plus UCS retrieval, or UCS retrieval plus 6-hours plus propranolol (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). The participants’ demographic data are shown in the Table.
The analysis of preference (liking) for the newly learned CS on days 5 and 7 showed a significant group × test interaction (mixed ANOVA, F2,198 = 7.75, P = .001) and cue type × test interaction (F2,198 = 4.69, P = .01) (Figure 3A). The analysis of preference (liking) for the newly learned CS during the priming test showed significant main effects of group (2-way ANOVA, F2,198 = 10.19, P < .001) and cue type (F2,198 = 6.75, P = .001) (Figure 3A). The analysis of preference (liking) for the preexisting real-life CS indicated a significant group × test interaction (mixed ANOVA, F2,66 = 3.74, P = .03) (Figure 3B) in the posttreatment test and a significant main effect of group (1-way ANOVA, F2,66 = 11.14, P < .001) in the nicotine priming test (Figure 3B). These results demonstrate that propranolol administration within the time window of memory reconsolidation after UCS retrieval decreased the participants’ preference (liking) to both newly learned and preexisting nicotine-associated CS.
We also examined the effect of propranolol plus UCS retrieval on nicotine craving induced by newly learned and preexisting CS and nicotine priming. For newly learned CS, there were no significant group differences (mixed-measures ANOVA, P > .1) in the posttreatment test (Figure 4A). By contrast, for preexisting CSs, there was a significant group × test interaction (mixed ANOVA, F2,66 = 5.82, P = .005) in the posttreatment test and a significant main effect of group (1-way ANOVA, F2,66 = 7.91, P = .001) in the priming test (Figure 4B). These results showed that nicotine craving in our procedure was induced by preexisting but not newly learned nicotine-associated cues, but more importantly, that propranolol plus UCS retrieval decreased the nicotine craving induced by preexisting CS and nicotine priming.
In this rat-to-human translational study, we introduced a UCS-induced memory retrieval-reconsolidation interference procedure in which we reactivated nicotine reward memories by acute exposure to nicotine (the UCS) and then pharmacologically interfered with the putative process of memory reconsolidation. In rat relapse models, systemic injections of the β-adrenoceptor blocker propranolol immediately after UCS-induced, but not CS-induced, memory retrieval inhibited subsequent nicotine CPP and relapse to operant nicotine seeking after short or prolonged abstinence. Among people who smoke, propranolol administration decreased subsequent nicotine preference induced by both newly learned and preexisting (ie, real-life) nicotine CS, and nicotine craving induced by the preexisting nicotine CS and nicotine priming. In both rats and humans, the pharmacological manipulations had no effect on any of these behavioral measures when administered 6 hours after UCS retrieval (outside the temporal window within which reconsolidation is thought to occur58), supporting a memory reconsolidation account of the present results.
UCS Memory Retrieval vs CS Memory Retrieval
Since 2005,59,60 many studies using rats and mice showed that consolidated drug-associated memories undergo reconsolidation when reactivated by drug-associated CS, and that neuropharmacological or behavioral interference with reconsolidation can inhibit subsequent drug CPP and relapse into operant drug seeking.14,21,61,62 However, with few exceptions,45,63 the “translation” of these preclinical studies to people addicted to drugs has not been successful.25-27 This situation, and the realization that a major limitation of CS-induced retrieval and reconsolidation procedures is their specificity to the reactivated CS,28,29,33 inspired us to develop an alternative procedure in which the drug-reward memories are reactivated by exposure to the drug itself (the UCS).
In a recent study,31 we introduced a UCS memory retrieval-extinction procedure and showed that, unlike the more established CS-induced procedure,45,64 the UCS-induced procedure can prevent relapse into cocaine seeking induced by multiple cocaine-associated CS under a wide range of experimental conditions. However, from a clinical perspective, the UCS memory retrieval-extinction procedure can be difficult to implement, because it requires multiple sessions of UCS retrieval plus extinction training. By contrast, the UCS memory retrieval-reconsolidation procedure described here was effective among rats and humans after a single UCS session. Additionally, in the human translational study, the procedure’s effects generalized to multiple nicotine CS during the early abstinence period. Furthermore, in rats trained for both nicotine CPP and operant nicotine self-administration, reconsolidation interference after nicotine UCS retrieval inhibited both nicotine CPP and operant nicotine seeking. By contrast, reconsolidation interference after CPP-CS or operant CS memory retrieval selectively inhibited subsequent nicotine CPP, but not operant nicotine seeking and vice versa, respectively.
Together, the findings across species suggest that the UCS-induced memory retrieval-reconsolidation interference procedures are more promising for relapse prevention than the CS-induced procedures.
Methodological and Translational Considerations and Limitations
Before we reach the goal of clinical implementation, several issues should be considered. First, the results of our human study were limited to the measurement of craving and preference during an early abstinence period within a laboratory setting. Craving often precedes and predicts drug relapse, but is neither necessary nor sufficient for it to occur.65,66 Additionally, the response to drug-associated CS in rat models67 and among people68 is often context-specific. Even after prolonged extinction of the response to drug CS within a non-drug context, the same CS can provoke relapse in the drug environment. Thus, we still need to test whether the UCS-induced memory retrieval-reconsolidation interference procedure will decrease craving and relapse in the smokers’ home environments. In the absence of data from a larger clinical study with additional outcome measures, we cannot draw strong conclusions about the clinical utility of the procedure.
Another issue to consider from a translational perspective is that the timing of propranolol administration differed between our rat and human studies: we administered propranolol after UCS retrieval in rats but prior to UCS retrieval in the human study because of the pharmacokinetics of oral administration in humans. This was a procedural necessity, but it complicates our data interpretation. Specifically, it cannot be ruled out that propranolol’s effect on nicotine preference and craving among people was because of interference with memory retrieval rather than memory reconsolidation. Another issue arising in our human study is that under our experimental conditions, nicotine craving were only observed after exposure to preexisting CS, not newly learned CS. This observation agrees with results from a previous study using a similar procedure69 and is likely because of insufficient pairings between the new CS and cigarette smoking.
Finally, the generalizability of our results is limited by our having used only males of each species. We recognize that this approach is no longer tenable70 because of sex differences in nicotine’s effects and smoking cessation,50,51,71 and we plan to address it in the future.
We introduced a UCS-induced memory retrieval-reconsolidation interference procedure and demonstrated its efficacy in simultaneously preventing nicotine CPP and operant nicotine seeking during prolonged abstinence periods in a rat model. Our results among people who smoke suggest that a UCS-induced procedure may be a promising method for decreasing nicotine craving. Additionally, to the degree that results from rat models generalize to drug addiction among people,72 the potential value of the procedure should be tested for the prevention of relapse to smoking.
Corresponding Author: Lin Lu, MD, PhD, Peking University Sixth Hospital (Institute of Mental Health), Peking University, 51 Huayuan Rd, Beijing 100191 China (linlu@bjmu.edu.cn).
Accepted for Publication: November 24, 2016.
Published Online: February 1, 2017. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3907
Author Contributions: Drs Xue and Lu had full access to all of the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Drs Xue and Deng contributed equally to this work.
Concept and design: Xue, Deng, Wu, Luo, Wang, Shaham, Lu.
Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Xue, Deng, Chen, Zhang, Huang, Luo, Bao, Shaham, Shi, Lu.
Drafting of the manuscript: Xue, Deng, Luo, Wang, Shaham, Lu.
Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Xue, Chen, Zhang, Wu, Huang, Luo, Bao, Shaham, Shi, Lu.
Statistical analysis: Xue, Deng, Luo, Bao.
Obtained funding: Xue, Lu.
Administrative, technical, or material support: Deng, Wu, Luo, Wang, Shi.
Supervision: Shi, Lu.
Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.
Funding/Support: This research was conducted with support from grants 2015CB856400, 2015CB559200, and 2015CB553503 from the National Basic Research Program of China and grants 31230033, 91432303, 31300930, 81221002, and 81201032 from the Natural Science Foundation of China. This research also received support from Ten Thousand Youth Talents and the Intramural Research Program of the National Insitutue on Drug Abuse.
Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The Committee for National Basic Research Program of China and Ministry of Science and Technology of China had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.
Additional Contributions: We thank David Epstein, PhD, National Institutes of Health; Zhong-Wei Jia, PhD, Peking University; and Ying Han, PhD, Peking University for helpful comments and advice on the revision of the manuscript. None of these individuals were compensated for their contributions.
2.Zhou
X, Nonnemaker
J, Sherrill
B, Gilsenan
AW, Coste
F, West
R. Attempts to quit smoking and relapse: factors associated with success or failure from the ATTEMPT cohort study.
Addict Behav. 2009;34(4):365-373.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Herd
N, Borland
R, Hyland
A. Predictors of smoking relapse by duration of abstinence: findings from the International Tobacco Control (ITC) Four Country Survey.
Addiction. 2009;104(12):2088-2099.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Pickens
CL, Airavaara
M, Theberge
F, Fanous
S, Hope
BT, Shaham
Y. Neurobiology of the incubation of drug craving.
Trends Neurosci. 2011;34(8):411-420.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Shaham
Y, Shalev
U, Lu
L, De Wit
H, Stewart
J. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: history, methodology and major findings.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2003;168(1-2):3-20.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Jasinska
AJ, Stein
EA, Kaiser
J, Naumer
MJ, Yalachkov
Y. Factors modulating neural reactivity to drug cues in addiction: a survey of human neuroimaging studies.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;38:1-16.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Stoker
AK, Markou
A. Neurobiological bases of cue- and nicotine-induced reinstatement of nicotine seeking: implications for the development of smoking cessation medications.
Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2015;24:125-154.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 9.Alberini
CM. Mechanisms of memory stabilization: are consolidation and reconsolidation similar or distinct processes?
Trends Neurosci. 2005;28(1):51-56.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Misanin
JR, Miller
RR, Lewis
DJ. Retrograde amnesia produced by electroconvulsive shock after reactivation of a consolidated memory trace.
Science. 1968;160(3827):554-555.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Debiec
J, Ledoux
JE. Disruption of reconsolidation but not consolidation of auditory fear conditioning by noradrenergic blockade in the amygdala.
Neuroscience. 2004;129(2):267-272.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Przybyslawski
J, Roullet
P, Sara
SJ. Attenuation of emotional and nonemotional memories after their reactivation: role of beta adrenergic receptors.
J Neurosci. 1999;19(15):6623-6628.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 14.Torregrossa
MM, Taylor
JR. Learning to forget: manipulating extinction and reconsolidation processes to treat addiction.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;226(4):659-672.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Soeter
M, Kindt
M. An abrupt transformation of phobic behavior after a post-retrieval amnesic agent.
Biol Psychiatry. 2015;78(12):880-886.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Brunet
A, Orr
SP, Tremblay
J, Robertson
K, Nader
K, Pitman
RK. Effect of post-retrieval propranolol on psychophysiologic responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress disorder.
J Psychiatr Res. 2008;42(6):503-506.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Brunet
A, Thomas
É, Saumier
D,
et al. Trauma reactivation plus propranolol is associated with durably low physiological responding during subsequent script-driven traumatic imagery.
Can J Psychiatry. 2014;59(4):228-232.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Fricks-Gleason
AN, Marshall
JF. Post-retrieval beta-adrenergic receptor blockade: effects on extinction and reconsolidation of cocaine-cue memories.
Learn Mem. 2008;15(9):643-648.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Otis
JM, Mueller
D. Inhibition of β-adrenergic receptors induces a persistent deficit in retrieval of a cocaine-associated memory providing protection against reinstatement.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011;36(9):1912-1920.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Robinson
MJ, Franklin
KB. Reconsolidation of a morphine place preference: impact of the strength and age of memory on disruption by propranolol and midazolam.
Behav Brain Res. 2010;213(2):201-207.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Milton
AL, Everitt
BJ. The psychological and neurochemical mechanisms of drug memory reconsolidation: implications for the treatment of addiction.
Eur J Neurosci. 2010;31(12):2308-2319.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Diergaarde
L, Schoffelmeer
AN, De Vries
TJ. Pharmacological manipulation of memory reconsolidation: towards a novel treatment of pathogenic memories.
Eur J Pharmacol. 2008;585(2-3):453-457.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Zhao
LY, Sun
LL, Shi
J, Li
P, Zhang
Y, Lu
L. Effects of β-adrenergic receptor blockade on drug-related memory reconsolidation in abstinent heroin addicts.
Drug Alcohol Depend. 2011;118(2-3):224-229.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Saladin
ME, Gray
KM, McRae-Clark
AL,
et al. A double blind, placebo-controlled study of the effects of post-retrieval propranolol on reconsolidation of memory for craving and cue reactivity in cocaine dependent humans.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;226(4):721-737.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Jobes
ML, Aharonovich
E, Epstein
DH,
et al. Effects of prereactivation propranolol on cocaine craving elicited by imagery script/cue sets in opioid-dependent polydrug users: a randomized study.
J Addict Med. 2015;9(6):491-498.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Pachas
GN, Gilman
J, Orr
SP,
et al. Single dose propranolol does not affect physiologic or emotional reactivity to smoking cues.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2015;232(9):1619-1628.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Debiec
J, Díaz-Mataix
L, Bush
DE, Doyère
V, Ledoux
JE. The amygdala encodes specific sensory features of an aversive reinforcer.
Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(5):536-537.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Díaz-Mataix
L, Debiec
J, LeDoux
JE, Doyère
V. Sensory-specific associations stored in the lateral amygdala allow for selective alteration of fear memories.
J Neurosci. 2011;31(26):9538-9543.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Schiller
D, Monfils
MH, Raio
CM, Johnson
DC, Ledoux
JE, Phelps
EA. Preventing the return of fear in humans using reconsolidation update mechanisms.
Nature. 2010;463(7277):49-53.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Luo
YX, Xue
YX, Liu
JF,
et al. A novel UCS memory retrieval-extinction procedure to inhibit relapse to drug seeking.
Nat Commun. 2015;6(7675)1-14.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 32.Pericot-Valverde
I, Secades-Villa
R, Gutiérrez-Maldonado
J, García-Rodríguez
O. Effects of systematic cue exposure through virtual reality on cigarette craving.
Nicotine Tob Res. 2014;16(11):1470-1477.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Liu
J, Zhao
L, Xue
Y,
et al. An unconditioned stimulus retrieval extinction procedure to prevent the return of fear memory.
Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76(11):895-901.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Venniro
M, Caprioli
D, Shaham
Y. Animal models of drug relapse and craving: from drug priming-induced reinstatement to incubation of craving after voluntary abstinence.
Prog Brain Res. 2016;224:25-52.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 35.Mucha
RF, van der Kooy
D, O’Shaughnessy
M, Bucenieks
P. Drug reinforcement studied by the use of place conditioning in rat.
Brain Res. 1982;243(1):91-105.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.O’Brien
CP, Childress
AR, McLellan
AT, Ehrman
R. Classical conditioning in drug-dependent humans.
Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1992;654:400-415.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Sinha
R, Fuse
T, Aubin
LR, O’Malley
SS. Psychological stress, drug-related cues and cocaine craving.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2000;152(2):140-148.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Janes
AC, Ross
RS, Farmer
S,
et al. Memory retrieval of smoking-related images induce greater insula activation as revealed by an fMRI-based delayed matching to sample task.
Addict Biol. 2015;20(2):349-356.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Li
FQ, Xue
YX, Wang
JS,
et al. Basolateral amygdala cdk5 activity mediates consolidation and reconsolidation of memories for cocaine cues.
J Neurosci. 2010;30(31):10351-10359.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Li
SX, Zou
Y, Liu
LJ, Wu
P, Lu
L. Aripiprazole blocks reinstatement but not expression of morphine conditioned place preference in rats.
Pharmacol Biochem Behav. 2009;92(2):370-375.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Wang
XY, Zhao
M, Ghitza
UE, Li
YQ, Lu
L. Stress impairs reconsolidation of drug memory via glucocorticoid receptors in the basolateral amygdala.
J Neurosci. 2008;28(21):5602-5610.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Guillem
K, Peoples
LL. Progressive and lasting amplification of accumbal nicotine-seeking neural signals.
J Neurosci. 2010;30(1):276-286.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Xue
YX, Luo
YX, Wu
P,
et al. A memory retrieval-extinction procedure to prevent drug craving and relapse.
Science. 2012;336(6078):241-245.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Lê
AD, Li
Z, Funk
D, Shram
M, Li
TK, Shaham
Y. Increased vulnerability to nicotine self-administration and relapse in alcohol-naive offspring of rats selectively bred for high alcohol intake.
J Neurosci. 2006;26(6):1872-1879.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Hollander
JA, Lu
Q, Cameron
MD, Kamenecka
TM, Kenny
PJ. Insular hypocretin transmission regulates nicotine reward.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2008;105(49):19480-19485.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Shaham
Y, Adamson
LK, Grocki
S, Corrigall
WA. Reinstatement and spontaneous recovery of nicotine seeking in rats.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 1997;130(4):396-403.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Yan
Y, Pushparaj
A, Le Strat
Y,
et al. Blockade of dopamine d4 receptors attenuates reinstatement of extinguished nicotine-seeking behavior in rats.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(3):685-696.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Franklin
TR, Jagannathan
K, Wetherill
RR,
et al. Influence of menstrual cycle phase on neural and craving responses to appetitive smoking cues in naturally cycling females.
Nicotine Tob Res. 2015;17(4):390-397.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Huttlin
EA, Allen
AM, Tosun
NL, Allen
SS, al’Absi
M. Associations between adrenocortical activity and nicotine response in female smokers by menstrual phase.
Addict Behav. 2015;50:135-139.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Johnsson
G, Regàrdh
CG. Clinical pharmacokinetics of beta-adrenoreceptor blocking drugs.
Clin Pharmacokinet. 1976;1(4):233-263.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Kindt
M, Soeter
M, Vervliet
B. Beyond extinction: erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear.
Nat Neurosci. 2009;12(3):256-258.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Mayo
LM, Fraser
D, Childs
E,
et al. Conditioned preference to a methamphetamine-associated contextual cue in humans.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2013;38(6):921-929.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Ebbert
JO, Hatsukami
DK, Croghan
IT,
et al. Combination varenicline and bupropion SR for tobacco-dependence treatment in cigarette smokers: a randomized trial.
JAMA. 2014;311(2):155-163.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Nader
K, Schafe
GE, Le Doux
JE. Fear memories require protein synthesis in the amygdala for reconsolidation after retrieval.
Nature. 2000;406(6797):722-726.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Miller
CA, Marshall
JF. Molecular substrates for retrieval and reconsolidation of cocaine-associated contextual memory.
Neuron. 2005;47(6):873-884.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Lee
JL, Di Ciano
P, Thomas
KL, Everitt
BJ. Disrupting reconsolidation of drug memories reduces cocaine-seeking behavior.
Neuron. 2005;47(6):795-801.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Diergaarde
L, Schoffelmeer
AN, De Vries
TJ. Beta-adrenoceptor mediated inhibition of long-term reward-related memory reconsolidation.
Behav Brain Res. 2006;170(2):333-336.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Wouda
JA, Diergaarde
L, Riga
D, van Mourik
Y, Schoffelmeer
AN, De Vries
TJ. Disruption of long-term alcohol-related memory reconsolidation: role of β-adrenoceptors and NMDA receptors.
Front Behav Neurosci. 2010;4(179)1-7.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 63.Lonergan
M, Saumier
D, Tremblay
J, Kieffer
B, Brown
TG, Brunet
A. Reactivating addiction-related memories under propranolol to reduce craving: A pilot randomized controlled trial.
J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2016;50:245-249.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Hutton-Bedbrook
K, McNally
GP. The promises and pitfalls of retrieval-extinction procedures in preventing relapse to drug seeking.
Front Psychiatry. 2013;4(14)1-4.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 66.Epstein
DH, Willner-Reid
J, Vahabzadeh
M, Mezghanni
M, Lin
JL, Preston
KL. Real-time electronic diary reports of cue exposure and mood in the hours before cocaine and heroin craving and use.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009;66(1):88-94.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 67.Bossert
JM, Marchant
NJ, Calu
DJ, Shaham
Y. The reinstatement model of drug relapse: recent neurobiological findings, emerging research topics, and translational research.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2013;229(3):453-476.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Carter
BL, Tiffany
ST. The cue-availability paradigm: the effects of cigarette availability on cue reactivity in smokers.
Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. 2001;9(2):183-190.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 69.Winkler
MH, Weyers
P, Mucha
RF, Stippekohl
B, Stark
R, Pauli
P. Conditioned cues for smoking elicit preparatory responses in healthy smokers.
Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;213(4):781-789.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Smith
PH, Bessette
AJ, Weinberger
AH, Sheffer
CE, McKee
SA. Sex/gender differences in smoking cessation: a review.
Prev Med. 2016;92:135-140.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 72.Heilig
M, Epstein
DH, Nader
MA, Shaham
Y. Time to connect: bringing social context into addiction neuroscience.
Nat Rev Neurosci. 2016;17(9):592-599.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref