[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  Goal-Directed Planning Task
Goal-Directed Planning Task

Goal-directed (model-based) planning was assessed using a 2-step decision-making task.42 In each trial, individuals were asked to select between 1 of 2 choices (top). On the basis of the depicted probabilities (70% or 30%) for each of these options, individuals would transition to a second stage, where they were again asked to choose between 2 options. These choices were rewarded (or not rewarded) with a 1-cent coin based on the current probability of reward assigned to that fractal. In the example trial depicted here, the leftmost fractal had a 34% chance of producing a coin. This probability changed slowly throughout the task, encouraging individuals to update their action preferences and regularly explore new options.

Figure 2.  Factor Analysis of 3 Transdiagnostic Dimensions: Compulsivity, Obsessionality, and General Distress
Factor Analysis of 3 Transdiagnostic Dimensions: Compulsivity, Obsessionality, and General Distress

Each bar represents the loadings for each subscale onto the 3 factors (distress, compulsivity, and obsessionality). The height of each bar reflects its loading onto the relevant factor. Color codes indicate the questionnaire from which each subscale was drawn. DASS indicates Depression and Anxiety and Stress Scale; MCQ, Metacognitive Beliefs Questionnaire; OCI-R, Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory–Revised; SDS, Sheehan Disability Scale.

Figure 3.  Association Among Model-Based Planning Scores, Diagnostic Status, and Compulsivity
Association Among Model-Based Planning Scores, Diagnostic Status, and Compulsivity

A, Bars display mean model-based planning scores (after controlling for age) by group, and dots indicate individual participant’s performance. No significant association was found for obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) (P = .18) or generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) diagnosis (P = .71) with model-based planning. The coefficients are from a regression model, specifically the interaction between reward and transition on stay behavior in the 2-step task. Scores below 0 were possible but rare (5 of 285 scores were below 0); scores close to 0 indicated a poor fit of the model to behavior. B, Scatterplot depicting the association between scores on the transdiagnostic compulsivity dimension and model-based planning ability, controlling for age. A significant negative association was found (P = .003). Individuals who had the highest self-reported compulsivity had the lowest scores on the test of model-based planning. Colors indicate the diagnoses for which each participant met the criteria (OCD, GAD, or combined OCD and GAD). C, Results are from a regression analysis comparing OCD diagnosis with the dimensional compulsivity factor in the same analysis. The association of OCD with model-based planning approached 0 when compulsivity was included in the same model (P = .91), whereas the association with compulsivity remained strong (P = .007). Error bars indicate SEs.

Table 1.  Demographics, Clinical Characteristics, and Cognitive Task Performance by Diagnosisa
Demographics, Clinical Characteristics, and Cognitive Task Performance by Diagnosisa
Table 2.  Cognitive Test Performance and Dimensions
Cognitive Test Performance and Dimensions
1.
Association  AP.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
2.
World Health Organization.  The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1992.
3.
Hyman  SE.  Can neuroscience be integrated into the DSM-V?  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(9):725-732. doi:10.1038/nrn2218PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Hyman  SE.  The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification.  Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:155-179. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091532PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Gillan  CM, Fineberg  NA, Robbins  TW.  A trans-diagnostic perspective on obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Psychol Med. 2017;47(9):1528-1548. doi:10.1017/S0033291716002786PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Sanislow  CA, Pine  DS, Quinn  KJ,  et al.  Developing constructs for psychopathology research: research domain criteria.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119(4):631-639. doi:10.1037/a0020909PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Fried  EI, Nesse  RM.  Depression is not a consistent syndrome: an investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study.  J Affect Disord. 2015;172:96-102. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Kessler  RC, Chiu  WT, Demler  O, Merikangas  KR, Walters  EE.  Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617-627. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Wittchen  HU, Zhao  S, Kessler  RC, Eaton  WW.  DSM-III-R generalized anxiety disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(5):355-364. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950050015002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Kessler  RC, Berglund  P, Demler  O,  et al; National Comorbidity Survey Replication.  The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R).  JAMA. 2003;289(23):3095-3105. doi:10.1001/jama.289.23.3095PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
De Hert  M, Cohen  D, Bobes  J,  et al.  Physical illness in patients with severe mental disorders, II: barriers to care, monitoring and treatment guidelines, plus recommendations at the system and individual level.  World Psychiatry. 2011;10(2):138-151. doi:10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00036.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Carr  CP, Martins  CM, Stingel  AM, Lemgruber  VB, Juruena  MF.  The role of early life stress in adult psychiatric disorders: a systematic review according to childhood trauma subtypes.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013;201(12):1007-1020. doi:10.1097/NMD.0000000000000049PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Cuthbert  BN, Kozak  MJ.  Constructing constructs for psychopathology: the NIMH research domain criteria.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122(3):928-937. doi:10.1037/a0034028PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Patzelt  EH, Kool  W, Millner  AJ, Gershman  SJ.  Incentives boost model-based control across a range of severity on several psychiatric constructs.  Biol Psychiatry. 2019;85(5):425-433. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.06.018PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Rouault  M, Seow  T, Gillan  CM, Fleming  SM.  Psychiatric symptom dimensions are associated with dissociable shifts in metacognition but not task performance.  Biol Psychiatry. 2018;84(6):443-451. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.12.017PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Hunter  LE, Meer  EA, Gillan  CM, Hsu  M, Daw  ND.  Excessive deliberation in social anxiety.  Preprint. Published online January 17, 2019. bioRxiv. doi:10.1101/522433v1
17.
Gillan  CM, Kosinski  M, Whelan  R, Phelps  EA, Daw  ND.  Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control.  Elife. 2016;5:5. doi:10.7554/eLife.11305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Dickinson  A.  Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy.  Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1985;308(1135):67-78. doi:10.1098/rstb.1985.0010Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Dickinson  A, Balleine  B.  Motivational control of goal-directed action.  Anim Learn Behav. 1994;22(1):1-18. Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Gillan  CM, Otto  AR, Phelps  EA, Daw  ND.  Model-based learning protects against forming habits.  Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2015;15(3):523-536. doi:10.3758/s13415-015-0347-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Gillan  CM, Robbins  TW.  Goal-directed learning and obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1655):20130475. PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Gillan  CM, Papmeyer  M, Morein-Zamir  S,  et al.  Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(7):718-726. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Gillan  CM, Morein-Zamir  S, Kaser  M,  et al.  Counterfactual processing of economic action-outcome alternatives in obsessive-compulsive disorder: further evidence of impaired goal-directed behavior.  Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(8):639-646. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.018PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Gillan  CM, Morein-Zamir  S, Urcelay  GP,  et al.  Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(8):631-638. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Gillan  CM, Apergis-Schoute  AM, Morein-Zamir  S,  et al.  Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(3):284-293. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040525PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Graybiel  AM, Rauch  SL.  Toward a neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Neuron. 2000;28(2):343-347. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00113-6PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Everitt  BJ, Robbins  TW.  Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion.  Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1481-1489. doi:10.1038/nn1579PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Ersche  KD, Gillan  CM, Jones  PS,  et al.  Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction.  Science. 2016;352(6292):1468-1471. doi:10.1126/science.aaf3700PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Voon  V, Derbyshire  K, Rück  C,  et al.  Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits.  Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(3):345-352. doi:10.1038/mp.2014.44PubMedGoogle Scholar
30.
Daw  ND, Gershman  SJ, Seymour  B, Dayan  P, Dolan  RJ.  Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors.  Neuron. 2011;69(6):1204-1215. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Doll  BB, Duncan  KD, Simon  DA, Shohamy  D, Daw  ND.  Model-based choices involve prospective neural activity.  Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(5):767-772. doi:10.1038/nn.3981PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Doll  BB, Bath  KG, Daw  ND, Frank  MJ.  Variability in dopamine genes dissociates model-based and model-free reinforcement learning.  J Neurosci. 2016;36(4):1211-1222. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1901-15.2016PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Wunderlich  K, Smittenaar  P, Dolan  RJ.  Dopamine enhances model-based over model-free choice behavior.  Neuron. 2012;75(3):418-424. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.042PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Morris  RW, Quail  S, Griffiths  KR, Green  MJ, Balleine  BW.  Corticostriatal control of goal-directed action is impaired in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(2):187-195. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.005PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Alvares  GA, Balleine  BW, Guastella  AJ.  Impairments in goal-directed actions predict treatment response to cognitive-behavioral therapy in social anxiety disorder.  PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e94778. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094778PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
Alvares  GA, Balleine  BW, Whittle  L, Guastella  AJ.  Reduced goal-directed action control in autism spectrum disorder.  Autism Res. 2016;9(12):1285-1293. doi:10.1002/aur.1613PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Henry  JD.  A meta-analytic review of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and verbal fluency performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2006;11(2):156-176. doi:10.1080/13546800444000227PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Shin  NY, Lee  TY, Kim  E, Kwon  JS.  Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis.  Psychol Med. 2014;44(6):1121-1130. doi:10.1017/S0033291713001803PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Abramovitch  A, Anholt  G, Raveh-Gottfried  S, Hamo  N, Abramowitz  JS.  Meta-analysis of intelligence quotient (IQ) in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28(1):111-120. doi:10.1007/s11065-017-9358-0PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Prabhakaran  V, Smith  JA, Desmond  JE, Glover  GH, Gabrieli  JD.  Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test.  Cogn Psychol. 1997;33(1):43-63. doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0659PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Yuan  P, Raz  N.  Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:180-192. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005PubMedGoogle Scholar
42.
Daw  ND, Niv  Y, Dayan  P.  Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control.  Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(12):1704-1711. doi:10.1038/nn1560PubMedGoogle Scholar
43.
Nelson  HE.  A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects.  Cortex. 1976;12(4):313-324. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(76)80035-4PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Raven  J.  The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time.  Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):1-48. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0735PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Foa  EB, Huppert  JD, Leiberg  S,  et al.  The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version.  Psychol Assess. 2002;14(4):485-496. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.485PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Wells  A, Cartwright-Hatton  S.  A short form of the metacognitions questionnaire: properties of the MCQ-30.  Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(4):385-396. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00147-5PubMedGoogle Scholar
47.
Lovibond  SH, Lovibond  PF.  Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. 2nd ed. Sydney, Australia: Psychology Foundation; 1995.
48.
Sheehan  DV, Lecrubier  Y, Sheehan  KH,  et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10.  J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(suppl 20):22-33.PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
US Census Bureau.  2013-2017 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates. Atlanta, GA: US Census Bureau; 2017.
50.
Schwabe  L, Wolf  OT.  Stress prompts habit behavior in humans.  J Neurosci. 2009;29(22):7191-7198. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0979-09.2009PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Schwabe  L, Wolf  OT.  Socially evaluated cold pressor stress after instrumental learning favors habits over goal-directed action.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(7):977-986. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.010PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Park  H, Lee  D, Chey  J.  Stress enhances model-free reinforcement learning only after negative outcome.  PLoS One. 2017;12(7):e0180588. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0180588PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Otto  AR, Raio  CM, Chiang  A, Phelps  EA, Daw  ND.  Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(52):20941-20946. doi:10.1073/pnas.1312011110PubMedGoogle Scholar
54.
Radenbach  C, Reiter  AM, Engert  V,  et al.  The interaction of acute and chronic stress impairs model-based behavioral control.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;53:268-280. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.12.017PubMedGoogle Scholar
55.
Heller  AS, Ezie  CEC, Otto  AR, Timpano  KR.  Model-based learning and individual differences in depression: The moderating role of stress.  Behav Res Ther. 2018;111:19-26. PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Gillan  C, Vaghi  M, Hezemans  F,  et al Experimentally-induced and real-world acute anxiety have no effect on goal-directed behaviour. Preprint. Published online April 12, 2019. bioRxiv. doi:10.1101/606145v1.full
57.
Grisanzio  KA, Goldstein-Piekarski  AN, Wang  MY, Rashed Ahmed  AP, Samara  Z, Williams  LM.  Transdiagnostic symptom clusters and associations with brain, behavior, and daily function in mood, anxiety, and trauma disorders.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(2):201-209. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3951PubMedGoogle Scholar
58.
Drysdale  AT, Grosenick  L, Downar  J,  et al.  Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression.  Nat Med. 2017;23(1):28-38. doi:10.1038/nm.4246PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Dinga  R, Schmaal  L, Penninx  BWJH,  et al.  Evaluating the evidence for biotypes of depression: methodological replication and extension of.  Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101796. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101796PubMedGoogle Scholar
60.
Dolan  RJ, Dayan  P.  Goals and habits in the brain.  Neuron. 2013;80(2):312-325. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.007PubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Fineberg  NA, Chamberlain  SR, Goudriaan  AE,  et al.  New developments in human neurocognition: clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity.  CNS Spectr. 2014;19(1):69-89. doi:10.1017/S1092852913000801PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Gillan  CM, Daw  ND.  Taking Psychiatry Research Online.  Neuron. 2016;91(1):19-23. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.002PubMedGoogle Scholar
63.
Chekroud  AM, Zotti  RJ, Shehzad  Z,  et al.  Cross-trial prediction of treatment outcome in depression: a machine learning approach.  Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(3):243-250. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00471-XPubMedGoogle Scholar
64.
Gillan  C, Whelan  R.  What big data can do for treatment in psychiatry.  Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2017;18:34-42. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.003Google Scholar
65.
Muskens  EM, Lucassen  P, Groenleer  W, van Weel  C, Oude Voshaar  R, Speckens  A.  Psychiatric diagnosis by telephone: is it an opportunity?  Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(10):1677-1689. doi:10.1007/s00127-014-0861-9PubMedGoogle Scholar
Original Investigation
October 9, 2019

Comparison of the Association Between Goal-Directed Planning and Self-reported Compulsivity vs Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder Diagnosis

Author Affiliations
  • 1School of Psychology, Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience and Global Brain Health Institute, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland
  • 2Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Mount Scopus, Israel
  • 3Department of Psychology, New York University, New York
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Massachusetts General Hospital, Boston
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, Columbia Irving University Medical Center, New York, New York
  • 7New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York
  • 8Department of Psychiatry, Harvard Medical School, McLean Hospital, Belmont, Massachusetts
  • 9Department of Psychiatry, Stony Brook University, Stony Brook, New York
  • 10VISN 2 Mental Illness Research Education and Clinical Centers, New York, New York
  • 11James J. Peters Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Bronx, New York
  • 12Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York
  • 13Warren Alpert Medical School of Brown University, Providence, Rhode Island
  • 14Butler Hospital, Providence, Rhode Island
  • 15Department of Psychology, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts
  • 16Department of Psychiatry, Hofstra Northwell School of Medicine, Hempstead, New York
  • 17Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois
  • 18Princeton Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychology, Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey
JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77(1):77-85. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2998
Key Points

Question  Are deficits in goal-directed planning better identified by self-reported compulsivity or a diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder?

Findings  In this cross-sectional study of 285 patients diagnosed with obsessive-compulsive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, or both, self-reported compulsivity was more strongly associated with goal-directed deficits than a diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder compared with generalized anxiety disorder.

Meaning  This study suggests that transdiagnostic compulsivity symptoms may have greater biological validity than a diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder.

Abstract

Importance  Dimensional definitions of transdiagnostic mental health problems have been suggested as an alternative to categorical diagnoses, having the advantage of capturing heterogeneity within diagnostic categories and similarity across them and bridging more naturally psychological and neural substrates.

Objective  To examine whether a self-reported compulsivity dimension has a stronger association with goal-directed and related higher-order cognitive deficits compared with a diagnosis of obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD).

Design, Setting, and Participants  In this cross-sectional study, patients with OCD and/or generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) from across the United States completed a telephone-based diagnostic interview by a trained rater, internet-based cognitive testing, and self-reported clinical assessments from October 8, 2015, to October 1, 2017. Follow-up data were collected to test for replicability.

Main Outcomes and Measures  Performance was measured on a test of goal-directed planning and cognitive flexibility (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test [WCST]) and a test of abstract reasoning. Clinical variables included DSM-5 diagnosis of OCD and GAD and 3 psychiatric symptom dimensions (general distress, compulsivity, and obsessionality) derived from a factor analysis.

Results  Of 285 individuals in the analysis (mean [SD] age, 32 [12] years; age range, 18-77 years; 219 [76.8%] female), 111 had OCD; 82, GAD; and 92, OCD and GAD. A diagnosis of OCD was not associated with goal-directed performance compared with GAD at baseline (β [SE], −0.02 [0.02]; P = .18). In contrast, a compulsivity dimension was negatively associated with goal-directed performance (β [SE], −0.05 [0.02]; P = .003). Results for abstract reasoning task and WCST mirrored this pattern; the compulsivity dimension was associated with abstract reasoning (β [SE], 2.99 [0.63]; P < .001) and several indicators of WCST performance (eg, categories completed: β [SE], −0.57 [0.09]; P < .001), whereas OCD diagnosis was not (abstract reasoning: β [SE], 0.39 [0.66]; P = .56; categories completed: β [SE], −0.09 [0.10]; P = .38). Other symptom dimensions relevant to OCD, obsessionality, and general distress had no reliable association with goal-directed performance, WCST, or abstract reasoning. Obsessionality had a positive association with requiring more trials to reach the first category on the WCST at baseline (β [SE], 2.92 [1.39]; P = .04), and general distress was associated with impaired goal-directed performance at baseline (β [SE],−0.04 [0.02]; P = .01). However, unlike the key results of this study, neither survived correction for multiple comparisons or was replicated at follow-up testing.

Conclusions and Relevance  Deficits in goal-directed planning in OCD may be more strongly associated with a compulsivity dimension than with OCD diagnosis. This result may have implications for research assessing the association between brain mechanisms and clinical manifestations and for understanding the structure of mental illness.

Introduction

Fundamental issues with the use of DSM-51 and International Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders, 10th Revision2 disorder categories for neurobiological research are increasingly recognized.3-6 Diagnostic groups are highly heterogeneous; patients often have the same diagnosis with vastly different symptom profiles.7 Moreover, comorbidity is the rule rather than the exception.8 Individuals with no psychiatric diagnosis are usually the control group, even though they differ from patients with a diagnosis in many ways beyond the diagnosis under investigation, including anxiety,9 depression,10 physical illness,11 and early-life adversity.12 As a result, potential biomarkers, intermediate phenotypes, or etiologic substrates can at best show a modest association with a categorical clinical phenotype, and this association is unlikely to be specific to that phenotype.

Whether there is an alternative way to conceptualize psychiatric ill health that might provide a closer and more specific fit to underlying biological states remains unknown. A long-standing suggestion has been to dispense with categories and instead study graded clinical phenotypes (dimensions) that manifest transdiagnostically.13 Although initial results have been promising,14-17 whether a dimensional framework for understanding psychiatric states provides a better match to brain-based measures than the extant categorical one remains an open question.

We tested this question with respect to goal-directed control, a cognitive capacity that protects against forming maladaptive habits18-20 and has been suggested to underlie compulsivity in obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD)21-26 and other compulsive disorders, such as addiction27,28 and binge-eating disorder.29 Goal-directed control refers to our ability to make prospective decisions, to simulate alternative futures, and to make decisions that align with our current needs and wants.30 It has well-defined neural substrates25,30,31 and pharmacologic correlates32,33 and has been computationally formalized30 and studied across species.27 Like almost all biomarkers in psychiatry, issues of specificity have emerged, with other disorders showing impairment in goal-directed control.34-36 Recent evidence from a large internet-based general population study17 found that a transdiagnostic dimension relating to compulsive behavior and intrusive thought might explain this pattern of nonspecific results. However, it is currently unknown whether these results from the general population apply to patients and, more importantly, how these self-reported dimensions compare with DSM-5 categories.

The current study investigated self-reported dimensions vs disorder diagnoses using an internet-based, dimensional method to assess patients in whom diagnoses were established using a structured clinical telephone interview. Given the confounders associated with using a healthy control group with no diagnosis (eg, comorbid anxiety, depression, and life stress), we recruited individuals with generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) to serve as a patient control group. We selected this disorder because the clinical presentation of GAD does not involve compulsive behavior and individual differences in trait anxiety have not been linked to goal-directed planning deficits.14,17 However, GAD shares a pattern of excessive worry and intrusive thoughts with OCD as well as nonspecific clinical features, such as general distress and impairment.1 The use of patients with GAD allowed us to assess the potential contribution of these features to goal-directed deficits in OCD, separating compulsivity from obsessionality and general distress.

We hypothesized that goal-directed deficits in patients with diagnoses of OCD, GAD, or both would be specifically associated with a compulsivity dimension that manifests transdiagnostically and that this would outperform OCD diagnosis in its association with goal-directed deficits. We also probed the generality of our findings to other, related, aspects of higher-order cognition, which have been consistently linked to OCD diagnosis but not previously studied in the context of compulsivity. We investigated cognitive flexibility37,38 and abstract reasoning39 as a first step because, like goal-directed planning, these are executive functions that rely on the integrity of the prefrontal cortex and striatum40,41; thus, we reasoned that they represent good candidate characteristics of a compulsivity dimension.

Methods
Participants

In this cross-sectional study, 285 participants were recruited from across the United States using internet-based advertising. A total of 1136 individuals expressed an interest, of whom 394 met the inclusion criteria (likely diagnosis of OCD and/or GAD, English language, and no history of stroke, neurologic problems, or head injury) and responded to a scheduling email. These individuals were interviewed over the telephone using an electronic version of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview, version 7.0 for DSM-5. Patients completed all study components remotely from October 8, 2015, to October 1, 2017. A total of 335 individuals met the criteria for OCD or GAD, 43 did not, and an additional 16 withdrew. Of the 335 invited to participate, 285 completed the study. Participants were paid $20 plus a small bonus (<$3). Additional details of the recruitment and screening can be found in the eMethods in the Supplement. Ethical approval was obtained from the New York University Committee on Activities Involving Human Subjects, in addition to institutional review board approvals from the New York State Psychiatric Institute, Care New England–Butler Hospital, Massachusetts General Hospital Partners Human Research Committee, and Northwell Health. All participants provided electronic informed consent. Data were stored in a pseudo-anonymized format.

Goal-Directed Control

Participants completed a 2-step decision-making task that allowed us to derive individual estimates of model-based planning42 (Figure 1). Model-based planning is considered as a formalization of goal-directed control, such that choices are made prospectively and influenced by known environmental contingencies and the current desirability of rewards. Specifically, model-based planning reflects the extent to which individuals use their knowledge of the transition structure of the task to make choices. For example, if a first-stage choice is followed by a rare (30%) transition and individuals ultimately get a reward at stage 2, they should be less likely to repeat that choice on the next trial because the alternative choice has a higher probability (ie, common, 70%) of returning them to that valuable second-stage state. To measure goal-directed (model-based) control, we used a regression-based procedure17 documented in the eMethods in the Supplement. This procedure was complemented with computational modeling (eMethods in the Supplement).

Other Cognitive Tests

Cognitive flexibility was assessed using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST).43 Abstract reasoning ability was assessed using a task17 based on Raven’s Progressive Matrices44 (more information is available in the eMethods in the Supplement).

Self-report Symptoms

Participants completed the Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory–Revised (OCI-R),45 which has 6 subscales (washing, checking, neutralizing, counting, hoarding, and obsessing); the Metacognitive Beliefs Questionnaire (MCQ46), which has 5 subscales (cognitive confidence, positive beliefs about worry, cognitive self-consciousness, negative beliefs about uncontrollability and danger, and need to control thoughts); the Depression and Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS47), which has 3 subscales (depression, anxiety, and stress); and the Sheehan Disability Scale (SDS48), which assesses functional impairment arising from one’s disorder.

Follow-up Testing

To assess reproducibility, we invited all individuals to participate again, and data were collected from 110 participants. Procedures were identical to testing session 1 except that we did not conduct the telephone interview again. Data were collected in a single wave with a mean (SD) of 413 (144) days since the initial assessment (range, 42-685 days).

Statistical Analysis

Cognitive test performance was analyzed to produce individual participant’s scores (eMethods in the Supplement), which were brought forward to secondary analyses to test the association with clinical variables. The association of OCD diagnosis (independent variable) with cognition was determined using regression analysis in R, version 3.5.2 (lme4 package; R Foundation for Statistical Computing). Results from this analysis correspond to OCD and OCD plus GAD vs GAD. Likewise, analysis of the association of GAD with cognition corresponds to GAD and OCD plus GAD vs OCD. The analysis for dimensions was identical except that the independent variables were continuous. Because of the overlapping set of variables, we reported results from separate models for each psychiatric variable (ie, not controlling for one another). When significant associations were revealed for more than 1 variable, we addressed comparative questions using combined analyses to disambiguate the variable influencing the association. Independent variables in all analyses were zero centered. Significance was assessed using a 2-tailed P<.05 significance threshold, but for analysis of the 6 cognitive measures, we also assessed whether findings remained after a more stringent Bonferroni correction that corresponded to an adjusted α = .008. Supplementary analyses are presented in the eResults in the Supplement, including additional controls for age, medication, and comorbidity.

Results
Diagnosis

Analysis was conducted for 285 individuals (mean [SD] age, 32 [12] years; age range, 18-77 years; 219 [76.8%] female), 111 with OCD, 82 with GAD, and 92 with OCD and GAD. The sample was racially and ethnically representative.49 That is, 211 (74.0%) were white, 34 (11.9%) black, 19 (6.7%) Asian, 4 (1.4%) American Indian or Alaskan Native, 1 (0.4%) Native Hawaiian or other Pacific Islander, and 16 (5.6%) nondisclosed. In terms of ethnicity, 238 (83.5%) were non-Hispanic, 33 (11.6%) Latino/Hispanic, and 14 (4.9%) nondisclosed. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the sample are given in Table 1. A total of 157 individuals were receiving treatment (55.1%), and the proportion did not differ as a function of diagnosis (Table 1). A diagnosis of OCD was associated with higher scores on the OCI-R and the MCQ but not the DASS or SDS. Conversely, a GAD diagnosis was associated with lower OCI-R scores and higher DASS scores but had no association with MCQ or SDS scores. Individuals with an OCD diagnosis were older (mean [SD] age, 36.6 [12.9] years) than those without (mean [SD] age, 31.2 [11.4] years) (β [SE], 2.48 [0.74]; P < .001). As in a previous study,17 age was associated with goal-directed planning, with younger persons performing better (β [SE], −0.05 [0.02], P = .002). Thus, age was controlled for in all analyses. No significant association was found between OCD (β [SE], −0.02 [0.02]; P = .18) and GAD diagnoses (β [SE], −0.01 [0.02]; P = .71) and model-based planning (Table 1). Secondary analyses revealed similar results for cognitive flexibility and abstract reasoning. OCD diagnosis was not significantly associated with any of these measures (categories complete: β [SE], −0.09 [0.10]; P = .38; nonperseverative errors: β [SE], 1.37 [1.07]; P = .20; trials to first category: β [SE], 2.23 [1.41]; P = .12; perseverative errors: β [SE], −0.15 [0.59]; P = .80) on the WCST and with abstract reasoning (β [SE], 0.39 [0.66]; P = .56).

Dimensions

To achieve a better separation of obsessions from compulsions, we conducted a factor analysis using the subscales of the OCI-R, DASS, and MCQ and the global disability score from the SDS (Figure 2). The result was 3 factors (dimensions) that constituted a slight but informative reformulation. The SDS loaded with anxiety, depression, and stress from the DASS, comprising a general distress factor. Most critically, the obsessions subscale from the OCI-R loaded strongly with all subscales from the MCQ to form an obsessionality factor, whereas the other 5 of the 6 subscales on the OCI-R loaded together on a separate compulsivity factor. Results for the original questionnaires are presented in eTable 1 through eTable 5 in the Supplement. Similar to OCD diagnosis, the compulsivity factor was associated with older age (r = 0.17; P = .005). Obsessionality was associated with a younger age (r = −0.14; P = .02), and general distress showed no association (r = −0.09; P = .12). Patients receiving medication had lower scores on the compulsivity dimension (β [SE], −0.28 [0.11]; P = .02). Ancillary analyses of the association of age and treatment with the results are presented in eFigure 1 and eFigure 2 in the Supplement.

The compulsivity factor was associated with model-based planning failures (Figure 3A), but the obsessionality factor was not (Figure 3B and Table 2). A significant association was found between the general distress factor and model-based planning (β [SE], −0.04 [0.02]; P = .01), but the association was not maintained after Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons, or after controlling for compulsivity in the uncorrected model (β [SE], −0.02 [0.02]; P = .19). Secondary results for other cognitive tests showed a similar pattern. In terms of cognitive flexibility (WCST), the compulsive factor was associated with fewer categories completed (β [SE], −0.57 [0.09]; P < .001) and more nonperseverative errors (β [SE], 5.86 [1.01]; P < .001) and trials to complete the first category (β [SE], 6.32 [1.36]; P < .001). Perseverative errors were not significantly associated with compulsivity (β [SE], 0.87 [0.59]; P = .14). In addition, no association was found between the obsessionality factor and WCST, with the exception of an increase in the number of trials to complete the first category (β [SE], 2.92 [1.39]; P = .04), but the association was not maintained after correction for multiple comparisons. The compulsivity factor was associated with deficits in abstract reasoning (β [SE], −2.99 [0.63]; P < .001), whereas general distress (β [SE], −1.02 [0.65]; P = .12) and obsessionality (β [SE], −0.42 [0.65]; P = .52) were not (Table 2).

Dimension vs Diagnosis

When the compulsivity factor and OCD diagnosis were present in the same model, the association between OCD diagnosis and model-based deficits approached zero (β [SE], −0.002 [0.02]; P = .91), whereas the association with compulsivity remained significant (β [SE], −0.05 [0.02]; P = .007) (Figure 3C). Thus, in a diagnosed patient population, scores on a self-reported compulsivity factor have more relevance to model-based deficits than diagnosis. Similar patterns were observed when diagnosis was compared with the compulsivity dimension for other cognitive measures. For cognitive flexibility, the number of categories completed (compulsivity: β [SE], −0.65 [0.10]; P<.001; OCD: β [SE], 0.18 [0.10]; P = .08), nonperseverative errors (compulsivity: β [SE], 6.37 [1.10]; P<.001; OCD: β [SE], −1.25 [1.11]; P = .26), and trials to first category (compulsivity: β [SE], 6.50 [1.49]; P<.001; OCD: β [SE], −0.45 [1.50]; P = .76) were each associated with the compulsivity factor and not OCD. Perseverative errors were not significantly associated with the compulsivity factor (β [SE], 1.12 [0.65]; P = .08) or OCD diagnosis (β [SE], −0.61 [0.65]; P = .35). Finally, abstract reasoning was associated with the compulsivity factor (β [SE], −3.79 [0.69]; P<.001), but OCD diagnosis was surprisingly associated with improved performance after controlling for compulsivity (β [SE], 1.95 [0.69]; P = .005). All results were maintained following Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons unless otherwise stated.

Replicability

We tested whether the associations between symptom dimensions and cognition could be replicated in a smaller set of individuals from whom we collected follow-up data (n = 110). We used the factor loadings defined at time 1 to define scores on the dimensions at time 2. We tested a priori hypotheses based on the results from testing session 1 and as such did not apply an additional correction for multiple comparisons. All associations between compulsivity and cognition were replicated: goal-directed control (β [SE], −0.04 [0.02]; P = .04), number of categories completed on the WCST (β [SE], −0.31 [0.14]; P = .02), nonperseverative errors (β [SE], 4.82 [1.51]; P = .002), trials to complete first category (β [SE], 5.95 [2.04]; P = .004), and abstract reasoning (β [SE], −3.60 [0.97]; P < .001). Consistent with the baseline testing session, there was no significant association with compulsivity and perseverative errors (β [SE], 0.92 [0.71]; P = .20). Obsessionality was associated with none of these cognitive assessments (model based: β [SE], −0.03 [0.02]; P = .16; number of categories: β [SE], −0.21 [0.14]; P = .13; perseverative errors: β [SE], 0.10 [0.72]; P = .89; nonperseverative errors: β [SE], 1.64 [1.57]; P = .30; trials to first category: β [SE], 3.31 [2.09]; P = .12; and abstract reasoning: β [SE], −1.62 [1.02]; P = .11). General distress was likewise not associated with model-based planning (β [SE], −0.02 [0.02]; P = .22), perseverative errors (β [SE], 0.70 [0.71]; P = .33), nonperseverative errors (β [SE], 1.73 [1.57]; P = .27), or trials to first category (β [SE], 2.34 [2.10]; P = .27) but was associated with completing fewer categories on the WCST (β [SE], −0.31 [0.14]; P = .02) and poorer performance at on the test of abstract reasoning (β [SE], −2.01 [1.01]; P = .05). These latter 2 results are considered unreliable, as they were not observed at time 1 (general distress and categories completed at time 1: β [SE], −0.11 [0.10]; P = .26; general distress and abstract reasoning at time 1: β [SE], −1.02 [0.65]; P = .12).

Discussion

In this internet-based study of cognition with 285 patients diagnosed with OCD, GAD, or both, we found that having an OCD diagnosis was not associated with a reduction in goal-directed control. In contrast, a significant and replicable association was observed between self-reported scores on a compulsivity dimension (which manifested transdiagnostically) and deficits in goal-directed planning. When OCD diagnosis and compulsivity were included in the same analysis, the effect of OCD on goal-directed planning approached 0. Other symptom dimensions were identified in this study, corresponding to obsessionality and general distress factors. These variables had no robust association with goal-directed deficits.

Goal-directed deficits in patients with OCD have been previously described compared with healthy volunteers.22-25 However, no prior study, to our knowledge, has compared patients with OCD with a psychiatric control group in which deficits would not be anticipated, such as GAD. We found that an OCD diagnosis may not best capture these deficits, in contrast to a compulsivity factor, which can be expressed transdiagnostically in the general population17 and in patients with mental health disorders. The finding that goal-directed deficits were specific to the compulsivity dimension (vs general distress) aligns with the recent suggestion that trait anxiety17 and major life stress17 have no association with planning failures. Although some studies50-52 have found that acute stressors impair goal-directed planning, others53-55 have failed to replicate this, and acute anxiety induction appears to have no association with goal-directed control.56 Beyond general distress, these data suggest that obsessionality is phenomenologically and neurobiologically distinct from compulsivity. Formalizing the distinction between these dimensions provides a new opportunity for research to more precisely characterize their distinct cognitive underpinnings in patients, in the general population, and across species.

These findings were not specific to model-based planning; we obtained similar findings in relation to cognitive flexibility assessed using the WCST43 and abstract reasoning using a task based on the Raven matrices.17 Performance of both tasks is reliably impaired in OCD,37,39 and abstract reasoning has been previously reported to correlate with model-based planning.17 Although OCD diagnostic status was not associated with any of the performance measures, compulsivity was associated with abstract reasoning and all but 1 measure of flexibility on the WCST. No reliable associations were found with obsessionality or general distress. These data corroborate the finding that the compulsivity dimension maps onto putatively underlying neurocognitive deficits more closely than diagnosis. It is a limitation that we did not include tests that were unrelated to compulsivity, but previous internet-based work has already found that compulsivity has a distinct pattern of cognitive impairment to anxious-depression15 and social withdrawal.16 A direction for future research may be to test the extent to which compulsivity, obsessionality, and general distress map dissociable cognitive processes in patients with diagnosed disorders.

This is not the first study to assess alternative formulations of psychiatric phenotypes. Examples of data-driven work include the identification of clusters that cut across disorders and show differential cognitive or neural correlates57 or biotypes within a depression sample based on resting state connectivity.58Although this approach has potential, absent of theory, the risk of overfitting is high.59 Our study adds to this literature by taking a theory-driven approach that focuses on goal-directed control, a neural process that has been examined across species,18,60 in the context of a rodent model of addiction,27 in OCD,21 and more recently in the context of a broader class of compulsive behaviors.61

With the exception of the diagnostic interview, all data were collected online. The ability to derive robust and replicable findings of clinical importance using this method has broad implications for increasing the scalability of research in psychiatry.62 Large samples are needed to move beyond cross-sectional, case-control designs and harness the complexity of individual experiences of mental health, particularly if these insights are to be leveraged into individual-participant level estimations.58,63,64 Internet-based studies assessing cognition and self-report symptoms are in most cases considerably faster and less expensive than in-person studies. As such, the demonstrated validity of this method paves the way for a new wave of online psychiatry research.

There are several implications of these findings for research and practice. If dimensions are more proximal to underlying biological states, the use of transdiagnostic dimensions might reduce noise in research studies that aim to identify biomarkers of phenotype or treatment response. If these results are confirmed and extended, these data suggest that a move toward dimensional stratification of patients in the clinic may be feasible and provide a pathway to enhanced care through individualized treatment assignment, for example.58 The replicability of the internet-based method for cognitive research in psychiatry makes large-scale psychiatry studies more achievable outside large centers and consortia, allowing for cheaper, faster, and better powered studies that incorporate replication as standard.

Limitations

This study has limitations. Diagnosis was determined using a telephone-based structured clinical interview. Few studies have assessed differences between telephone-based and in-person clinical interview, but results thus far are promising.65 Recruitment was entirely internet based; whether this might affect the generalizability of our findings to treatment-seeking populations is unclear. Our definition of compulsivity was narrow because we constrained our investigation to OCD- and GAD-relevant symptoms. Prior work17 in the general population found that a broader definition of compulsivity (including aspects of addiction and eating disorders) showed a stronger association with goal-directed planning than OCD severity, suggesting that our results might have been bolstered by a broader definition of compulsivity. The compulsivity dimension is not intended to be definitive but to test comparative questions about dimensions vs diagnosis in a mixed OCD and GAD sample. Working toward a definition, an ideal study would measure a broader range of symptoms and recruit a large all-comers sample with representation from multiple compulsive and noncompulsive disorders, healthy controls, and others.

Conclusions

The findings suggest that deficits in goal-directed planning in OCD may be more strongly associated with a compulsivity dimension than with OCD diagnosis. This result may have implications for basic research that aims to assess the association between brain mechanisms and clinical manifestations and for understanding the structure of mental illness.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: July 28, 2019.

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2019 Gillan CM et al. JAMA Psychiatry.

Published Online: October 9, 2019. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.2998

Correction: This article was corrected on November 20, 2019, to fix incorrect part labels and x-axis labels in Figure 2B and C.

Corresponding Author: Claire M. Gillan, PhD, School of Psychology, Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience and Global Brain Health Institute, Trinity College Dublin, Lloyd Building, Dublin 2, Ireland (gillancl@tcd.ie).

Author Contributions: Dr Gillan had full access to all the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Gillan, Evans, Phelps, Wilhelm, Daw, Simpson.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Gillan, Kalanthroff, Weingarden, Jacoby, Gershkovich, Snorrason, Campeas, Cervoni, Crimarco, Sokol, Garnaat, McLaughlin, Pinto, Boisseau, Wilhelm, Simpson.

Drafting of the manuscript: Gillan.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Gillan, Kalanthroff, Weingarden, Jacoby, Gershkovich, Snorrason, Campeas, Cervoni, Crimarco, Sokol, Garnaat, McLaughlin, Phelps, Pinto, Boisseau, Wilhelm, Daw, Simpson.

Statistical analysis: Gillan.

Obtained funding: Gillan.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Gillan, Kalanthroff, Evans, Jacoby, Gershkovich, Snorrason, Campeas, Cervoni, Crimarco, Pinto, Boisseau, Wilhelm, Daw, Simpson.

Supervision: Gillan, Phelps, Pinto, Wilhelm, Daw, Simpson.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Weingarden reported receiving grants from Telefónica Innovación Alpha SL and MGH Psychiatry Academy outside the submitted work. Dr Jacoby reported receiving personal fees from Massachusetts General Hospital Psychiatry Academy and Hogrefe Publishing outside the submitted work. Dr Garnaat reported receiving grants from Norman Prince Neurosciences Institute/Carney Institute for Brain Science outside the submitted work. Dr Wilhelm reported receiving grants from Oxford University Press, New Harbinger Publications, Guildford Publications, Springer Publications, One-Mind, Massachusetts General Hospital Psychiatry Academy, Elsevier, International OCD Foundation, National Institute of Mental Health, Tourette Association of America, and Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies and receiving grants from Telefonica Alpha Inc outside the submitted work. Dr Simpson reported receiving grants from Biohaven and Cambridge University Press, UpToDate Inc, and the JAMA Network outside the submitted work. No other disclosures were reported.

Funding/Support: This work was funded by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship (Dr Gillan). Dr Jacoby was funded by the Charles A. King Post-Doctoral Fellowship and the International OCD Foundation Young Investigator Award. Dr McLaughlin was funded by grant K23MH100607 from the National Institute of Mental Health.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funding sources had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: Maria Mancebo, PhD, Jennifer Barnes, Agustin Yip, MD, Arturo Sánchez-LaCay, MD, and Najate Ojeil, MA, LMHC, performed diagnostic interviews at their respective sites. They were not compensated for their work.

References
1.
Association  AP.  Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition: DSM-5. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2013. doi:10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596
2.
World Health Organization.  The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization; 1992.
3.
Hyman  SE.  Can neuroscience be integrated into the DSM-V?  Nat Rev Neurosci. 2007;8(9):725-732. doi:10.1038/nrn2218PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Hyman  SE.  The diagnosis of mental disorders: the problem of reification.  Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:155-179. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091532PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Gillan  CM, Fineberg  NA, Robbins  TW.  A trans-diagnostic perspective on obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Psychol Med. 2017;47(9):1528-1548. doi:10.1017/S0033291716002786PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Sanislow  CA, Pine  DS, Quinn  KJ,  et al.  Developing constructs for psychopathology research: research domain criteria.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2010;119(4):631-639. doi:10.1037/a0020909PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Fried  EI, Nesse  RM.  Depression is not a consistent syndrome: an investigation of unique symptom patterns in the STAR*D study.  J Affect Disord. 2015;172:96-102. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2014.10.010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
8.
Kessler  RC, Chiu  WT, Demler  O, Merikangas  KR, Walters  EE.  Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2005;62(6):617-627. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.617PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Wittchen  HU, Zhao  S, Kessler  RC, Eaton  WW.  DSM-III-R generalized anxiety disorder in the National Comorbidity Survey.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51(5):355-364. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1994.03950050015002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Kessler  RC, Berglund  P, Demler  O,  et al; National Comorbidity Survey Replication.  The epidemiology of major depressive disorder: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication (NCS-R).  JAMA. 2003;289(23):3095-3105. doi:10.1001/jama.289.23.3095PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
De Hert  M, Cohen  D, Bobes  J,  et al.  Physical illness in patients with severe mental disorders, II: barriers to care, monitoring and treatment guidelines, plus recommendations at the system and individual level.  World Psychiatry. 2011;10(2):138-151. doi:10.1002/j.2051-5545.2011.tb00036.xPubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Carr  CP, Martins  CM, Stingel  AM, Lemgruber  VB, Juruena  MF.  The role of early life stress in adult psychiatric disorders: a systematic review according to childhood trauma subtypes.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 2013;201(12):1007-1020. doi:10.1097/NMD.0000000000000049PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Cuthbert  BN, Kozak  MJ.  Constructing constructs for psychopathology: the NIMH research domain criteria.  J Abnorm Psychol. 2013;122(3):928-937. doi:10.1037/a0034028PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Patzelt  EH, Kool  W, Millner  AJ, Gershman  SJ.  Incentives boost model-based control across a range of severity on several psychiatric constructs.  Biol Psychiatry. 2019;85(5):425-433. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.06.018PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Rouault  M, Seow  T, Gillan  CM, Fleming  SM.  Psychiatric symptom dimensions are associated with dissociable shifts in metacognition but not task performance.  Biol Psychiatry. 2018;84(6):443-451. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2017.12.017PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Hunter  LE, Meer  EA, Gillan  CM, Hsu  M, Daw  ND.  Excessive deliberation in social anxiety.  Preprint. Published online January 17, 2019. bioRxiv. doi:10.1101/522433v1
17.
Gillan  CM, Kosinski  M, Whelan  R, Phelps  EA, Daw  ND.  Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control.  Elife. 2016;5:5. doi:10.7554/eLife.11305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Dickinson  A.  Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy.  Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1985;308(1135):67-78. doi:10.1098/rstb.1985.0010Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Dickinson  A, Balleine  B.  Motivational control of goal-directed action.  Anim Learn Behav. 1994;22(1):1-18. Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Gillan  CM, Otto  AR, Phelps  EA, Daw  ND.  Model-based learning protects against forming habits.  Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2015;15(3):523-536. doi:10.3758/s13415-015-0347-6PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Gillan  CM, Robbins  TW.  Goal-directed learning and obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2014;369(1655):20130475. PubMedGoogle Scholar
22.
Gillan  CM, Papmeyer  M, Morein-Zamir  S,  et al.  Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2011;168(7):718-726. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.10071062PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Gillan  CM, Morein-Zamir  S, Kaser  M,  et al.  Counterfactual processing of economic action-outcome alternatives in obsessive-compulsive disorder: further evidence of impaired goal-directed behavior.  Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(8):639-646. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.01.018PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Gillan  CM, Morein-Zamir  S, Urcelay  GP,  et al.  Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Biol Psychiatry. 2014;75(8):631-638. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.002PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Gillan  CM, Apergis-Schoute  AM, Morein-Zamir  S,  et al.  Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 2015;172(3):284-293. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040525PubMedGoogle Scholar
26.
Graybiel  AM, Rauch  SL.  Toward a neurobiology of obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Neuron. 2000;28(2):343-347. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00113-6PubMedGoogle Scholar
27.
Everitt  BJ, Robbins  TW.  Neural systems of reinforcement for drug addiction: from actions to habits to compulsion.  Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(11):1481-1489. doi:10.1038/nn1579PubMedGoogle Scholar
28.
Ersche  KD, Gillan  CM, Jones  PS,  et al.  Carrots and sticks fail to change behavior in cocaine addiction.  Science. 2016;352(6292):1468-1471. doi:10.1126/science.aaf3700PubMedGoogle Scholar
29.
Voon  V, Derbyshire  K, Rück  C,  et al.  Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits.  Mol Psychiatry. 2015;20(3):345-352. doi:10.1038/mp.2014.44PubMedGoogle Scholar
30.
Daw  ND, Gershman  SJ, Seymour  B, Dayan  P, Dolan  RJ.  Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors.  Neuron. 2011;69(6):1204-1215. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2011.02.027PubMedGoogle Scholar
31.
Doll  BB, Duncan  KD, Simon  DA, Shohamy  D, Daw  ND.  Model-based choices involve prospective neural activity.  Nat Neurosci. 2015;18(5):767-772. doi:10.1038/nn.3981PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Doll  BB, Bath  KG, Daw  ND, Frank  MJ.  Variability in dopamine genes dissociates model-based and model-free reinforcement learning.  J Neurosci. 2016;36(4):1211-1222. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1901-15.2016PubMedGoogle Scholar
33.
Wunderlich  K, Smittenaar  P, Dolan  RJ.  Dopamine enhances model-based over model-free choice behavior.  Neuron. 2012;75(3):418-424. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.042PubMedGoogle Scholar
34.
Morris  RW, Quail  S, Griffiths  KR, Green  MJ, Balleine  BW.  Corticostriatal control of goal-directed action is impaired in schizophrenia.  Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(2):187-195. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2014.06.005PubMedGoogle Scholar
35.
Alvares  GA, Balleine  BW, Guastella  AJ.  Impairments in goal-directed actions predict treatment response to cognitive-behavioral therapy in social anxiety disorder.  PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e94778. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0094778PubMedGoogle Scholar
36.
Alvares  GA, Balleine  BW, Whittle  L, Guastella  AJ.  Reduced goal-directed action control in autism spectrum disorder.  Autism Res. 2016;9(12):1285-1293. doi:10.1002/aur.1613PubMedGoogle Scholar
37.
Henry  JD.  A meta-analytic review of Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and verbal fluency performance in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2006;11(2):156-176. doi:10.1080/13546800444000227PubMedGoogle Scholar
38.
Shin  NY, Lee  TY, Kim  E, Kwon  JS.  Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: a meta-analysis.  Psychol Med. 2014;44(6):1121-1130. doi:10.1017/S0033291713001803PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Abramovitch  A, Anholt  G, Raveh-Gottfried  S, Hamo  N, Abramowitz  JS.  Meta-analysis of intelligence quotient (IQ) in obsessive-compulsive disorder.  Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28(1):111-120. doi:10.1007/s11065-017-9358-0PubMedGoogle Scholar
40.
Prabhakaran  V, Smith  JA, Desmond  JE, Glover  GH, Gabrieli  JD.  Neural substrates of fluid reasoning: an fMRI study of neocortical activation during performance of the Raven’s Progressive Matrices Test.  Cogn Psychol. 1997;33(1):43-63. doi:10.1006/cogp.1997.0659PubMedGoogle Scholar
41.
Yuan  P, Raz  N.  Prefrontal cortex and executive functions in healthy adults: a meta-analysis of structural neuroimaging studies.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2014;42:180-192. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.02.005PubMedGoogle Scholar
42.
Daw  ND, Niv  Y, Dayan  P.  Uncertainty-based competition between prefrontal and dorsolateral striatal systems for behavioral control.  Nat Neurosci. 2005;8(12):1704-1711. doi:10.1038/nn1560PubMedGoogle Scholar
43.
Nelson  HE.  A modified card sorting test sensitive to frontal lobe defects.  Cortex. 1976;12(4):313-324. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(76)80035-4PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Raven  J.  The Raven’s progressive matrices: change and stability over culture and time.  Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):1-48. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0735PubMedGoogle Scholar
45.
Foa  EB, Huppert  JD, Leiberg  S,  et al.  The Obsessive-Compulsive Inventory: development and validation of a short version.  Psychol Assess. 2002;14(4):485-496. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.485PubMedGoogle Scholar
46.
Wells  A, Cartwright-Hatton  S.  A short form of the metacognitions questionnaire: properties of the MCQ-30.  Behav Res Ther. 2004;42(4):385-396. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00147-5PubMedGoogle Scholar
47.
Lovibond  SH, Lovibond  PF.  Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. 2nd ed. Sydney, Australia: Psychology Foundation; 1995.
48.
Sheehan  DV, Lecrubier  Y, Sheehan  KH,  et al.  The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10.  J Clin Psychiatry. 1998;59(suppl 20):22-33.PubMedGoogle Scholar
49.
US Census Bureau.  2013-2017 American Community Survey 5-Year Estimates. Atlanta, GA: US Census Bureau; 2017.
50.
Schwabe  L, Wolf  OT.  Stress prompts habit behavior in humans.  J Neurosci. 2009;29(22):7191-7198. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0979-09.2009PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Schwabe  L, Wolf  OT.  Socially evaluated cold pressor stress after instrumental learning favors habits over goal-directed action.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2010;35(7):977-986. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2009.12.010PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Park  H, Lee  D, Chey  J.  Stress enhances model-free reinforcement learning only after negative outcome.  PLoS One. 2017;12(7):e0180588. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0180588PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Otto  AR, Raio  CM, Chiang  A, Phelps  EA, Daw  ND.  Working-memory capacity protects model-based learning from stress.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(52):20941-20946. doi:10.1073/pnas.1312011110PubMedGoogle Scholar
54.
Radenbach  C, Reiter  AM, Engert  V,  et al.  The interaction of acute and chronic stress impairs model-based behavioral control.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2015;53:268-280. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.12.017PubMedGoogle Scholar
55.
Heller  AS, Ezie  CEC, Otto  AR, Timpano  KR.  Model-based learning and individual differences in depression: The moderating role of stress.  Behav Res Ther. 2018;111:19-26. PubMedGoogle Scholar
56.
Gillan  C, Vaghi  M, Hezemans  F,  et al Experimentally-induced and real-world acute anxiety have no effect on goal-directed behaviour. Preprint. Published online April 12, 2019. bioRxiv. doi:10.1101/606145v1.full
57.
Grisanzio  KA, Goldstein-Piekarski  AN, Wang  MY, Rashed Ahmed  AP, Samara  Z, Williams  LM.  Transdiagnostic symptom clusters and associations with brain, behavior, and daily function in mood, anxiety, and trauma disorders.  JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(2):201-209. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.3951PubMedGoogle Scholar
58.
Drysdale  AT, Grosenick  L, Downar  J,  et al.  Resting-state connectivity biomarkers define neurophysiological subtypes of depression.  Nat Med. 2017;23(1):28-38. doi:10.1038/nm.4246PubMedGoogle Scholar
59.
Dinga  R, Schmaal  L, Penninx  BWJH,  et al.  Evaluating the evidence for biotypes of depression: methodological replication and extension of.  Neuroimage Clin. 2019;22:101796. doi:10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101796PubMedGoogle Scholar
60.
Dolan  RJ, Dayan  P.  Goals and habits in the brain.  Neuron. 2013;80(2):312-325. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.09.007PubMedGoogle Scholar
61.
Fineberg  NA, Chamberlain  SR, Goudriaan  AE,  et al.  New developments in human neurocognition: clinical, genetic, and brain imaging correlates of impulsivity and compulsivity.  CNS Spectr. 2014;19(1):69-89. doi:10.1017/S1092852913000801PubMedGoogle Scholar
62.
Gillan  CM, Daw  ND.  Taking Psychiatry Research Online.  Neuron. 2016;91(1):19-23. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2016.06.002PubMedGoogle Scholar
63.
Chekroud  AM, Zotti  RJ, Shehzad  Z,  et al.  Cross-trial prediction of treatment outcome in depression: a machine learning approach.  Lancet Psychiatry. 2016;3(3):243-250. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(15)00471-XPubMedGoogle Scholar
64.
Gillan  C, Whelan  R.  What big data can do for treatment in psychiatry.  Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2017;18:34-42. doi:10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.07.003Google Scholar
65.
Muskens  EM, Lucassen  P, Groenleer  W, van Weel  C, Oude Voshaar  R, Speckens  A.  Psychiatric diagnosis by telephone: is it an opportunity?  Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2014;49(10):1677-1689. doi:10.1007/s00127-014-0861-9PubMedGoogle Scholar
×