[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Effective Connectivity of Sucrose Receipt
Effective Connectivity of Sucrose Receipt

The effective connectivity pattern in healthy controls (A) from the right hypothalamus to the ventral striatum is the opposite pattern seen in participants with anorexia nervosa (AN) (B). The connectivity patterns indicate directionality within group but do not reflect a direct group contrast. The blue arrows indicate patterns unique to controls; the green arrows, patterns unique to participants with AN. OFC indicates orbitofrontal cortex.

Figure 2.  Correlation of Prediction Error (PE) With Taste Pleasantness
Correlation of Prediction Error (PE) With Taste Pleasantness

Regional PE results across groups and their correlation with taste pleasantness ratings for 1 molar sucrose solution. Prediction errors and pleasantness ratings were rank-transformed. See eTable 2 in the Supplement for full correlation results across regions and groups.

Figure 3.  Model of Association of Reward Learning Prediction Error (PE) With Harm Avoidance, Weight Change, and Effective Connectivity in Adolescents With Anorexia Nervosa
Model of Association of Reward Learning Prediction Error (PE) With Harm Avoidance, Weight Change, and Effective Connectivity in Adolescents With Anorexia Nervosa

Based on the correlational results, we propose that weight loss is associated with expected sensitization of the dopamine system, reflected in elevated PE to stimulate food approach. However, increased PE may elevate anxiety (harm avoidance) in anorexia nervosa because this is in conflict with a high drive for thinness and body dissatisfaction. Prediction error activation may then become part of a fear-driven mechanism to override homeostatic signals from the hypothalamus, signals that would normally trigger eating. BMI indicates body mass index; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex.

Table 1.  Demographic and Behavioral Variables
Demographic and Behavioral Variables
Table 2.  Parameter Estimate Analyses Across Groupsa
Parameter Estimate Analyses Across Groupsa
1.
American Psychiatric Association.  Desk Reference to the Diagnostic Criteria from DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
2.
Arcelus  J, Mitchell  AJ, Wales  J, Nielsen  S.  Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders: a meta-analysis of 36 studies.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(7):724-731. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.74PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Kaye  WH, Fudge  JL, Paulus  M.  New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(8):573-584. doi:10.1038/nrn2682PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Klump  KL, Bulik  CM, Pollice  C,  et al.  Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 2000;188(9):559-567. doi:10.1097/00005053-200009000-00001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Strober  M, Freeman  R, Lampert  C, Diamond  J.  The association of anxiety disorders and obsessive compulsive personality disorder with anorexia nervosa: evidence from a family study with discussion of nosological and neurodevelopmental implications.  Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40(suppl):S46-S51. doi:10.1002/eat.20429PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Deep  AL, Nagy  LM, Weltzin  TE, Rao  R, Kaye  WH.  Premorbid onset of psychopathology in long-term recovered anorexia nervosa.  Int J Eat Disord. 1995;17(3):291-297.PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Bulik  CM, Sullivan  PF, Fear  JL, Pickering  A.  Outcome of anorexia nervosa: eating attitudes, personality, and parental bonding.  Int J Eat Disord. 2000;28(2):139-147. doi:10.1002/1098-108X(200009)28:2<139::AID-EAT2>3.0.CO;2-GPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Klump  KL, Strober  M, Bulik  CM,  et al.  Personality characteristics of women before and after recovery from an eating disorder.  Psychol Med. 2004;34(8):1407-1418. doi:10.1017/S0033291704002442PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Bailer  UF, Frank  GK, Price  JC,  et al.  Interaction between serotonin transporter and dopamine D2/D3 receptor radioligand measures is associated with harm avoidant symptoms in anorexia and bulimia nervosa.  Psychiatry Res. 2013;211(2):160-168. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.06.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Bailer  UF, Price  JC, Meltzer  CC,  et al.  Dopaminergic activity and altered reward modulation in anorexia nervosa: insight from multimodal imaging.  Int J Eat Disord. 2017;50(5):593-596. doi:10.1002/eat.22638PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Bowman  ES, Coons  PM.  The differential diagnosis of epilepsy, pseudoseizures, dissociative identity disorder, and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified.  Bull Menninger Clin. 2000;64(2):164-180.PubMedGoogle Scholar
12.
Frank  GK, Bailer  UF, Henry  SE,  et al.  Increased dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [11c]raclopride.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(11):908-912. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Kaye  WH, Wierenga  CE, Bailer  UF, Simmons  AN, Bischoff-Grethe  A.  Nothing tastes as good as skinny feels: the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa.  Trends Neurosci. 2013;36(2):110-120. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2013.01.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Oinio  V, Bäckström  P, Uhari-Väänänen  J, Raasmaja  A, Piepponen  P, Kiianmaa  K.  Dopaminergic modulation of reward-guided decision making in alcohol-preferring AA rats.  Behav Brain Res. 2017;326:87-95. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2017.03.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Kelley  AE.  Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: role in ingestive behavior and reward-related learning.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27(8):765-776. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Monteleone  AM, Castellini  G, Volpe  U,  et al.  Neuroendocrinology and brain imaging of reward in eating disorders: a possible key to the treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2018;80(pt B):132-142. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.02.020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Frank  GK.  The perfect storm: a bio-psycho-social risk model for developing and maintaining eating disorders.  Front Behav Neurosci. 2016;10:44. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00044PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Schultz  W.  Getting formal with dopamine and reward.  Neuron. 2002;36(2):241-263. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
O’Doherty  JP, Dayan  P, Friston  K, Critchley  H, Dolan  RJ.  Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain.  Neuron. 2003;38(2):329-337. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00169-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Cowdrey  FA, Park  RJ, Harmer  CJ, McCabe  C.  Increased neural processing of rewarding and aversive food stimuli in recovered anorexia nervosa.  Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(8):736-743. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.028PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Frank  GK, Reynolds  JR, Shott  ME,  et al.  Anorexia nervosa and obesity are associated with opposite brain reward response.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(9):2031-2046. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.51PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
DeGuzman  M, Shott  ME, Yang  TT, Riederer  J, Frank  GKW.  Association of elevated reward prediction error response with weight gain in adolescent anorexia nervosa.  Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(6):557-565. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16060671PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Golden  NH.  Eating disorders in adolescence and their sequelae.  Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol. 2003;17(1):57-73. doi:10.1053/ybeog.2003.0344PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Silverman  MH, Jedd  K, Luciana  M.  Neural networks involved in adolescent reward processing: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies.  Neuroimage. 2015;122:427-439. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.083PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Barkley-Levenson  E, Galván  A.  Neural representation of expected value in the adolescent brain.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(4):1646-1651. doi:10.1073/pnas.1319762111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Cohen  JR, Asarnow  RF, Sabb  FW,  et al.  A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors.  Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(6):669-671. doi:10.1038/nn.2558PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Frank  GK, Shott  ME, Riederer  J, Pryor  TL.  Altered structural and effective connectivity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa in circuits that regulate energy and reward homeostasis.  Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(11):e932. doi:10.1038/tp.2016.199PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Beck  KD, Luine  VN.  Food deprivation modulates chronic stress effects on object recognition in male rats: role of monoamines and amino acids.  Brain Res. 1999;830(1):56-71. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01380-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Monteleone  P, Scognamiglio  P, Monteleone  AM,  et al.  Abnormal diurnal patterns of salivary α-amylase and cortisol secretion in acute patients with anorexia nervosa.  World J Biol Psychiatry. 2011;12(6):455-461. doi:10.3109/15622975.2011.590226PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Paszynska  E, Dmitrzak-Weglarz  M, Tyszkiewicz-Nwafor  M, Slopien  A.  Salivary alpha-amylase, secretory IgA and free cortisol as neurobiological components of the stress response in the acute phase of anorexia nervosa.  World J Biol Psychiatry. 2016;17(4):266-273. doi:10.3109/15622975.2016.1163419PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Oswald  LM, Wong  DF, McCaul  M,  et al.  Relationships among ventral striatal dopamine release, cortisol secretion, and subjective responses to amphetamine.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30(4):821-832. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300667PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Pérez-Neri  I, Montes  S, Ojeda-López  C, Ramírez-Bermúdez  J, Ríos  C.  Modulation of neurotransmitter systems by dehydroepiandrosterone and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate: mechanism of action and relevance to psychiatric disorders.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008;32(5):1118-1130. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.12.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Wise  RA, Morales  M.  A ventral tegmental CRF-glutamate-dopamine interaction in addiction.  Brain Res. 2010;1314:38-43. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.101PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Koob  G, Kreek  MJ.  Stress, dysregulation of drug reward pathways, and the transition to drug dependence.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(8):1149-1159. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05030503PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Therrien  F, Drapeau  V, Lupien  SJ,  et al.  Awakening cortisol response in relation to psychosocial profiles and eating behaviors.  Physiol Behav. 2008;93(1-2):282-288. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.08.019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Lupis  SB, Sabik  NJ, Wolf  JM.  Role of shame and body esteem in cortisol stress responses.  J Behav Med. 2016;39(2):262-275. doi:10.1007/s10865-015-9695-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Garner  D.  Eating Disorder Inventory™-3 (EDI™-3). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2004.
38.
Torrubia  R, Avila  C, Molto  J, Caseras  X.  The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions.  Pers Individ Dif. 2001;31:837-862. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00183-5Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Spielberger  CD.  Manual for the State-Trate Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc; 1983.
40.
Cloninger  CR, Przybeck  TR, Svrakic  DM, Wetzel  RD.  The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to its Development and Use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University; 1994.
41.
Kovacs  M.  The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI). Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Inc; 1992.
42.
Frank  GKW, Favaro  A, Marsh  R, Ehrlich  S, Lawson  EA.  Toward valid and reliable brain imaging results in eating disorders.  Int J Eat Disord. 2018;51(3):250-261. doi:10.1002/eat.22829PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Lock  J, Le Grange  D, Agras  WS, Moye  A, Bryson  SW, Jo  B.  Randomized clinical trial comparing family-based treatment with adolescent-focused individual therapy for adolescents with anorexia nervosa.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(10):1025-1032. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.128PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Sheehan  DV, Sheehan  KH, Shytle  RD,  et al.  Reliability and validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID).  J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(3):313-326. doi:10.4088/JCP.09m05305whiPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Frank  GK, Kaye  WH, Carter  CS,  et al.  The evaluation of brain activity in response to taste stimuli: a pilot study and method for central taste activation as assessed by event-related fMRI.  J Neurosci Methods. 2003;131(1-2):99-105. doi:10.1016/S0165-0270(03)00240-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
O’Doherty  J, Dayan  P, Schultz  J, Deichmann  R, Friston  K, Dolan  RJ.  Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning.  Science. 2004;304(5669):452-454. doi:10.1126/science.1094285PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Breiter  HC, Gollub  RL, Weisskoff  RM,  et al.  Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion.  Neuron. 1997;19(3):591-611. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80374-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Tzourio-Mazoyer  N, Landeau  B, Papathanassiou  D,  et al.  Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain.  Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273-289. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0978PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Ramsey  JD, Hanson  SJ, Hanson  C, Halchenko  YO, Poldrack  RA, Glymour  C.  Six problems for causal inference from fMRI.  Neuroimage. 2010;49(2):1545-1558. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.065PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Westfall  PH.  On using the bootstrap for multiple comparisons.  J Biopharm Stat. 2011;21(6):1187-1205. doi:10.1080/10543406.2011.607751PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Branch  SY, Goertz  RB, Sharpe  AL,  et al.  Food restriction increases glutamate receptor-mediated burst firing of dopamine neurons.  J Neurosci. 2013;33(34):13861-13872. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5099-12.2013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Carr  KD, Tsimberg  Y, Berman  Y, Yamamoto  N.  Evidence of increased dopamine receptor signaling in food-restricted rats.  Neuroscience. 2003;119(4):1157-1167. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00227-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Thanos  PK, Michaelides  M, Piyis  YK, Wang  GJ, Volkow  ND.  Food restriction markedly increases dopamine D2 receptor (D2R) in a rat model of obesity as assessed with in-vivo muPET imaging ([11C] raclopride) and in-vitro ([3H] spiperone) autoradiography.  Synapse. 2008;62(1):50-61. doi:10.1002/syn.20468PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Frank  GK, Collier  S, Shott  ME, O’Reilly  RC.  Prediction error and somatosensory insula activation in women recovered from anorexia nervosa.  J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2016;41(5):304-311. doi:10.1503/jpn.150103PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Stratford  TR, Kelley  AE.  Evidence of a functional relationship between the nucleus accumbens shell and lateral hypothalamus subserving the control of feeding behavior.  J Neurosci. 1999;19(24):11040-11048. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-11040.1999PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Castro  DC, Cole  SL, Berridge  KC.  Lateral hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, and ventral pallidum roles in eating and hunger: interactions between homeostatic and reward circuitry.  Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:90. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2015.00090PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Izquierdo  A, Jentsch  JD.  Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012;219(2):607-620. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2579-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Kringelbach  ML, Rolls  ET.  The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology.  Prog Neurobiol. 2004;72(5):341-372. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.03.006PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Small  DM, Bender  G, Veldhuizen  MG, Rudenga  K, Nachtigal  D, Felsted  J.  The role of the human orbitofrontal cortex in taste and flavor processing.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:136-151. doi:10.1196/annals.1401.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Small  DM, Jones-Gotman  M, Dagher  A.  Feeding-induced dopamine release in dorsal striatum correlates with meal pleasantness ratings in healthy human volunteers.  Neuroimage. 2003;19(4):1709-1715. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00253-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Ahn  S, Phillips  AG.  Modulation by central and basolateral amygdalar nuclei of dopaminergic correlates of feeding to satiety in the rat nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex.  J Neurosci. 2002;22(24):10958-10965. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10958.2002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Hajnal  A, Norgren  R.  Accumbens dopamine mechanisms in sucrose intake.  Brain Res. 2001;904(1):76-84. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02451-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Lawson  EA, Holsen  LM, Desanti  R,  et al.  Increased hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal drive is associated with decreased appetite and hypoactivation of food-motivation neurocircuitry in anorexia nervosa.  Eur J Endocrinol. 2013;169(5):639-647. doi:10.1530/EJE-13-0433PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Robinson  OJ, Overstreet  C, Charney  DR, Vytal  K, Grillon  C.  Stress increases aversive prediction error signal in the ventral striatum.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(10):4129-4133. doi:10.1073/pnas.1213923110PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Anderson  BA, Kuwabara  H, Wong  DF,  et al.  Linking dopaminergic reward signals to the development of attentional bias: a positron emission tomographic study.  Neuroimage. 2017;157:27-33. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.062PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Investigation
October 2018

Association of Brain Reward Learning Response With Harm Avoidance, Weight Gain, and Hypothalamic Effective Connectivity in Adolescent Anorexia Nervosa

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, School of Medicine, Aurora
  • 2Neuroscience Program, University of Colorado Anschutz Medical Campus, Aurora
  • 3Eating Disorder Care, Denver, Colorado
JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(10):1071-1080. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2151
Key Points

Question  How does brain response in participants with adolescent anorexia nervosa (AN) compare with healthy controls during taste reward conditioning?

Findings  In this cross-sectional multimodal brain imaging study of 56 female adolescents and young adults with AN and 52 matched controls, the AN group showed hyperactivation in the caudate head, nucleus accumbens, and insula compared with controls during a classical conditioning paradigm that has been associated with dopamine function. Orbitofrontal brain response in the AN group was positively associated with harm avoidance and striatal-hypothalamic connectivity but negatively associated with change in body mass index during treatment.

Meanings  Altered brain reward response in adolescent AN may indicate altered dopamine function, may have a key role in AN’s specific pathophysiology, and should be explored as a target for biological treatments.

Abstract

Importance  Anorexia nervosa (AN) is associated with adolescent onset, severe low body weight, and high mortality as well as high harm avoidance. The brain reward system could have an important role in the perplexing drive for thinness and food avoidance in AN.

Objective  To test whether brain reward learning response to taste in adolescent AN is altered and associated with treatment response, striatal-hypothalamic connectivity, and elevated harm avoidance.

Design, Setting, and Participants  In this cross-sectional multimodal brain imaging study, adolescents and young adults with AN were matched with healthy controls at a university brain imaging facility and eating disorder treatment program. During a sucrose taste classical conditioning paradigm, violations of learned associations between conditioned visual and unconditioned taste stimuli evoked the dopamine-related prediction error (PE). Dynamic effective connectivity during sweet taste receipt was studied to investigate hierarchical brain activation across the brain network that regulates eating. The study was conducted from July 2012 to May 2017, and data were analyzed from June 2017 to December 2017.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Prediction error brain reward response across the insula, caudate, and orbitofrontal cortex; dynamic effective connectivity between hypothalamus and ventral striatum; and treatment weight gain, harm avoidance scores, and salivary cortisol levels and their correlations with PE brain response.

Results  Of 56 female participants with AN included in the study, the mean (SD) age was 16.6 (2.5) years, and the mean (SD) body mass index (BMI; calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared) was 15.9 (0.9); of 52 matched female controls, the mean (SD) age was 16.0 (2.8) years, and the mean (SD) BMI was 20.9 (2.1). Prediction error response was elevated in participants with AN in the caudate head, nucleus accumbens, and insula (multivariate analysis of covariance: Wilks λ, 0.707; P = .02; partial η2 = 0.296), which correlated negatively with sucrose taste pleasantness. Bilateral AN orbitofrontal gyrus rectus PE response was positively correlated with harm avoidance (right ρ, 0.317; 95% CI, 0.091 to 0.539; P < .02; left ρ, 0.336; 95% CI, 0.112 to 0.550; P < .01) but negatively correlated with treatment BMI change (right ρ, −0.282; 95% CI, −0.534 to −0.014; P < .04; left ρ, −0.268; 95% CI, −0.509 to −0.018; P < .045). Participants with AN showed effective connectivity from ventral striatum to hypothalamus, and connectivity strength was positively correlated with insula and orbitofrontal PE response. Right frontal cortex PE response was associated with cortisol, which correlated with body dissatisfaction.

Conclusions and Relevance  These results further support elevated PE signal in AN and suggest a link between PE and elevated harm avoidance, brain connectivity, and weight gain in AN. Prediction error may have a central role in adolescent AN in driving anxiety and ventral striatal-hypothalamus circuit-controlled food avoidance.

Introduction

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is a psychiatric disorder characterized by fear of weight gain and dangerously low body weight.1 Anorexia nervosa primarily affects young girls and young women, and its mortality rate exceeds that of other psychiatric disorders.2 Self-starvation and fear of weight gain despite severe underweight and risk of death have been puzzling, and finding comprehensive brain-based models that could explain the constellation of these behaviors has been difficult.3

It has been hypothesized that anxious traits are vulnerability factors for AN; specifically, elevated harm avoidance (HA) has been found in individuals with AN.4-6 Harm avoidance is a temperament trait characterized by excessive worry and fearfulness that has been associated with poor AN treatment outcome and is higher in the ill state compared with the recovered state.7,8 Harm avoidance was found to be correlated with serotonin neurotransmitter receptor availability and, more recently, dopamine receptor binding in AN after recovery.3,9-13 Dopamine is a learning signal and is important for food approach, and animal models suggest enhanced neuronal dopamine activation following food restriction.14,15 This led to the hypothesis that brain circuits that involve dopamine are important for the pathophysiology of AN.16,17 Unexpected reward receipt and omission have been associated with brain dopamine level, the so-called prediction error (PE) response, and have been studied in AN.18,19 In adults with AN in the ill and recovered states, unexpected or randomly applied sucrose taste stimuli evoked higher insular and striatal responses,20,21 and unexpected omission or receipt of monetary reward in adolescent AN also resulted in heightened responses in those regions.22 One interpretation of such findings may lend itself to a model of brain changes in AN: an enhanced dopamine reward system response is an adaptation to starvation to stimulate motivation to approach food.14 Notably, peak onset of AN is in midadolescence,23 when sensitivity to reward and reward PE response is still developing.24-26 Individuals vulnerable to develop AN could be particularly sensitive to food restriction and adaptations of reward response during that developmental period.

In this study, we tested specific hypotheses to integrate PE reward signals with anxiety and core eating disorder signs.17 Specifically, we hypothesized that reward learning PE response would be elevated in AN and associated with HA. Second, we wanted to test the hypothesis that PE response is associated with neurocircuitry that regulates appetite and food intake. Previously, we found a pattern of effective connectivity (direction of activation) from ventral striatum to hypothalamus in adults with AN.27 We interpreted this as a possible mechanism of how the brain could override hunger signals in AN. We hypothesized that adolescents with AN show a similar pattern. Third, food restriction is a stressor and is associated with increased brain cortisol levels.28-30 Cortisol level affects dopamine release and postsynaptic dopamine D2 receptors,31-33 and we wanted to test whether cortisol level was associated with PE response and core AN behaviors.34-36

Methods
Participants

Fifty-six female adolescents and young adults with AN (age range, 11-21 years) and 52 healthy matched control participants (age range, 11-21 years) were included in this study (Table 1).37-41 The AN group was recruited from partial hospitalization treatment, where closely supervised meal plans mitigated confounding brain effects of acute starvation or dehydration.42 Treatment involved a highly structured program aimed at weight restoration over 5 weeks, including parent training in meal support according to the family-based treatment model.43 Control participants were recruited through local advertisements. In the AN group, 53 participants were diagnosed as having pure restricting type and 3 as having infrequent purge episodes (less than once a month), and all 56 participants with AN fell below the 10th percentile for body mass index (BMI; calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared) for age. All participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI); individuals with AN were without menses and controls were in the early follicular phase to control for sex hormone effects. Participants 18 years or older, including 19 participants with AN and 11 controls, were administered the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 by a doctoral-level interviewer. Those younger than 18 years completed the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview.44 Participants were right-handed and had no history of head trauma, neurological disease, major medical illness, psychosis, or substance use disorders. Six participants with AN and 11 controls were taking oral contraceptives. Twenty-six participants with AN were taking antidepressants, and 7 were taking atypical antipsychotics. The Colorado Multiple institutional review board approved the study. All participants provided written informed consent.

Self-Assessments

In addition to diagnostic interviews, participants completed a battery of self-assessments. Participants completed the Eating Disorder Inventory–3,37 Revised Sensitivity to Punishment and Reward Questionnaire,38 State-Trait Anxiety Inventory,39 Temperament and Character Inventory,40 and Children’s Depression Inventory.41

Brain Imaging Methods
fMRI Image Acquisition

Between 7:00 am and 9:00 am on the study day, participants with AN ate their meal plan breakfast and controls ate a quality-matched and calorie-matched breakfast (Table 1). Brain imaging was performed between 8:00 am and 9:00 am using the 3T Signa scanner (General Electric Company) or Skyra 3T scanner (Siemens) using the following criteria: 3-plane scout scan (16 seconds), sagittally acquired, spoiled gradient sequence T1-weighted (172 slices; thickness, 1 mm; inversion time, 450 ms; repetition time, 8 ms; echo time, 4 ms; flip angle, 12°; field of view, 22 cm; scan matrix, 64 × 64), and T2-weighted echo planar scans for blood oxygen level–dependent functional activity (3.4 × 3.4 × 2.6-mm voxels; repetition time, 2100 ms; echo time, 30 ms; flip angle, 70°; 28 axial slices; thickness, 2.6 mm; gap, 1.4 mm) (eMethods 1 in the Supplement).

Taste Reward Task

The design of this study was adapted from O’Doherty et al19 (eAppendix 1 in the Supplement). Participants learned to associate 3 unconditioned taste stimuli (1 molar sucrose solution, no solution, or artificial saliva) with paired conditioned visual stimuli. Each conditioned visual stimulus was probabilistically associated with its unconditioned taste stimulus such that 20% of sucrose and no solution conditioned visual stimulus trials were unexpectedly followed by no solution and sucrose unconditioned taste stimuli, respectively. Taste stimuli were applied using a customized programmable syringe pump (J-Kem Scientific) and E-Prime software version 2 (Psychological Software Tools).45

fMRI Analysis

Image preprocessing and analysis were performed using statistical parametric mapping version 12 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging). Images were realigned to the first volume, normalized to the Montreal Neurological Institute template, and smoothed at 6-mm full width at half maximum gaussian kernel. Data were preprocessed with slice time correction and modeled with a hemodynamic response convolved function using the general linear model, including temporal and dispersion derivatives. A 128-second high-pass filter was applied for low-frequency blood oxygen level–dependent signal fluctuations and motion parameters as first-level analysis regressors.

PE Analysis

Each participant’s PE signal was modeled based on trial sequence (absolute of positive and negative PE) and regressed with brain activation across all trials19,21,22 (eMethods 2 in the Supplement). We extracted mean parameter estimates across all voxels from 18 predefined anatomical regions of interest (ROIs) based on previous studies,22 including the bilateral dorsal anterior insula, ventral anterior insula, caudate head, orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) gyrus rectus, medial OFC, middle OFC, inferior OFC, ventral striatum,46 and nucleus accumbens47 (http://marsbar.sourceforge.net/; automated anatomical labeling Atlas48).

Effective Connectivity Analysis

We extracted ROI functional activation for trials of expected receipt of 1 molar sucrose solution (n = 80), with conditioned visual stimuli and unconditioned taste stimuli trial length of 6 seconds, as previously studied in adults.27 The Tetrad-V program49 was used to infer effective connectivity with independent multisample greedy equivalence search and linear nongaussian orientation, fixed structure search algorithms. This analysis aimed to understand causal associations among neuronal populations whose activity gives rise to observed fMRI signals in spatially localized ROIs27 (eMethods 3 in the Supplement). We extracted edge coefficients for ventral striatum–hypothalamus connectivity to test for correlations with behavior or PE values.

Cortisol Collection and Analysis

On the scan day, a subset of 20 participants with AN and 25 controls provided 0.5-mL samples of saliva (passive drool) in 5-mL Screw Cap Micro Tubes (Thermo Fisher Scientific; eAppendix 2 and eTable 1 in the Supplement). Samples were collected 30 minutes prior to breakfast, 30 minutes after breakfast, and right before brain imaging. Samples were stored at −15° C until analysis. Cortisol was assayed using commercial immunoassays (Salimetrics). The area under the curve (AUC; trapezoid method with 3 time points) was calculated and correlated with ROI PE response and across the whole brain (family-wise error rate [FWE] P < .05; eAppendix 2 in the Supplement).

Statistical Analysis

SPSS Statistics 25 (IBM) was used for statistical analyses. Demographic and behavior data were analyzed using t test. Extracted regional brain activation parameter estimates were tested for normality with the Shapiro-Wilk test, rank-transformed when nonnormally distributed, and analyzed using multivariate analysis of variance and multivariate analysis of covariance with covariates to account for confounding factors, such as comorbidity or medication, as in previous studies.22 Spearman rank was used for correlation analyses and controlled for multiple comparisons using bootstrapping procedures (1000 samples).50 All P values were 2-tailed, and a P value less than .05 was considered significant. Results were corrected for multiple comparison.

Results
Demographic and Behavioral Data

There were no significant group differences for age, breakfast calories, or sucrose pleasantness or sweetness ratings (Table 1). Participants with AN had significantly lower age-adjusted BMI percentile and novelty-seeking scores. Participants with AN also had elevated drive for thinness and body dissatisfaction and significantly higher HA, punishment sensitivity, state and trait anxiety, and depression scores.

Brain Imaging Results
PE ROI Analysis

Results were nonnormally distributed. Multivariate analysis of variance (no covariates) resulted in a Wilks λ of 0.642 (P < .001; partial η2 = 0.358), with associations with bilateral caudate head, ventral striatum, nucleus accumbens, right inferior OFC, right medial OFC, right gyrus rectus, right dorsal anterior insula, and right ventral anterior insula surviving Bonferroni correction (Table 2). Multivariate analysis of covariance (with age, scanner, antidepressant use, antipsychotic use, comorbid depression, and comorbid anxiety as covariates) resulted in a Wilks λ of 0.707 (P = .02; partial η2 = 0.296), with associations with right and left caudate head, right and left nucleus accumbens, and right ventral anterior insula surviving Bonferroni correction. There were no significant differences between scanners for within-group ROI comparisons, nor did results change when the scanner covariate was removed (eFigure 1 in the Supplement).

Effective Connectivity

Sucrose anticipation and receipt elicited patterns of connectivity that were similar for 50% of identified connections across groups. However, for our effective connectivity of interest, there were different patterns between groups bilaterally; the hypothalamus in controls directed activation to the ventral striatum, whereas in participants with AN, the ventral striatum directed effective connectivity to the hypothalamus (Figure 1).

Correlation Analyses

Rate of age-adjusted change of BMI percentile to reach target weight (BMI percentile change mean [SD] of 20.37 [15.12] over a mean [SD] time of 40.01 [10.94] days = 0.51 age-adjusted BMI percentile change per day) was negatively correlated with response in the inferior OFC (right ρ, −0.389; 95% CI, −0.612 to −0.100; P < .003), middle OFC (right ρ, −0.281; 95% CI, −0.522 to −0.018; P < .04), gyrus rectus (right ρ, −0.282; 95% CI, −0.534 to −0.014; P < .04; left ρ, −0.268; 95% CI, −0.509 to −0.018; P < .045), dorsal anterior insula (right ρ, −0.358; 95% CI, −0.590 to −0.092; P < .007; left ρ, −0.281; 95% CI, −0.542 to −0.004; P < .04), and ventral anterior insula (right ρ, −0.274; 95% CI, −0.512 to −0.016; P < .04). Prediction error regression weights did not significantly correlate with admission age-adjusted BMI percentiles. In participants with AN, HA was positively correlated with response in OFC gyrus rectus (right ρ, 0.317; 95% CI, 0.091 to 0.539; P < .02; left ρ, 0.336; 95% CI, 0.112 to 0.550; P < .01). Harm avoidance in participants with AN was positively correlated with drive for thinness (ρ, 0.381; 95% CI, 0.140 to 0.587; P < .004) and body dissatisfaction (ρ, 0.312; 95% CI, 0.055 to 0.541; P < .02) (eFigure 2 in the Supplement).

Sucrose pleasantness was negatively correlated with PE in all regions studied (ρ range, −0.451 to −0.249) in participants with AN but only in the middle OFC in controls. A comparison of regression slopes showed significantly different slopes between groups in the caudate head (right Fisher z, 2.103; P < .04), medial OFC (left Fisher z, 2.204; P < .03), and nucleus accumbens (right Fisher z, 1.958; P < .050; left Fisher z, 2.293; P < .02) (Figure 2) (eTable 2 in the Supplement). In the AN group, taste pleasantness was negatively correlated with HA (ρ, −0.294; 95% CI, −0.041 to −0.510; P < .03). In participants with AN, ventral striatum–hypothalamus edge coefficients were correlated with ipsilateral inferior OFC PE (right ρ, 0.318; 95% CI, 0.063 to 0.547; P < .02; left ρ, 0.354; 95% CI, 0.059 to 0.606; P < .007), middle OFC PE (right ρ, 0.308; 95% CI, 0.044 to 0.554; P < .02; left ρ, 0.427; 95% CI, 0.142 to 0.646; P < .001), and dorsal anterior insula PE (right ρ, 0.392; 95% CI, 0.170 to 0.602; P < .003).

Cortisol Analysis

The area under the curve for cortisol levels was elevated in participants with AN compared with controls (t29.169 = −2.515; P = .02) (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). In participants with AN, the AUC for cortisol levels was positively correlated with caudate head PE (right ρ, 0.0457; 95% CI, 0.122 to 0.692; P < .04). Whole-brain regression (FWE corrected) showed that cortisol level was significantly positively correlated with PE response in the right superior frontal gyrus (x = −18; y = 58; z = 6) in participants with AN (peak FWE P = .005; κ = 1). Subsequent small volume correction (P < .001; κ = 10) within the anatomical superior frontal gyrus ROI resulted in a significant cluster (peak FWE P < .001; κ = 53) (eAppendix 2 in the Supplement). When parameter estimates were extracted from the FWE-corrected cluster, the strength of the cortisol regression was significantly positively correlated with body dissatisfaction scores in participants with AN (ρ, 0.484; 95% CI, 0.013 to 0.815; P < .03) (eFigure 3 in the Supplement). No significant clusters or behavioral correlations were found in the control group.

Discussion

Anorexia nervosa is a perplexing psychiatric illness, and the complex biopsychosocial aspects of the illness have made it difficult to develop brain-based models for food restriction. The results from this study show (1) heightened caudate, nucleus accumbens, and insula taste PE signal in adolescents with AN; (2) ventral striatal–hypothalamic dynamic effective connectivity in participants with AN that was opposite to controls and that was positively correlated with insular and orbitofrontal PE signal; (3) a positive correlation of cortisol level with PE in a subset of participants; and (4) a positive correlation of OFC PE with HA and a negative correlation of orbitofrontal and insula PE with BMI change during treatment, as seen previously.22 The results suggest that the PE signal could be an important marker for weight gain and anxiety in adolescent AN.

The first part of this study shows heightened taste reward PE signal across the caudate, nucleus accumbens, and insula in a large sample of adolescents and young adults with AN, comparable with results in adults.21 Starvation is associated with adaptations of the body to drive food intake,51-53 including changes in dopamine release and receptor expression.53 We expect that in AN, the elevated PE response, which has been associated with brain dopamine activity, is an adaptation to food restriction and weight loss that normalizes with long-term recovery.54

The second major finding was a pattern of activation during sweet taste anticipation and receipt that was directed from the ventral striatum to the hypothalamus in AN and was positively correlated with OFC and insular PE signal. This was in contrast to the direction of activation in controls from the hypothalamus to the ventral striatum, a connection thought to be particularly important for feeding regulation.55 A dopamine-dependent pathway from the ventral striatum to the hypothalamus has been described that mediates fear.56 This lends itself to the hypothesis that PE signal in AN might activate this circuitry and override appetitive hypothalamic signals.

Our analyses also indicate direct associations of PE response with HA, BMI change, and taste perception. Orbitofrontal cortex gyrus rectus PE was negatively correlated with BMI increase during treatment but positively correlated with HA, which in turn was positively correlated with drive for thinness and body dissatisfaction. This indicates that PE values could be directly associated with HA and BMI change in AN when in the ill state, although OFC PE was not elevated in participants with AN compared with controls. Complex behaviors are driven by the balance between neurotransmitter systems57 and imbalance between, for instance, dopamine and serotonin neurotransmission in those with AN could make the PE signal relatively more important in its association with HA. Our data also show that HA is directly correlated with core AN behavior, such as drive for thinness and body dissatisfaction, suggesting that anxiety is an important driver of the cognitive/emotional aspects specific to AN. This study suggests that dopamine circuits via PE signaling could be involved with elevated HA. However, the specific neurotransmitter systems underlying those results need further exploration.9,10,12 Pleasant taste stimulates dopamine release to promote eating and typically activates OFC response.58-60 Our data raise the possibility that adolescents with AN in this study were negatively conditioned to sweet taste and may have developed an inverse association with dopamine release across the larger reward circuitry.61,62 A possible explanation could be that high HA drives low taste pleasantness, making the taste experience less pleasant.

Consistent with other studies, those with AN exhibited higher AUC for cortisol levels compared with controls,29,30 which may alter appetite regulation in AN.63 Depression scores were not correlated with cortisol levels. The positive regression of AUC for cortisol level and PE signal in participants with AN suggests that stress enhances PE signals.64 Prediction error acts as a learning signal and affects value-driven attentional bias,65 and stress response may affect how individuals with AN process and form associations with salient stimuli. Although correlational, our data point to a model hypothesis for further investigation: cognitive drivers in AN, such as severe body dissatisfaction, could increase stress hormone levels, which both suppress eating and enhance PE signals.

Taking our data together with previous research, we propose the following model to explain the paradoxical food restriction in AN (Figure 3): Food restriction and weight loss are associated with sensitization of the dopamine system14 and reflected in AN by elevated PE signal, probably to stimulate food approach.11,12 However, PE response may increase HA in AN because this biological mechanism to seek out food is inconsistent with the high drive for thinness and body dissatisfaction. Thus, there is a conflict between food approach mechanisms (PE) and cognitive-emotional processes that oppose eating (body dissatisfaction and drive for thinness). Prediction error activation may then become part of a fear-driven mechanism that includes the ventral striatum to override homeostatic signals from the hypothalamus, which would normally trigger food intake. Future studies will have to test the validity of this model and test whether this circuitry indeed activates or involves the previously described fear-mediating pathway from the ventral striatum to the hypothalamus.56

Limitations

This study has limitations. Functional magnetic resonance imaging does not directly measure dopaminergic signaling; the biologically based computational model used in this study provides strong evidence of altered dopamine-related taste reward processing in adolescent AN, but specific pharmacological challenge studies are needed to further support this model. The dynamic connectivity analysis included conditioned visual stimulus and unconditioned taste stimulus response; future studies will be required to separate these over the full hemodynamic response time. Power for the cortisol level analysis was limited because we were unable to collect salivary cortisol samples from the entire cohort. The analysis of 20 participants with AN and 25 controls still resulted in robust, multiple comparison–corrected findings, although replication is needed. The data were collected on 2 separate MRI scanners. However, comparison results did not indicate within-group differences. We did not assess learning rates; however, a subset of the study participants showed similar learning rates across groups in a monetary PE paradigm.22 Structural brain alterations could affect brain function, but there were no differences across groups (eFigure 4 in the Supplement). The age range up to 21 years was within child range by National Institute of Mental Health standards at the start of the study. However, there is no evidence that extending the current definition by 3 years would confound the results.

Conclusions

Prediction error brain response may have a central role in adolescent AN and illness behaviors. However, longitudinal studies and neurotransmitter challenge studies are needed to further understand how brain circuits are disrupted or altered in AN. This will help to identify neurobiological systems that are involved in AN pathophysiology and to develop targeted biological interventions. Another goal will be to identify demographic, behavioral, and biological variables that can predict PE signal and can be measured without a brain scan to make this mechanism clinically more accessible and useful.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: June 25, 2018.

Published Online: July 19, 2018. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.2151

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2018 Frank GKW et al. JAMA Psychiatry.

Corresponding Author: Guido K. W. Frank, MD, Children’s Hospital Colorado, Gary Pavilion B-130, 13123 E 16th Ave, Aurora, CO 80045 (guido.frank@ucdenver.edu).

Author Contributions: Dr Frank had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis. Dr Frank and Jason Tregellas, PhD, are principal investigators.

Study concept and design: Frank, Shott.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Frank, DeGuzman, Shott, Laudenslager, Rossi.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Frank, DeGuzman, Shott, Laudenslager, Pryor.

Statistical analysis: Frank, DeGuzman, Shott.

Obtained funding: Frank.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Frank, Shott, Laudenslager, Rossi, Pryor.

Study supervision: Frank.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: The study was supported by grants MH096777 and MH103436 from the National Institute of Mental Health (Dr Frank) and grant 1S10OD018435-01 from the National Institutes of Health (principal investigator Jason Tregellas). Ms DeGuzman was supported by grant T32HD041697 to the University of Colorado Neuroscience Program from the National Institutes of Health and by grant TL1 TR001081 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences Colorado Clinical and Translational Science Awards.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Meeting Presentation: This paper was presented at the 26th Annual Meeting of the Society for the Study of Ingestive Behavior; July 19, 2018; Bonita Springs, Florida.

References
1.
American Psychiatric Association.  Desk Reference to the Diagnostic Criteria from DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013.
2.
Arcelus  J, Mitchell  AJ, Wales  J, Nielsen  S.  Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders: a meta-analysis of 36 studies.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(7):724-731. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.74PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Kaye  WH, Fudge  JL, Paulus  M.  New insights into symptoms and neurocircuit function of anorexia nervosa.  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009;10(8):573-584. doi:10.1038/nrn2682PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Klump  KL, Bulik  CM, Pollice  C,  et al.  Temperament and character in women with anorexia nervosa.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 2000;188(9):559-567. doi:10.1097/00005053-200009000-00001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Strober  M, Freeman  R, Lampert  C, Diamond  J.  The association of anxiety disorders and obsessive compulsive personality disorder with anorexia nervosa: evidence from a family study with discussion of nosological and neurodevelopmental implications.  Int J Eat Disord. 2007;40(suppl):S46-S51. doi:10.1002/eat.20429PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Deep  AL, Nagy  LM, Weltzin  TE, Rao  R, Kaye  WH.  Premorbid onset of psychopathology in long-term recovered anorexia nervosa.  Int J Eat Disord. 1995;17(3):291-297.PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Bulik  CM, Sullivan  PF, Fear  JL, Pickering  A.  Outcome of anorexia nervosa: eating attitudes, personality, and parental bonding.  Int J Eat Disord. 2000;28(2):139-147. doi:10.1002/1098-108X(200009)28:2<139::AID-EAT2>3.0.CO;2-GPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Klump  KL, Strober  M, Bulik  CM,  et al.  Personality characteristics of women before and after recovery from an eating disorder.  Psychol Med. 2004;34(8):1407-1418. doi:10.1017/S0033291704002442PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Bailer  UF, Frank  GK, Price  JC,  et al.  Interaction between serotonin transporter and dopamine D2/D3 receptor radioligand measures is associated with harm avoidant symptoms in anorexia and bulimia nervosa.  Psychiatry Res. 2013;211(2):160-168. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2012.06.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Bailer  UF, Price  JC, Meltzer  CC,  et al.  Dopaminergic activity and altered reward modulation in anorexia nervosa: insight from multimodal imaging.  Int J Eat Disord. 2017;50(5):593-596. doi:10.1002/eat.22638PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Bowman  ES, Coons  PM.  The differential diagnosis of epilepsy, pseudoseizures, dissociative identity disorder, and dissociative disorder not otherwise specified.  Bull Menninger Clin. 2000;64(2):164-180.PubMedGoogle Scholar
12.
Frank  GK, Bailer  UF, Henry  SE,  et al.  Increased dopamine D2/D3 receptor binding after recovery from anorexia nervosa measured by positron emission tomography and [11c]raclopride.  Biol Psychiatry. 2005;58(11):908-912. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.05.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Kaye  WH, Wierenga  CE, Bailer  UF, Simmons  AN, Bischoff-Grethe  A.  Nothing tastes as good as skinny feels: the neurobiology of anorexia nervosa.  Trends Neurosci. 2013;36(2):110-120. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2013.01.003PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Oinio  V, Bäckström  P, Uhari-Väänänen  J, Raasmaja  A, Piepponen  P, Kiianmaa  K.  Dopaminergic modulation of reward-guided decision making in alcohol-preferring AA rats.  Behav Brain Res. 2017;326:87-95. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2017.03.007PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Kelley  AE.  Ventral striatal control of appetitive motivation: role in ingestive behavior and reward-related learning.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2004;27(8):765-776. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2003.11.015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Monteleone  AM, Castellini  G, Volpe  U,  et al.  Neuroendocrinology and brain imaging of reward in eating disorders: a possible key to the treatment of anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2018;80(pt B):132-142. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.02.020PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Frank  GK.  The perfect storm: a bio-psycho-social risk model for developing and maintaining eating disorders.  Front Behav Neurosci. 2016;10:44. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00044PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Schultz  W.  Getting formal with dopamine and reward.  Neuron. 2002;36(2):241-263. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
O’Doherty  JP, Dayan  P, Friston  K, Critchley  H, Dolan  RJ.  Temporal difference models and reward-related learning in the human brain.  Neuron. 2003;38(2):329-337. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00169-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Cowdrey  FA, Park  RJ, Harmer  CJ, McCabe  C.  Increased neural processing of rewarding and aversive food stimuli in recovered anorexia nervosa.  Biol Psychiatry. 2011;70(8):736-743. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.05.028PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Frank  GK, Reynolds  JR, Shott  ME,  et al.  Anorexia nervosa and obesity are associated with opposite brain reward response.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37(9):2031-2046. doi:10.1038/npp.2012.51PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
DeGuzman  M, Shott  ME, Yang  TT, Riederer  J, Frank  GKW.  Association of elevated reward prediction error response with weight gain in adolescent anorexia nervosa.  Am J Psychiatry. 2017;174(6):557-565. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16060671PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Golden  NH.  Eating disorders in adolescence and their sequelae.  Best Pract Res Clin Obstet Gynaecol. 2003;17(1):57-73. doi:10.1053/ybeog.2003.0344PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Silverman  MH, Jedd  K, Luciana  M.  Neural networks involved in adolescent reward processing: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies.  Neuroimage. 2015;122:427-439. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.083PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Barkley-Levenson  E, Galván  A.  Neural representation of expected value in the adolescent brain.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111(4):1646-1651. doi:10.1073/pnas.1319762111PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Cohen  JR, Asarnow  RF, Sabb  FW,  et al.  A unique adolescent response to reward prediction errors.  Nat Neurosci. 2010;13(6):669-671. doi:10.1038/nn.2558PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Frank  GK, Shott  ME, Riederer  J, Pryor  TL.  Altered structural and effective connectivity in anorexia and bulimia nervosa in circuits that regulate energy and reward homeostasis.  Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(11):e932. doi:10.1038/tp.2016.199PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
Beck  KD, Luine  VN.  Food deprivation modulates chronic stress effects on object recognition in male rats: role of monoamines and amino acids.  Brain Res. 1999;830(1):56-71. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(99)01380-3PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Monteleone  P, Scognamiglio  P, Monteleone  AM,  et al.  Abnormal diurnal patterns of salivary α-amylase and cortisol secretion in acute patients with anorexia nervosa.  World J Biol Psychiatry. 2011;12(6):455-461. doi:10.3109/15622975.2011.590226PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Paszynska  E, Dmitrzak-Weglarz  M, Tyszkiewicz-Nwafor  M, Slopien  A.  Salivary alpha-amylase, secretory IgA and free cortisol as neurobiological components of the stress response in the acute phase of anorexia nervosa.  World J Biol Psychiatry. 2016;17(4):266-273. doi:10.3109/15622975.2016.1163419PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Oswald  LM, Wong  DF, McCaul  M,  et al.  Relationships among ventral striatal dopamine release, cortisol secretion, and subjective responses to amphetamine.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30(4):821-832. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1300667PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Pérez-Neri  I, Montes  S, Ojeda-López  C, Ramírez-Bermúdez  J, Ríos  C.  Modulation of neurotransmitter systems by dehydroepiandrosterone and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate: mechanism of action and relevance to psychiatric disorders.  Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2008;32(5):1118-1130. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2007.12.001PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Wise  RA, Morales  M.  A ventral tegmental CRF-glutamate-dopamine interaction in addiction.  Brain Res. 2010;1314:38-43. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.09.101PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Koob  G, Kreek  MJ.  Stress, dysregulation of drug reward pathways, and the transition to drug dependence.  Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164(8):1149-1159. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.05030503PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Therrien  F, Drapeau  V, Lupien  SJ,  et al.  Awakening cortisol response in relation to psychosocial profiles and eating behaviors.  Physiol Behav. 2008;93(1-2):282-288. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2007.08.019PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Lupis  SB, Sabik  NJ, Wolf  JM.  Role of shame and body esteem in cortisol stress responses.  J Behav Med. 2016;39(2):262-275. doi:10.1007/s10865-015-9695-5PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Garner  D.  Eating Disorder Inventory™-3 (EDI™-3). Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 2004.
38.
Torrubia  R, Avila  C, Molto  J, Caseras  X.  The Sensitivity to Punishment and Sensitivity to Reward Questionnaire (SPSRQ) as a measure of Gray’s anxiety and impulsivity dimensions.  Pers Individ Dif. 2001;31:837-862. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00183-5Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Spielberger  CD.  Manual for the State-Trate Anxiety Inventory. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc; 1983.
40.
Cloninger  CR, Przybeck  TR, Svrakic  DM, Wetzel  RD.  The Temperament and Character Inventory (TCI): A Guide to its Development and Use. St. Louis, MO: Center for Psychobiology of Personality, Washington University; 1994.
41.
Kovacs  M.  The Children’s Depression Inventory (CDI). Toronto, ON, Canada: Multi-Health Systems, Inc; 1992.
42.
Frank  GKW, Favaro  A, Marsh  R, Ehrlich  S, Lawson  EA.  Toward valid and reliable brain imaging results in eating disorders.  Int J Eat Disord. 2018;51(3):250-261. doi:10.1002/eat.22829PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Lock  J, Le Grange  D, Agras  WS, Moye  A, Bryson  SW, Jo  B.  Randomized clinical trial comparing family-based treatment with adolescent-focused individual therapy for adolescents with anorexia nervosa.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(10):1025-1032. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.128PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Sheehan  DV, Sheehan  KH, Shytle  RD,  et al.  Reliability and validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID).  J Clin Psychiatry. 2010;71(3):313-326. doi:10.4088/JCP.09m05305whiPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Frank  GK, Kaye  WH, Carter  CS,  et al.  The evaluation of brain activity in response to taste stimuli: a pilot study and method for central taste activation as assessed by event-related fMRI.  J Neurosci Methods. 2003;131(1-2):99-105. doi:10.1016/S0165-0270(03)00240-1PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
O’Doherty  J, Dayan  P, Schultz  J, Deichmann  R, Friston  K, Dolan  RJ.  Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning.  Science. 2004;304(5669):452-454. doi:10.1126/science.1094285PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Breiter  HC, Gollub  RL, Weisskoff  RM,  et al.  Acute effects of cocaine on human brain activity and emotion.  Neuron. 1997;19(3):591-611. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80374-8PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Tzourio-Mazoyer  N, Landeau  B, Papathanassiou  D,  et al.  Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain.  Neuroimage. 2002;15(1):273-289. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0978PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Ramsey  JD, Hanson  SJ, Hanson  C, Halchenko  YO, Poldrack  RA, Glymour  C.  Six problems for causal inference from fMRI.  Neuroimage. 2010;49(2):1545-1558. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.065PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Westfall  PH.  On using the bootstrap for multiple comparisons.  J Biopharm Stat. 2011;21(6):1187-1205. doi:10.1080/10543406.2011.607751PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Branch  SY, Goertz  RB, Sharpe  AL,  et al.  Food restriction increases glutamate receptor-mediated burst firing of dopamine neurons.  J Neurosci. 2013;33(34):13861-13872. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5099-12.2013PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Carr  KD, Tsimberg  Y, Berman  Y, Yamamoto  N.  Evidence of increased dopamine receptor signaling in food-restricted rats.  Neuroscience. 2003;119(4):1157-1167. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00227-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Thanos  PK, Michaelides  M, Piyis  YK, Wang  GJ, Volkow  ND.  Food restriction markedly increases dopamine D2 receptor (D2R) in a rat model of obesity as assessed with in-vivo muPET imaging ([11C] raclopride) and in-vitro ([3H] spiperone) autoradiography.  Synapse. 2008;62(1):50-61. doi:10.1002/syn.20468PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Frank  GK, Collier  S, Shott  ME, O’Reilly  RC.  Prediction error and somatosensory insula activation in women recovered from anorexia nervosa.  J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2016;41(5):304-311. doi:10.1503/jpn.150103PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Stratford  TR, Kelley  AE.  Evidence of a functional relationship between the nucleus accumbens shell and lateral hypothalamus subserving the control of feeding behavior.  J Neurosci. 1999;19(24):11040-11048. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-11040.1999PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Castro  DC, Cole  SL, Berridge  KC.  Lateral hypothalamus, nucleus accumbens, and ventral pallidum roles in eating and hunger: interactions between homeostatic and reward circuitry.  Front Syst Neurosci. 2015;9:90. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2015.00090PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Izquierdo  A, Jentsch  JD.  Reversal learning as a measure of impulsive and compulsive behavior in addictions.  Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2012;219(2):607-620. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2579-7PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Kringelbach  ML, Rolls  ET.  The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology.  Prog Neurobiol. 2004;72(5):341-372. doi:10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.03.006PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Small  DM, Bender  G, Veldhuizen  MG, Rudenga  K, Nachtigal  D, Felsted  J.  The role of the human orbitofrontal cortex in taste and flavor processing.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2007;1121:136-151. doi:10.1196/annals.1401.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Small  DM, Jones-Gotman  M, Dagher  A.  Feeding-induced dopamine release in dorsal striatum correlates with meal pleasantness ratings in healthy human volunteers.  Neuroimage. 2003;19(4):1709-1715. doi:10.1016/S1053-8119(03)00253-2PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Ahn  S, Phillips  AG.  Modulation by central and basolateral amygdalar nuclei of dopaminergic correlates of feeding to satiety in the rat nucleus accumbens and medial prefrontal cortex.  J Neurosci. 2002;22(24):10958-10965. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-24-10958.2002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Hajnal  A, Norgren  R.  Accumbens dopamine mechanisms in sucrose intake.  Brain Res. 2001;904(1):76-84. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(01)02451-9PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
63.
Lawson  EA, Holsen  LM, Desanti  R,  et al.  Increased hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal drive is associated with decreased appetite and hypoactivation of food-motivation neurocircuitry in anorexia nervosa.  Eur J Endocrinol. 2013;169(5):639-647. doi:10.1530/EJE-13-0433PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Robinson  OJ, Overstreet  C, Charney  DR, Vytal  K, Grillon  C.  Stress increases aversive prediction error signal in the ventral striatum.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(10):4129-4133. doi:10.1073/pnas.1213923110PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Anderson  BA, Kuwabara  H, Wong  DF,  et al.  Linking dopaminergic reward signals to the development of attentional bias: a positron emission tomographic study.  Neuroimage. 2017;157:27-33. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.05.062PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×