[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1.  PRISMA Flow Diagram
PRISMA Flow Diagram

aInternalizing and externalizing categories were not mutually exclusive.

Figure 2.  Meta-analysis of the Correlation of Screen Time With Externalizing Problems
Meta-analysis of the Correlation of Screen Time With Externalizing Problems

Markers indicate estimates, with the size of the marker indicating weight; horizontal lines represent 95% CIs; diamonds represent pooled estimates, with the outer points indicating 95% CIs.

Figure 3.  Meta-analysis of the Correlation of Screen Time With Internalizing Problems
Meta-analysis of the Correlation of Screen Time With Internalizing Problems

Markers indicate estimates, with the size of the marker indicating weight; horizontal lines represent 95% CIs; diamonds represent pooled estimates, with the outer points indicating 95% CIs.

Table 1.  Moderator Analyses for the Correlation Between Screen Time and Behavior Problems
Moderator Analyses for the Correlation Between Screen Time and Behavior Problems
Table 2.  Meta-regression Analyses of the Association Between Screen Time and Externalizing and Internalizing Problems
Meta-regression Analyses of the Association Between Screen Time and Externalizing and Internalizing Problems
1.
Browne  D, Thompson  DA, Madigan  S.  Digital media use in children: clinical vs scientific responsibilities.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(2):111-112. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4559 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Odgers  CL, Jensen  MR.  Annual research review: adolescent mental health in the digital age: facts, fears, and future directions.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020;61(3):336-348. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13190 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Christakis  DA, Gilkerson  J, Richards  JA,  et al.  Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(6):554-558. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.61 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Christakis  DA.  Interactive media use at younger than the age of 2 years: time to rethink the American Academy of Pediatrics guideline?   JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(5):399-400. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.5081 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Linebarger  DL, Barr  R, Lapierre  MA, Piotrowski  JT.  Associations between parenting, media use, cumulative risk, and children’s executive functioning.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014;35(6):367-377. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000069 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Nikkelen  SW, Valkenburg  PM, Huizinga  M, Bushman  BJ.  Media use and ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis.   Dev Psychol. 2014;50(9):2228-2241. doi:10.1037/a0037318 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kaye  L, Orben  A, Ellis  D, Hunter  S, Houghton  S.  The conceptual and methodological mayhem of “screen time.”   Int J Env Res Public Health. 2020;17(10):3661. doi:10.3390/ijerph17103661 Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Blum-Ross  A, Livingstone  S. The trouble with “screen time” rules. In: Mascheroni G, Jorge A, Ponte C (Eds.). Digital Parenting: The Challenges for Families in the Digital Age. International Clearinghouse on Children, Youth, and the Media; 2018:179-187.
9.
Ferguson  CJ, Beresin  E.  Social science’s curious war with pop culture and how it was lost: the media violence debate and the risks it holds for social science.   Prev Med. 2017;99:69-76. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.02.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Elson  M, Ferguson  CJ, Gregerson  M,  et al  Do policy statements on media effects faithfully represent the science?   Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(1):12-25. doi:10.1177/2515245918811301 Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Cohn  LD, Becker  BJ.  How meta-analysis increases statistical power.   Psychol Methods. 2003;8(3):243-253. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.243 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
McBee  MT, Brand  RJ, Dixon  WE  Jr.  Challenging the link between early childhood television exposure and later attention problems: a multiverse approach.   Psychol Sci. 2021;32(4):496-518. doi:10.1177/0956797620971650 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Stiglic  N, Viner  RM.  Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: a systematic review of reviews.   BMJ Open. 2019;9(1):e023191. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
de Jong  E, Visscher  TLS, HiraSing  RA, Heymans  MW, Seidell  JC, Renders  CM.  Association between TV viewing, computer use and overweight, determinants and competing activities of screen time in 4- to 13-year-old children.   Int J Obes (Lond). 2013;37(1):47-53. doi:10.1038/ijo.2011.244 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Rideout  V, Hamel  E.  The Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers, Preschoolers and Their Parents. Kaiser Family Foundation;2006.
16.
Neville  RD, McArthur  BA, Eirich  R, Lakes  KD, Madigan  S. Bidirectional associations between screen time and children's externalizing and internalizing behaviors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2021;62(12):1475-1484. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13425
17.
Zahn-Waxler  C, Shirtcliff  EA, Marceau  K.  Disorders of childhood and adolescence: gender and psychopathology.   Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4:275-303. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091358 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Bado  P, Schafer  J, Simioni  AR,  et al.  Screen time and psychopathology: investigating directionality using cross-lagged panel models.   Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Published online November 1, 2020. doi:10.1007/s00787-020-01675-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Rideout  V, Robb MB. The common sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. Common Sense Media; 2020. Accessed January 20, 2021. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2020_zero_to_eight_census_final_web.pdf
20.
Carson  V, Spence  JC, Cutumisu  N, Cargill  L.  Association between neighborhood socioeconomic status and screen time among pre-school children: a cross-sectional study.   BMC Public Health. 2010;10:367. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-10-367 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Tandon  PS, Zhou  C, Sallis  JF, Cain  KL, Frank  LD, Saelens  BE.  Home environment relationships with children’s physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time by socioeconomic status.   Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:88. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-9-88 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Rideout  V, Robb  MB. The common sense census: media use by tweens and teens. Common Sense Media; 2019. Accessed January 10, 2021. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2019-census-8-to-18-full-report-updated.pdf
23.
Ramirez  ER, Norman  GJ, Rosenberg  DE,  et al.  Adolescent screen time and rules to limit screen time in the home.   J Adolesc Health. 2011;48(4):379-385. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.07.013 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Marceau  K, Laurent  HK, Neiderhiser  JM,  et al.  Combined influences of genes, prenatal environment, cortisol, and parenting on the development of children’s internalizing versus externalizing problems.   Behav Genet. 2015;45(3):268-282. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9689-z PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Achenbach  TM.  The classification of children’s psychiatric symptoms: a factor-analytic study.   Psychol Monogr. 1966;80(7):1-37. doi:10.1037/h0093906 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
McArthur  BA, Racine  N, Browne  D, McDonald  S, Tough  S, Madigan  S. Recreational screen time before and during COVID-19 in school-aged children. Acta Paediatr. Published online June 15, 2021. doi:10.1111/apa.15966
27.
Racine  N, McArthur  BA, Cooke  JE, Eirich  R, Zhu  J, Madigan  S.  Global prevalence of depressive and anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents during COVID-19: a meta-analysis.   JAMA Pediatr. 2021;175(11):1142-1150. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2482 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Study quality assessment tools: quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies. National Institutes of Health. Accessed February 10, 2021. https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools
29.
Cicchetti  DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(4):284–290.
30.
Caldwell  DM, Davies  SR, Hetrick  SE,  et al.  School-based interventions to prevent anxiety and depression in children and young people: a systematic review and network meta-analysis.   Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(12):1011-1020. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30403-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Brumariu  LE, Madigan  S, Giuseppone  KR, Movahed Abtahi  M, Kerns  KA.  The Security Scale as a measure of attachment: meta-analytic evidence of validity.   Attach Hum Dev. 2018;20(6):600-625. doi:10.1080/14616734.2018.1433217 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Witzel  TC, Eshun-Wilson  I, Jamil  MS,  et al.  Comparing the effects of HIV self-testing to standard HIV testing for key populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   BMC Med. 2020;18(1):381. doi:10.1186/s12916-020-01835-z PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Rosenthal  R.  Writing meta-analytic reviews.   Psychol Bull. 1995;118(2):183-192. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.183 Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Peterson  RA, Brown  SP.  On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis.   J Appl Psychol. 2005;90(1):175-181. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Borenstein  M, Hedges  L, Higgins  JPT, Rothstein  HR.  Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 3.0. Biostat; 2013.
36.
DerSimonian  R, Laird  N.  Meta-analysis in clinical trials.   Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177-188. doi:10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Funder  DC, Ozer  DJ.  Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense.   Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(2):156-168. doi:10.1177/2515245919847202 Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Egger  M, Davey Smith  G, Schneider  M, Minder  C.  Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test.   BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629-634. doi:10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Borenstein  M.  Introduction to Meta-Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2009. doi:10.1002/9780470743386
40.
Huesmann  LR.  The impact of electronic media violence: scientific theory and research.   J Adolesc Health. 2007;41(6)(suppl 1):S6-S13. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.09.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Christakis  DA, Zimmerman  FJ, DiGiuseppe  DL, McCarty  CA.  Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children.   Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):708-713. doi:10.1542/peds.113.4.708 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Liu  M, Wu  L, Yao  S.  Dose-response association of screen time-based sedentary behaviour in children and adolescents and depression: a meta-analysis of observational studies.   Br J Sports Med. 2016;50(20):1252-1258. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2015-095084 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Guerrero  MD, Barnes  JD, Chaput  J-P, Tremblay  MS.  Screen time and problem behaviors in children: exploring the mediating role of sleep duration.   Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2019;16(1):105. doi:10.1186/s12966-019-0862-x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Al-Ali  NM, Yaghy  HS, Shattnawi  KK, Al-Shdayfat  NM.  Parents’ knowledge and beliefs about the impact of exposure to media violence on children’s aggression.   Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2018;39(7):592-599. doi:10.1080/01612840.2017.1422201 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Allen  MS, Vella  SA.  Screen-based sedentary behaviour and psychosocial well-being in childhood: cross-sectional and longitudinal associations.   Ment Health Phys Act. 2015;9:41-47. doi:10.1016/j.mhpa.2015.10.002 Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Amawi  SO, Subki  AH, Khatib  HA,  et al.  Use of electronic entertainment and communication devices among a Saudi pediatric population: cross-sectional study.   Interact J Med Res. 2018;7(2):e13. doi:10.2196/ijmr.9103 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Barlett  ND, Gentile  DA, Barlett  CP, Eisenmann  JC, Walsh  DA.  Sleep as a mediator of screen time effects on US children’s health outcomes.   J Child Media. 2012;6(1):37-50. doi:10.1080/17482798.2011.633404 Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Byun  Y-H, Ha  M, Kwon  H-J,  et al.  Mobile phone use, blood lead levels, and attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms in children: a longitudinal study.   PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59742. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059742 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Cao  H, Yan  S, Gu  C,  et al.  Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and their associations with sleep schedules and sleep-related problems among preschoolers in mainland China.   BMC Pediatr. 2018;18(1):70. doi:10.1186/s12887-018-1022-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Chonchaiya  W, Sirachairat  C, Vijakkhana  N, Wilaisakditipakorn  T, Pruksananonda  C.  Elevated background TV exposure over time increases behavioural scores of 18-month-old toddlers.   Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(10):1039-1046. doi:10.1111/apa.13067 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Christodoulou  G, Majmundar  A, Chou  C-P, Pentz  MA.  Anhedonia, screen time, and substance use in early adolescents: a longitudinal mediation analysis.   J Adolesc. 2020;78(C):24-32. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.11.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Coker  TR, Elliott  MN, Schwebel  DC,  et al.  Media violence exposure and physical aggression in fifth-grade children.   Acad Pediatr. 2015;15(1):82-88. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2014.09.008 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Corkin  MT, Peterson  ER, Henderson  AME, Waldie  KE, Reese  E, Morton  SMB.  Preschool screen media exposure, executive functions and symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity.   J Appl Dev Psychol. 2021;73:101237. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101237 Google ScholarCrossref
54.
Davison  LL.  Media Viewing and the Neurological Development of ADHD: Voluntary and Involuntary Processing. Dissertation. Claremont Graduate University; 2004.
55.
Ebenegger  V, Marques-Vidal  P-M, Munsch  S,  et al.  Relationship of hyperactivity/inattention with adiposity and lifestyle characteristics in preschool children.   J Child Neurol. 2012;27(7):852-858. doi:10.1177/0883073811428009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Erdogan  A, Kiran  S, Aydogan  G,  et al  Behavioral correlates of television viewing time in a Turkish sample of preschool children.   Neurol Psychiatr Brain Res. 2006;13(4):225-230.Google Scholar
57.
Froiland  JM, Davison  ML.  Home literacy, television viewing, fidgeting and ADHD in young children.   Educational Psychology. 2016;36(8):1337-1353. doi:10.1080/01443410.2014.963031 Google ScholarCrossref
58.
Gentile  DA, Berch  ON, Choo  H, Khoo  A, Walsh  DA.  Bedroom media: one risk factor for development.   Dev Psychol. 2017;53(12):2340-2355. doi:10.1037/dev0000399 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Gialamas  A, Haag  DG, Mittinty  MN, Lynch  J.  Which time investments in the first 5 years of life matter most for children’s language and behavioural outcomes at school entry?   Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49(2):548-558. doi:10.1093/ije/dyz192 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Guxens  M, Vermeulen  R, Steenkamer  I,  et al.  Radiofrequency electromagnetic fields, screen time, and emotional and behavioural problems in 5-year-old children.   Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2019;222(2):188-194. doi:10.1016/j.ijheh.2018.09.006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Hastings  EC, Karas  TL, Winsler  A, Way  E, Madigan  A, Tyler  S.  Young children’s video/computer game use: relations with school performance and behavior.   Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2009;30(10):638-649. doi:10.1080/01612840903050414 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Hefner  HL.  Risks of Television Viewing Among Children With and Without the ADHD Diagnosis. Dissertation. Walden University; 2013.
63.
Hill  MM, Gangi  D, Miller  M, Rafi  SM, Ozonoff  S.  Screen time in 36-month-olds at increased likelihood for ASD and ADHD.  Infant Behav Dev. 2020;61:101484.
64.
Holton  KF, Nigg  JT.  The association of lifestyle factors and ADHD in children.   J Atten Disord. 2020;24(11):1511-1520. doi:10.1177/1087054716646452 PubMedGoogle Scholar
65.
Hong  GCC, Conduit  R, Wong  J, Di Benedetto  M, Lee  E.  Diet, physical activity, and screen time to sleep better: multiple mediation analysis of lifestyle factors in school-aged children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.   J Atten Disord. Published July 14, 2020.PubMedGoogle Scholar
66.
Hosokawa  R, Katsura  T.  Association between mobile technology use and child adjustment in early elementary school age.   PLoS One. 2018;13(7):e0199959. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0199959 PubMedGoogle Scholar
67.
Huesmann  LR, Eron  LD.  Cognitive processes and the persistence of aggressive behavior.   Aggress Behav. 1984;10(3):243-251. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1984)10:3<243::AID-AB2480100308>3.0.CO;2-6 Google Scholar
68.
Jia  S, Wang  L, Shi  Y, Li  P.  Family risk factors associated with aggressive behavior in Chinese preschool children.   J Pediatr Nurs. 2016;31(6):e367-e374. doi:10.1016/j.pedn.2016.08.001 PubMedGoogle Scholar
69.
Kahn  M, Schnabel  O, Gradisar  M,  et al. Sleep, screen time and behaviour problems in preschool children: an actigraphy study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Published online October 1, 2020.
70.
Kuhhirt  M, Klein  M. Parental education, television exposure, and children's early cognitive, language and behavioral development. Soc Sci Res. 2020;86:102391.
71.
Linebarger  DL.  Contextualizing video game play: the moderating effects of cumulative risk and parenting styles on the relations among video game exposure and problem behaviors.   Psychol Pop Media Cult. 2015;4(4):375-396. doi:10.1037/ppm0000069 Google Scholar
72.
Levelink  B, van der Vlegel  M, Mommers  M,  et al.  The longitudinal relationship between screen time, sleep and a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in childhood.   J Atten Disord. 2020.PubMedGoogle Scholar
73.
Lin  H-P, Chen  K-L, Chou  W,  et al.  Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated with emotional and behavioral problems, but not language delay, in toddlers.  Infant Behav Dev. 2020;58:101424.
74.
Lobel  A, Engels  RCME, Stone  LL, Burk  WJ, Granic  I.  Video gaming and children’s psychosocial wellbeing: a longitudinal study.   J Youth Adolesc. 2017;46(4):884-897. doi:10.1007/s10964-017-0646-z PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
Manganello  JA, Taylor  CA.  Television exposure as a risk factor for aggressive behavior among 3-year-old children.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(11):1037-1045. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.193 PubMedGoogle Scholar
76.
Martin  A, Razza  R, Brooks-Gunn  J.  Specifying the links between household chaos and preschool children’s development.   Early Child Dev Care. 2012;182(10):1247-1263. doi:10.1080/03004430.2011.605522 PubMedGoogle Scholar
77.
Martins  N, Wilson  BJ.  Social aggression on television and its relationship to children’s aggression in the classroom.   Hum Commun Res. 2012;38(1):48-71. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2011.01417.x Google Scholar
78.
McNeill  J, Howard  SJ, Vella  SA, Cliff  DP.  Longitudinal associations of electronic application use and media program viewing with cognitive and psychosocial development in preschoolers.   Acad Pediatr. 2019;19(5):520-528. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2019.02.010 PubMedGoogle Scholar
79.
Miller  CJ, Marks  DJ, Miller  SR,  et al.  Brief report: television viewing and risk for attention problems in preschool children.   J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(4):448-452. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsl035 PubMedGoogle Scholar
80.
Miller  LE, Grabell  A, Thomas  A, Bermann  E, Graham-Bermann  SA.  The associations between community violence, television violence, intimate partner violence, parent–child aggression, and aggression in sibling relationships of a sample of preschoolers.   Psychol Violence. 2012;2(2):165-178. doi:10.1037/a0027254 Google Scholar
81.
Mistry  KB, Minkovitz  CS, Strobino  DM, Borzekowski  DLG.  Children’s television exposure and behavioral and social outcomes at 5.5 years: does timing of exposure matter?   Pediatrics. 2007;120(4):762-769. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3573 PubMedGoogle Scholar
82.
Moyer  VS.  The Role of User Motivations in Moderating the Relation Between Video Game Playing and Children's Adjustment. Dissertation. Bowling Green State University; 2008.
83.
Mundy  LK, Canterford  L, Olds  T, Allen  NB, Patton  GC.  The association between electronic media and emotional and behavioral problems in late childhood.   Acad Pediatr. 2017;17(6):620-624. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2016.12.014 PubMedGoogle Scholar
84.
Niiranen  J, Kiviruusu  O, Vornanen  R, Saarenpää-Heikkilä  O, Paavonen  EJ.  High-dose electronic media use in five-year-olds and its association with their psychosocial symptoms: a cohort study.   BMJ Open. 2021;11(3):e040848. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2020-040848 PubMedGoogle Scholar
85.
Nikkelen  SWC, Vossen  HGM, Valkenburg  PM.  Children’s television viewing and ADHD-related behaviors: evidence from The Netherlands.   J Child Media. 2015;9(4):399-418. doi:10.1080/17482798.2015.1088872 Google Scholar
86.
Obel  C, Henriksen  TB, Dalsgaard  S,  et al.  Does children’s watching of television cause attention problems? retesting the hypothesis in a Danish cohort.   Pediatrics. 2004;114(5):1372-1373. doi:10.1542/peds.2004-0954 PubMedGoogle Scholar
87.
Özmert  E, Toyran  M, Yurdakök  K.  Behavioral correlates of television viewing in primary school children evaluated by the child behavior checklist.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(9):910-914. doi:10.1001/archpedi.156.9.910 PubMedGoogle Scholar
88.
Parkes  A, Sweeting  H, Wight  D, Henderson  M.  Do television and electronic games predict children’s psychosocial adjustment? Longitudinal research using the UK Millennium Cohort Study.   Arch Dis Child. 2013;98(5):341-348. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2011-301508 PubMedGoogle Scholar
89.
Patel  C. Co-occurrence and Non-overlap Among Behavioral and Health-Related Problems in Preschool Children. Dissertation. University of South Carolina; 2019.
90.
Paulus  FW, Sinzig  J, Mayer  H, Weber  M, von Gontard  A.  Computer gaming disorder and ADHD in young children—a population-based study.   Int J Ment Health Addict. 2018;16(5):1193-1207. doi:10.1007/s11469-017-9841-0 Google Scholar
91.
Peralta  GP, Forns  J, García de la Hera  M,  et al.  Sleeping, TV, cognitively stimulating activities, physical activity, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptom incidence in children: a prospective study.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2018;39(3):192-199. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000539 PubMedGoogle Scholar
92.
Poulain  T, Vogel  M, Neef  M,  et al.  Reciprocal associations between electronic media use and behavioral difficulties in preschoolers.   Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(4):814. doi:10.3390/ijerph15040814 PubMedGoogle Scholar
93.
Reynolds  JE. An Assessment of Variables Associated With Television Viewing and Their Influence on Aggressive Behavior of Third Grade Children. Dissertation. Southern Illinois University. ProQuest Dissertations; 1978.
94.
Rosen  LD, Lim  AF, Felt  J,  et al.  Media and technology use predicts ill-being among children, preteens and teenagers independent of the negative health impacts of exercise and eating habits.   Comput Human Behav. 2014;35:364-375. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.036 PubMedGoogle Scholar
95.
Sanders  W, Parent  J, Forehand  R, Sullivan  ADW, Jones  DJ.  Parental perceptions of technology and technology-focused parenting: associations with youth screen time.   J Appl Dev Psychol. 2016;44:28-38. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.005 PubMedGoogle Scholar
96.
Schaefer  WG.  The Effects of Television Viewing on the Academic Performance of Elementary School Children With Attention Deficit Disorder. Dissertation. Western Michigan University; 1991.
97.
Schmiedeler  S, Niklas  F, Schneider  W.  Symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and home learning environment (HLE): findings from a longitudinal study.   Eur J Psychol Educ. 2014;29(3):467-482. doi:10.1007/s10212-013-0208-z Google Scholar
98.
Séguin  D, Klimek  V.  Just five more minutes please: electronic media use, sleep and behaviour in young children.   Early Child Dev Care. 2016;186(6):981-1000. doi:10.1080/03004430.2015.1071528 Google Scholar
99.
Sheehan  PW.  Age trends and the correlates of children’s television viewing.   Aust J Psychol. 1983;35(3):417-431. doi:10.1080/00049538308258753 Google Scholar
100.
Shmukler  D.  A descriptive analysis of television viewing in South African Preschoolers and its relationship to their spontaneous play.   S Afr J Psychol. 1981;11(3):106-110. doi:10.1177/008124638101100303 Google Scholar
101.
Stenseng  F, Hygen  BW, Wichstrøm  L.  Time spent gaming and psychiatric symptoms in childhood: cross-sectional associations and longitudinal effects.   Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;29(6):839-847. doi:10.1007/s00787-019-01398-2 PubMedGoogle Scholar
102.
Stevens  T, Mulsow  M.  There is no meaningful relationship between television exposure and symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.   Pediatrics. 2006;117(3):665-672. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-0863 PubMedGoogle Scholar
103.
Sugawara  M, Matsumoto  S, Murohashi  H, Sakai  A, Isshiki  N.  Trajectories of early television contact in Japan: relationship with preschoolers’ externalizing problems.   J Child Media. 2015;9(4):453-471. doi:10.1080/17482798.2015.1089298 Google Scholar
104.
Swing  EL, Gentile  DA, Anderson  CA, Walsh  DA.  Television and video game exposure and the development of attention problems.   Pediatrics. 2010;126(2):214-221. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-1508 PubMedGoogle Scholar
105.
Tamana  SK, Ezeugwu  V, Chikuma  J,  et al; CHILD study Investigators.  Screen-time is associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: results from the CHILD birth cohort study.   PLoS One. 2019;14(4):e0213995. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0213995 PubMedGoogle Scholar
106.
Tansriratanawong  S, Louthrenoo  O, Chonchaiya  W, Charnsil  C.  Screen viewing time and externalising problems in pre-school children in Northern Thailand.   J Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2017;29(3):245-252. doi:10.2989/17280583.2017.1409226 PubMedGoogle Scholar
107.
Teramoto  S, Soeda  A, Hayashi  Y, Saito  K, Urashima  M.  Problematic behaviours of 3-year-old children in Japan: relationship with socioeconomic and family backgrounds.   Early Hum Dev. 2005;81(6):563-569. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.12.011 PubMedGoogle Scholar
108.
Verlinden  M, Tiemeier  H, Hudziak  JJ,  et al.  Television viewing and externalizing problems in preschool children: the Generation R Study.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(10):919-925. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.653 PubMedGoogle Scholar
109.
Viemerö  V, Paajanen  S.  The role of fantasies and dreams in the TV viewing—aggression relationship.   Aggress Behav. 1992;18(2):109-116. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1992)18:2<109::AID-AB2480180204>3.0.CO;2-1 Google Scholar
110.
Wan  MW, Fitch-Bunce  C, Heron  K, Lester  E.  Infant screen media usage and social-emotional functioning.  Infant Behav Dev. 2021;62:101509. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101509
111.
Wimbarti  S.  Children’s Aggression in Indonesia: The Effects of Culture, Familial Factors, Peers, TV Violence Viewing, and Temperament. Dissertation. University of Southern California; 2002.
112.
Woodfield  DL. Mass Media Viewing Habits and Toleration of Real-life Aggression. Dissertation. In: ProQuest Dissertations; 1987.
113.
Wu  X, Kirk  SFL, Ohinmaa  A, Veugelers  P.  Health behaviours, body weight and self-esteem among grade five students in Canada.   Springerplus. 2016;5(1):1099. doi:10.1186/s40064-016-2744-x PubMedGoogle Scholar
114.
Wu  X, Tao  S, Rutayisire  E, Chen  Y, Huang  K, Tao  F.  The relationship between screen time, nighttime sleep duration, and behavioural problems in preschool children in China.   Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(5):541-548. doi:10.1007/s00787-016-0912-8 PubMedGoogle Scholar
115.
Xie  G, Deng  Q, Cao  J, Chang  Q.  Digital screen time and its effect on preschoolers’ behavior in China: results from a cross-sectional study.   Ital J Pediatr. 2020;46(1):9. doi:10.1186/s13052-020-0776-x PubMedGoogle Scholar
116.
Yousef  S, Eapen  V, Zoubeidi  T, Mabrouk  A.  Behavioral correlation with television watching and videogame playing among children in the United Arab Emirates.   Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2014;18(3):203-207. doi:10.3109/13651501.2013.874442 PubMedGoogle Scholar
117.
Zimmerman  FJ, Christakis  DA.  Associations between content types of early media exposure and subsequent attentional problems.   Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):986-992. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3322 PubMedGoogle Scholar
118.
Zimmerman  FJ, Glew  GM, Christakis  DA, Katon  W.  Early cognitive stimulation, emotional support, and television watching as predictors of subsequent bullying among grade-school children.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(4):384-388. doi:10.1001/archpedi.159.4.384 PubMedGoogle Scholar
119.
Brubaker  MM.  Exploring the Relationship Between Social Support and Social Behavior: Moderating Effects of Video Game Use. Dissertation. Fielding Graduate University. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing; 2020.
120.
Cooper  NS. The Identification of Psychological and Social Correlates of internet Use in Children and Teenagers. Dissertation. Alliant International University; 2004.
121.
Dunkeld  C, Wright  ML, Banerjee  RA, Easterbrook  MJ, Slade  L.  Television exposure, consumer culture values, and lower well-being among preadolescent children: the mediating role of consumer-focused coping strategies.   Br J Soc Psychol. 2020;59(1):26-48. doi:10.1111/bjso.12325 PubMedGoogle Scholar
122.
Lam  MW, Burgoon  E, Jin  C.  5.37 Associations of screen time and depression in children in a US nationally representative sample.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58 (10)(suppl):S257. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2019.08.351 Google Scholar
123.
Lin  S-Y, Eaton  NR, Schleider  JL.  Unpacking associations between mood symptoms and screen time in preadolescents: a network analysis.   J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48(12):1635-1647. doi:10.1007/s10802-020-00703-x PubMedGoogle Scholar
124.
Zhang  J, Liu  M-W, Yu  H-J,  et al. Associations of health-risk behaviors with mental health among Chinese children. Psychol Health Med. Published December 9, 2020.
125.
Zhu  X, Haegele  JA, Healy  S.  Movement and mental health: behavioral correlates of anxiety and depression among children of 6-17 years old in the US.   Ment Health Phys Act. 2019;16:60-65. doi:10.1016/j.mhpa.2019.04.002 Google Scholar
126.
Madigan  S, McArthur  BA, Anhorn  C, Eirich  R, Christakis  DA.  Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665-675. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327 PubMedGoogle Scholar
127.
Adelantado-Renau  M, Moliner-Urdiales  D, Cavero-Redondo  I, Beltran-Valls  MR, Martínez-Vizcaíno  V, Álvarez-Bueno  C.  Association between screen media use and academic performance among children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(11):1058-1067. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3176 PubMedGoogle Scholar
128.
Hentges  RF, Devereux  C, Graham  SA, Madigan  S.  Child language difficulties and internalizing and externalizing symptoms: a meta-analysis.   Child Dev. 2021;92(4):e691-e715. doi:10.1111/cdev.13540 PubMedGoogle Scholar
129.
Peverill  M, Dirks  MA, Narvaja  T, Herts  KL, Comer  JS, McLaughlin  KA.  Socioeconomic status and child psychopathology in the United States: a meta-analysis of population-based studies.   Clin Psychol Rev. 2021;83:101933. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101933 PubMedGoogle Scholar
130.
Rutledge  T, Loh  C.  Effect sizes and statistical testing in the determination of clinical significance in behavioral medicine research.   Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(2):138-145. doi:10.1207/s15324796abm2702_9 PubMedGoogle Scholar
131.
McArthur  BA, Volkova  V, Tomopoulos  S, Madigan  S. Global prevalence of meeting screen time guidelines among children 5 years and younger: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. Published online February 14, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.6386
132.
Groh  AM, Roisman  GI, van Ijzendoorn  MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg  MJ, Fearon  RP.  The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing symptoms: a meta-analytic study.   Child Dev. 2012;83(2):591-610. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01711.x PubMedGoogle Scholar
133.
Hoyos Cillero  I, Jago  R.  Sociodemographic and home environment predictors of screen viewing among Spanish school children.   J Public Health (Oxf). 2011;33(3):392-402. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdq087 PubMedGoogle Scholar
134.
Mesman  J, Bongers  IL, Koot  HM.  Preschool developmental pathways to preadolescent internalizing and externalizing problems.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(5):679-689. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00763 PubMedGoogle Scholar
135.
Tomopoulos  S, Dreyer  BP, Valdez  P,  et al.  Media content and externalizing behaviors in Latino toddlers.   Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7(3):232-238. doi:10.1016/j.ambp.2007.02.004 PubMedGoogle Scholar
136.
Funk  JB, Baldacci  HB, Pasold  T, Baumgardner  J.  Violence exposure in real-life, video games, television, movies, and the internet: is there desensitization?   J Adolesc. 2004;27(1):23-39. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.005 PubMedGoogle Scholar
137.
Bandura  A.  Social Learning Theory. Prentice-Hall; 1977.
138.
World Health Organization. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for Children Under 5 Years of Age. World Health Organization; 2019. Accessed February 9, 2022. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664
139.
Madigan  S, Racine  N, Tough  S.  Prevalence of preschoolers meeting vs exceeding screen time guidelines.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):93-95. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4495 PubMedGoogle Scholar
140.
Sigman  A.  Time for a view on screen time.   Arch Dis Child. 2012;97(11):935-942. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2012-302196 PubMedGoogle Scholar
141.
Harvey  DL, Milton  K, Jones  AP, Atkin  AJ.  International trends in screen-based behaviours from 2012 to 2019.   Prev Med. 2022;154:106909. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106909 PubMedGoogle Scholar
142.
Paulo  MS, Nauman  J, Abdulle  A,  et al.  Results from the United Arab Emirates’ 2018 report card on physical activity for children and youth.   J Phys Act Health. 2018;15(S2):S419-S421. doi:10.1123/jpah.2018-0543 PubMedGoogle Scholar
143.
Ernest  JM, Causey  C, Newton  AB, Sharkins  K, Summerlin  J, Albaiz  N.  Extending the global dialogue about media, technology, screen time, and young children.   Childhood Education. 2014;90(3):182-191. doi:10.1080/00094056.2014.910046 Google Scholar
144.
De Los Reyes  A, Kazdin  AE.  Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: a critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study.   Psychol Bull. 2005;131(4):483-509. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483 PubMedGoogle Scholar
145.
De Los Reyes  A.  Introduction to the special section: more than measurement error: discovering meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical assessments of children and adolescents.   J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40(1):1-9. doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.533405 PubMedGoogle Scholar
146.
Achenbach  TM, Edelbrock  CS.  The classification of child psychopathology: a review and analysis of empirical efforts.   Psychol Bull. 1978;85(6):1275-1301. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.85.6.1275 PubMedGoogle Scholar
147.
Greenbaum  PE, Dedrick  RF.  Hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18.   Psychol Assess. 1998;10(2):149-155. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.149 Google Scholar
148.
Radesky  JS, Weeks  HM, Ball  R,  et al.  Young children’s use of smartphones and tablets.   Pediatrics. 2020;146(1):e20193518. doi:10.1542/peds.2019-3518 PubMedGoogle Scholar
149.
Ioannidis  JP.  Why most discovered true associations are inflated.   Epidemiology. 2008;19(5):640-648. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818131e7 PubMedGoogle Scholar
150.
Schooler  J. Unpublished results hide the decline effect. Nature. 2011;470(7335):437.
151.
de Bruin  A, Della Sala  S.  The decline effect: how initially strong results tend to decrease over time.   Cortex. 2015;73:375-377. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.025 PubMedGoogle Scholar
152.
Chen  W, Adler  JL.  Assessment of screen exposure in young children, 1997 to 2014.   JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(4):391-393. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5546 PubMedGoogle Scholar
153.
van IJzendoorn  MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg  MJ.  Differential susceptibility experiments: going beyond correlational evidence: comment on beyond mental health, differential susceptibility articles.   Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):769-774. doi:10.1037/a0027536 PubMedGoogle Scholar
154.
Sudan  M, Olsen  J, Arah  OA, Obel  C, Kheifets  L.  Prospective cohort analysis of cellphone use and emotional and behavioural difficulties in children.   J Epidemiol Community Health. 2016;70(12):1207-1213. doi:10.1136/jech-2016-207419 PubMedGoogle Scholar
155.
Christakis  DA.  The challenges of defining and studying “digital addiction” in children.   JAMA. 2019;321(23):2277-2278. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.4690 PubMedGoogle Scholar
156.
Shah  RR, Fahey  NM, Soni  AV, Phatak  AG, Nimbalkar  SM.  Screen time usage among preschoolers aged 2-6 in rural Western India: a cross-sectional study.   J Family Med Prim Care. 2019;8(6):1999-2002. doi:10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_206_19 PubMedGoogle Scholar
157.
Tang  L, Darlington  G, Ma  DWL, Haines  J; Guelph Family Health Study.  Mothers’ and fathers’ media parenting practices associated with young children’s screen-time: a cross-sectional study.   BMC Obes. 2018;5(1):37. doi:10.1186/s40608-018-0214-4 PubMedGoogle Scholar
158.
Korhonen  L.  The good, the bad and the ugly of children´s screen time during the COVID-19 pandemic.   Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(10):2671-2672. doi:10.1111/apa.16012 PubMedGoogle Scholar
159.
Xiang  M, Zhang  Z, Kuwahara  K.  Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on children and adolescents’ lifestyle behavior larger than expected.   Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 2020;63(4):531-532. doi:10.1016/j.pcad.2020.04.013 PubMedGoogle Scholar
160.
Hamaker  EL, Kuiper  RM, Grasman  RPPP.  A critique of the cross-lagged panel model.   Psychol Methods. 2015;20(1):102-116. doi:10.1037/a0038889 PubMedGoogle Scholar
Original Investigation
March 16, 2022

Association of Screen Time With Internalizing and Externalizing Behavior Problems in Children 12 Years or Younger: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta, Canada
  • 2Alberta Children’s Hospital Research Institute, Calgary, Alberta, Canada
  • 3Seattle Children’s Hospital Research Institute, University of Washington, Seattle
  • 4Editor, JAMA Pediatrics
JAMA Psychiatry. 2022;79(5):393-405. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.0155
Key Points

Question  Is there an association between screen time and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems in the extant screen time literature?

Findings  In this systematic review and meta-analysis of 87 studies (98 independent samples) including 159 425 children 12 years or younger, greater duration of screen time was weakly but significantly correlated with externalizing (eg, aggression, inattention) and internalizing (eg, anxiety, depression) behavior problems. Results varied as a function of demographic (eg, sex) and methodological factors (eg, informant and measurement method).

Meaning  The findings showed weak but significant correlations between screen time and children’s behavior problems and suggest that methodological variability may have contributed to mixed findings in the literature.

Abstract

Importance  Currently, there is a lack of consensus in the literature on the association between screen time (eg, television, video games) and children’s behavior problems.

Objective  To assess the association between the duration of screen time and externalizing and internalizing behavior problems among children 12 years or younger.

Data Sources  For this systematic review and meta-analysis, MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO databases were searched for articles published from January 1960 to May 2021. Reference lists were manually searched for additional studies.

Study Selection  Included studies measured screen time (ie, duration) and externalizing or internalizing behavior problems in children 12 years or younger, were observational or experimental (with baseline data), were available in English, and had data that could be transformed into an effect size. Studies conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic were excluded. Of 25 196 nonduplicate articles identified and screened for inclusion, 595 met the selection criteria.

Data Extraction and Synthesis  The study followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) reporting guideline. Extracted variables were child age, sex, and socioeconomic status; informants and measurement type for screen time and behavior problems; study publication year; and study design and quality. Data were extracted by 2 independent coders and were pooled using a random-effects model.

Main Outcomes and Measures  The primary outcome was the association of screen time duration with externalizing (eg, aggression, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms) and internalizing (eg, depression, anxiety) behaviors or diagnoses.

Results  Of the 595 full-text articles assessed for eligibility, 87 studies met all inclusion criteria, comprising 98 independent samples and 159 425 participants (mean [SD] age, 6.07 [2.89] years; 83 246 [51.30%] male). Increased duration of screen time had a small but significant correlation with more externalizing problems (90 samples; r, 0.11; 95% CI, 0.10-0.12) and internalizing problems (43 samples; r, 0.07; 95% CI, 0.05-0.08) in children. Several methodological moderators explained between-study heterogeneity. There was evidence of significant between study heterogeneity (I2 = 87.80).

Conclusions and Relevance  This systematic review and meta-analysis found small but significant correlations between screen time and children’s behavior problems. Methodological differences across studies likely contributed to the mixed findings in the literature.

Introduction

The effects of screen time on children’s mental health has been rigorously debated.1,2 Some literature suggests that screen time may be associated with risk of poor mental health outcomes by displacing sleep and physical activities as well as social exchanges and learning opportunities known to foster well-being.3,4 Screen media may also impede self-regulation strategies and increase arousal levels owing to fast-paced and intense audiovisual effects, which may be associated with inattention and aggressive behavior.5,6 However, it has been argued that concern with regard to screen time and its effect on child mental health is not empirically justified owing to conflicting research results and methodological shortcomings.7-10

Meta-analyses can address discrepancies in the literature by providing greater statistical precision through pooled results from multiple individual studies11 and can detect whether variations in study findings are explained by moderators. In the screen time literature, methodological differences have likely contributed to the conflicting findings: effect sizes may differ based on the statistical analyses conducted,12 how variables are measured,13 and whether data are cross-sectional or longtudinal.7 Effect sizes may also vary as a function of child sex (eg, sex differences in the associations of screen time14,15 with behavior problems),16-18 child age (eg, association of increased screen use with increasing age among children),15,19 and socioeconomic risk (eg, association of increased screen use with behavior problems in the context of low socioeconomic status).20,21 Identifying when and for whom correlations are stronger or weaker may inform the ongoing screen time debate and help develop targeted interventions for children most at risk for potential behavior problems associated with screen time.

Owing to substantial research to date on duration of screen time and behavior problems, it appeared timely to meta-analytically summarize this body of research. Given that the context and nature of screen use changes from childhood to adolescence from mostly parent-mediated and television-centric to less parent-mediated and weighted toward social media,19,22,23 this systematic review and meta-analysis focused on children 12 years or younger. Because of their different mechanisms,24,25 we conducted 2 separate meta-analyses for externalizing (eg, aggression, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms) and internalizing (eg, anxiety, depression) behavior problems.25 In addition, we sought to identify sources of between-study heterogeneity.

Methods
Search Strategy

In this systematic review and meta-analysis, searches were conducted in the MEDLINE, Embase, and PsycINFO databases by a science librarian for articles published from January 1960 to May 2021. The concepts of screen time, internalizing and externalizing behavior, and children were captured by searching database-specific subject headings and text word fields (eTable 1 in the Supplement). Synonymous terms were combined with the Boolean “OR” and then combined with the Boolean “AND.” The concept of children (12 years or younger) was searched using the “age limits” database functions and via text word search. In all databases, truncation symbols were used in text word searches to capture variations in phrasing and spelling. No language limits were applied. Reference lists in included studies and review articles were manually searched for additional studies. This study followed the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-analyses (PRISMA) reporting guideline.

Study Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

Studies were included if (1) the age range of included children was 12 years or less, (2) screen time duration was measured (hours and/or minutes), (3) behavior problems (ie, internalizing, externalizing) were measured, (4) the studies were observational or experimental (with baseline data), (5) statistical data were available, and (6) the article was available in English. Because child screen time and mental distress have increased during the COVID-19 pandemic,26,27 we excluded studies conducted during the pandemic. Inclusion and exclusion criteria are detailed in eTable 2 in the Supplement. Studies were assessed for inclusion by 2 coders (R.E. and C.A.). Any study deemed to meet inclusion criteria by either coder was considered for full-text review.

Data Extraction
Study Quality Assessment

Each study was evaluated by 2 independent coders (C.A. and C.M.) for quality based on an adapted 16-item quality assessment tool28 (eTable 3 in the Supplement), with each item coded as 0 (no) or 1 (yes) (eTable 4 in the Supplement). Intercoder agreement for the overall quality score was good (intraclass correlation coefficient, 0.75). Discrepancies were resolved via consensus.

Moderating Variables

Moderators extracted were (1) child sex (percentage of males in the study); (2) child age at outcome measurement (in months); (3) screen time informant (child or parent); (4) screen time measurement method (ie, activity log, questionnaire, or interview); (5) type of internalizing (anxiety, depression, or somatization) or externalizing (aggression or attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms) behavior problems; (6) informant (child, parent, clinician or coder, peers, teacher, or combination); (7) measurement method (diagnostic and structural interview, observer report, or questionnaire); (8) whether informants differed for screen time and behavior problems; (9) clinical sample (diagnosis of a preexisting behavior problem [yes or no]); (10) publication year; (11) study design (cross-sectional, longitudinal with baseline outcome adjustment, or longitudinal with no baseline adjustment); (12) sociodemographic risk (less than 80% vs 80% or more of the sample had at least 1 of the following: low income, low caregiver educational level, or an adolescent parent); and (13) geographic location. eTable 5 in the Supplement shows the data extraction document used. All included studies were independently coded by 2 trained coders (C.A. and C.M.), with excellent reliability29 for continuous moderators (intraclass correlation coefficient, 0.80-1.00). The mean percentage of agreement for categorical moderators was 92.5% (range, 85%-100%). Discrepancies were resolved via consensus.

Data Synthesis

When multiple studies conducted analyses on the same sample, the study with the largest sample size and most comprehensive information was selected. Adjusted statistics were selected over unadjusted statistics whenever available. Global measures of internalizing or externalizing symptoms and of screen time were selected over discrete symptoms (eg, depression, anxiety) or screen types (eg, tablet, video games). Similar to the methods of other meta-analyses,30-32 when a single study measured screen time and/or behavior problems at multiple time points, effect sizes with the largest temporal distance between measures were selected. When studies reported nonsignificant findings without any corresponding statistic or P value, a P value of .50 was entered.33 Studies reporting β coefficients between –0.50 and 0.50 were imputed to correlations (r).34 Effect sizes were pooled when a single effect size among many in a study could not be selected based on the aforementioned criteria. In addition, if a study provided effect sizes from multiple discrete samples with different population parameters (eg, different cohorts), these samples were entered into the meta-analysis separately.

Statistical Analysis

Pooled effect size estimates and moderator analyses were conducted using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis, version 3.0 (Biostat).35 Effect sizes were transformed into correlations (r) with 95% CIs using random-effects modeling.36 Correlations were interpreted as small (0.1), moderate (0.2), or large (0.3) based on conventional standards.37

Q and I2 statistics were used to assess between-study heterogeneity. Random-effect meta-regressions and subgroup comparisons were conducted to assess continuous and categorical moderators, respectively. Subgroup comparisons were only conducted when there were at least 3 samples (ie, ≥3 cells) available for each categorical comparison.35 Inspection of funnel plots and the Egger test were used to estimate publication bias.38 A 2-tailed P value of .05 was used for the primary analyses, and owing to the number of moderator analyses conducted, a conservative 2-sided α value of .01 was used to assess the significance of moderators.39

Results

The search strategy revealed 25 196 nonduplicate abstracts to be reviewed for determination of meeting inclusion criteria. A total of 595 full-text articles were assessed for eligibility, and 87 studies (with 98 unique samples) were included in the meta-analysis (Figure 1).

Study Characteristics

The 98 samples consisted of 159 425 participants. Sample sizes in the included studies ranged from 15 to 15 291 participants (median, 532.5 participants). The mean (SD) age of participants when screen time was assessed was 6.07 (2.89) years (range, 0.5-11.0 years), and the mean (SD) age when behavior problems were assessed was 7.16 (2.70) years (range, 1.3-12.0 years). A total of 83 246 participants (51.25%) were male (median percentage of males per study, 51.31% [range, 36.60% to 78.70%]). Of the 98 total samples, 44 (44.9%) were in North America, 24 (24.5%) in Europe, 1 (1.0%) in Africa, 14 (14.3%) in Asia, 7 (7.1%) in Australia or New Zealand, 5 (5.1%) in the Middle East, and 2 (2.0%) in South America; 1 sample (1.0%) was multisite (detailed study characteristics are shown in eTable 6 in the Supplement).

Screen Time and Externalizing Behavior Problems
Pooled Effect Sizes

In the 90 samples from 80 studies (124 027 children), the correlation was small but significant (r, 0.11; 95% CI, 0.10-0.12) (Figure 2).16,40-118 The Egger test result provided evidence that studies with smaller sample sizes had more extreme effect sizes, and the funnel plot showed asymmetry (eFigure 1 in the Supplement), indicating possible publication bias and/or small-study effects. There was evidence of significant between-study heterogeneity (Q, 729.78; P < .001; I2 = 87.80); therefore, moderators were explored (Table 1).

Moderator Analyses

Meta-regression analyses of the 90 samples showed that the effect size for the association between screen time and externalizing problems increased as the percentage of males in studies increased (b, 0.007 [SE, 0.002]; z, 3.23; P = .001). Effect sizes also decreased as the study publication year increased (b, −0.003 [SE, 0.001]; z, –3.98; P < .001). The effect size for the association between screen time and externalizing problems decreased as study quality increased (b, −0.017 [SE, 0.004]; z, –4.30; P < .001) (Table 2).

Correlations between screen time and externalizing problems were stronger in studies examining aggression (21 samples; r, 0.17; 95% CI, 0.13-0.20) compared with those examining attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms (25 samples; r, 0.09; 95% CI, 0.06-0.11). Correlations were weaker in longitudinal studies that controlled for baseline externalizing problems (19 samples; r, 0.06; 95% CI, 0.04-0.08) compared with cross-sectional studies (50 samples; r, 0.13; 95% CI, 0.11-0.15) and longitudinal studies without baseline control (21 samples; r, 0.11; 95% CI, 0.08-0.14). Studies conducted in the Middle East had stronger associations between screen time and externalizing problems (5 samples; r, 0.23; 95% CI, 0.17-0.29) compared with those conducted in all other geographic locations. Stronger correlations were also found in studies using observer reports (8 samples; r, 0.20; 95% CI, 0.14-0.26) compared with those using questionnaires (74 samples; r, 0.10; 95% CI, 0.08-0.12). In addition, studies using peers to assess externalizing problems had stronger correlations (8 samples; r, 0.20; 95% CI, 0.15-0.26) compared with those that used child report (4 samples; r, 0.08; 95% CI, 0.00-0.15), parent report (57 samples; r, 0.10; 95% CI, 0.08-0.12), teacher report (7 samples; r, 0.07; 95% CI, 0.02-0.13), and a combination of informants (8 samples; r, 0.09; 95% CI, 0.04-0.14).

Screen Time and Internalizing Behavior Problems
Pooled Effect Sizes

In 43 samples from 40 studies (99 603 children), the effect size between child screen time and internalizing problems was weak but significant (r, 0.07; 95% CI, 0.05-0.08) (Figure 3).16,42,43,45,50-52,56,60,66,69,73,74,76,78,81,83,84,87,88,92,94,95,98,101,105-107,110,113-116,119-125 The funnel plot showed some asymmetry (eFigure 2 in the Supplement), indicating possible publication bias; however, the Egger test result did not suggest that smaller sample sizes had more extreme effect sizes. The Q statistic was significant (Q, 285.10; P < .001; I2, 85.27), and moderator analyses were conducted to explain between-study heterogeneity (Table 1). Only 1 moderator analysis had significant results: studies that used different informants to measure internalizing problems and screen time had a stronger correlation (9 samples; r, 0.08; 95% CI, 0.05-0.10) compared with those with the same informant across measures (34 samples; r, 0.01; 95% CI, –0.02 to 0.03).

Discussion

The increasing rate of screen time in early childhood has engendered concern among clinicians, policy makers, and parents regarding its possible effects on children’s mental health. This study found small but significant correlations between screen time and children’s internalizing and externalizing behavior problems. The magnitude of these correlations is comparable with that found in other meta-analyses on the association between screen time and child language skills126 and academic performance.127 Moreover, effect sizes derived in this study are similar to those in other meta-analyses examining associations between various family and child factors (eg, language skills128 and socioeconomic status129) and internalizing and externalizing problems. Although the effect sizes found in this study were small, the consequences of screen time at a population level are likely meaningful,37,130 particularly because a recent meta-analysis131 suggested that 75% of children younger than 2 years and 64% of children aged 2 to 5 years exceed screen time guidelines.

Effect sizes were larger for the association between screen time and externalizing problems compared with internalizing problems given nonoverlapping 95% CIs. Future studies testing which discrete mechanisms underlie the association between screen time and externalizing vs internalizing problems are warranted. For example, it is possible that the content viewed (eg, inappropriate or violent) underlies the association between screen time and externalizing problems, whereas indirect effects such as social withdrawal or sleep disruption may underlie the association between screen time and internalizing problems. From a methodological perspective, externalizing problems may be easier for reporters to observe than internalizing problems owing to their outward display, leading to poorer sensitivity for identifying internalizing problems.132

We found stronger correlations between screen time and externalizing problems in boys compared with girls. This finding is consistent with that of previous research in which boys had higher screen use14,133 and more externalizing problems17 than did girls. Externalizing behaviors, such as aggression, may be more readily modeled by boys through sex-stereotypic socialization.134 In addition, we found that studies measuring aggression had larger effect sizes than those measuring attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. While viewing screen media, children may be exposed to inappropriate content, aggression, and violence.40,135,136 Consistent with social learning theory,137 children may become desensitized after repeated exposures and model aggressive or violent content toward others.6,40,136 Moreover, as screen time becomes more normalized, it is possible that aggressive behavior within some screen programming does as well. Screen use guidelines138 suggest that parents should monitor screen time, ensure the content their children are viewing is age-appropriate, limit exposure to violent content, communicate with their children about inappropriate on-screen content, and model healthy device habits.139

Correlations of screen time with externalizing behavior problems were stronger in the Middle East compared with any other geographic location; however, sample sizes in studies from the Middle East were small, and 95% CIs were wide, limiting the conclusions that can be made. A dose-response relationship between screen time and externalizing problems41,42,140 may explain the stronger correlations found in countries in the Middle East because early-onset screen use has been increasing at a faster rate in these countries compared with other countries.141,142 More research is needed to understand how cultural differences (eg, different guidelines and beliefs about screen time)143 contribute to associations between screen time and behavior problems.

The mixed findings in the literature on screen time and behavior problems may largely be the result of methodological variability.7 Studies that used more objective methods (eg, observer report) and reporters (eg, peers) of children’s externalizing problems had stronger correlations than those using child, parent, or teacher reports. In addition, studies that used different informants for screen time and internalizing problems had stronger correlations than those using the same informant across measures. Inconsistency between informants for children’s behavior problems is common,144 but informant discrepancies may indicate the contexts and ways in which behavior problems are perceived by the child and others.145 For example, aggression may be noticed more readily by peers owing to children having more opportunities to engage with peers aggressively (eg, bullying).

Variability exists both across (ie, aggression, oppositional behavior, and hyperactivity) and within (eg, demanding, hits others) the broad construct of externalizing behavior problems. However, historically, externalizing problems have been examined as a composite.25,146 This study’s finding that screen time was more strongly correlated with aggression suggests that the use of an externalizing composite may not capture inherent nuances in the association between screen time and behavior problems. Furthermore, as children move into adolescence, attention problems and aggression are often measured as separate constructs.147 Future research should consider disaggregating externalizing behaviors, presenting associations in their subcomponents (eg, aggression, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms), and examining which items within the subcomponents are correlated most strongly with screen time.

Given that methodological variability is critical for understanding patterns of associations in this body of research, several methodological implications are suggested for future research. To the extent possible, future research should use multi-informant (eg, peers, child), multimethod (eg, passive sensing apps and questionnaires), and longitudinal approaches to measure the association of screen time with child outcomes. Of importance, self-report screen time measurement may be biased.148 None of the studies identified in our meta-analysis used objective measures (eg, sensing apps) to evaluate screen time, presenting an opportunity for future research to use novel screen-time measurement methods.

Two characteristics of publications were identified as significant moderators of associations between screen time and externalizing problems: study quality and publication year. Associations found between screen time and externalizing problems decreased as study quality increased. Poorer quality studies often have more random error and inflated effects owing to less methodological rigor (eg, a single informant, lack of statistical controls). Study quality is also closely connected to study year, with larger effect sizes found in older, discovery studies typically owing to more reliance on significance testing (P value thresholds), inadequate power, and measurement error.149 Thus, effect sizes also decreased in newer studies, which is consistent with the notion of a decline effect (ie, diminishing effect sizes over time).150,151 The accessibility and use of screens have increased over time.152 As screens become normalized in childhood and contemporary culture, the risks associated with their use may become less consequential for children’s behavior problems. Furthermore, parents today may be more informed and better at monitoring screen time and content owing to awareness of screen time guidelines.131

Limitations

This study has several limitations. First and of most importance, effect sizes are correlational, not causal. Causal reductionism should be avoided because behavior problems are associated with a complex combination of genetic and environmental factors. Additional research that addresses this complexity is needed (eg, genetically informative designs).153-155 Directionality cannot be inferred; screens may be used as a tool to placate or negotiate with children.156,157 Second, although there was significant heterogeneity for internalizing problems, only 1 of the moderator analyses had significant results. Other important but unexamined moderators, such as genetic susceptibility,155 sleep,43 or language development,126 may have a significant role in associations. Third, with rapid shifts in technology, generational cohorts may not use devices comparably.7 Mobile phones were originally used almost exclusively for calling, and few children had their own; now, smartphones are used to access web pages, play games, stream videos, and socialize, and 69% of US children have their own smartphone by 12 years of age.22 Although we excluded studies conducted during the COVID-19 pandemic, a cohort of children are growing up during the pandemic. Screen time has increased significantly during the pandemic, and this increase may affect child development.26,158,159 In addition, not all screen time is equal in terms of content, subject, and formal features; some screen time may be more consequential than other screen time. Our findings only apply to screen time broadly in terms of duration or quantity of use. More nuanced aspects of screen time, such as screen content (eg, social media, violent video games), context (eg, coviewing vs passive viewing), and quality (eg, educational vs entertainment), should be examined. Future studies should also ensure that screen time is not measured as a single construct across multiple devices and should instead focus on the function of screen use (eg, entertainment, socializing, or education) to examine the way screens are being used without being device-specific.

Conclusions

The association between screen time and children’s mental health has garnered marked attention from academic, health, and public sectors. This systematic review and meta-analysis found that screen time was weakly but significantly correlated with children’s internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Our study identified several important methodological moderators of the association between screen time and behavior problems, such as the type of informants and the measures of screen time used, that highlight the lack of harmony in the screen time literature. It is essential for researchers to use more rigorous methods and approaches to analyzing screen time data16,160 and to further explore the various facets of screen use (ie, content and context) associated with children’s mental health in a rapidly evolving digital world.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: January 17, 2022.

Published Online: March 16, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2022.0155

Corresponding Author: Sheri Madigan, PhD, Department of Psychology, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta T2N 1N4, Canada (sheri.madigan@ucalgary.ca).

Author Contributions: Ms Eirich and Dr Madigan had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Eirich, Christakis, Madigan.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: All authors.

Drafting of the manuscript: Eirich, McArthur, McGuinness, Madigan.

Critical revision of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Eirich, Anhorn, McGuinness, Christakis, Madigan.

Statistical analysis: Eirich, McGuinness, Madigan.

Obtained funding: Madigan.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Eirich, Anhorn, McGuinness, Christakis, Madigan.

Supervision: Eirich, Christakis, Madigan.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: None reported.

Funding/Support: This work was supported by the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council and the Canada Research Chairs program.

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Additional Contributions: Cheri Nickel, MLIS, University of Calgary, conducted the literature search for this project and received compensation.

References
1.
Browne  D, Thompson  DA, Madigan  S.  Digital media use in children: clinical vs scientific responsibilities.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(2):111-112. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4559 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Odgers  CL, Jensen  MR.  Annual research review: adolescent mental health in the digital age: facts, fears, and future directions.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2020;61(3):336-348. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13190 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Christakis  DA, Gilkerson  J, Richards  JA,  et al.  Audible television and decreased adult words, infant vocalizations, and conversational turns: a population-based study.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(6):554-558. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.61 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Christakis  DA.  Interactive media use at younger than the age of 2 years: time to rethink the American Academy of Pediatrics guideline?   JAMA Pediatr. 2014;168(5):399-400. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.5081 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Linebarger  DL, Barr  R, Lapierre  MA, Piotrowski  JT.  Associations between parenting, media use, cumulative risk, and children’s executive functioning.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2014;35(6):367-377. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000069 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Nikkelen  SW, Valkenburg  PM, Huizinga  M, Bushman  BJ.  Media use and ADHD-related behaviors in children and adolescents: a meta-analysis.   Dev Psychol. 2014;50(9):2228-2241. doi:10.1037/a0037318 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kaye  L, Orben  A, Ellis  D, Hunter  S, Houghton  S.  The conceptual and methodological mayhem of “screen time.”   Int J Env Res Public Health. 2020;17(10):3661. doi:10.3390/ijerph17103661 Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Blum-Ross  A, Livingstone  S. The trouble with “screen time” rules. In: Mascheroni G, Jorge A, Ponte C (Eds.). Digital Parenting: The Challenges for Families in the Digital Age. International Clearinghouse on Children, Youth, and the Media; 2018:179-187.
9.
Ferguson  CJ, Beresin  E.  Social science’s curious war with pop culture and how it was lost: the media violence debate and the risks it holds for social science.   Prev Med. 2017;99:69-76. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2017.02.009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Elson  M, Ferguson  CJ, Gregerson  M,  et al  Do policy statements on media effects faithfully represent the science?   Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(1):12-25. doi:10.1177/2515245918811301 Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Cohn  LD, Becker  BJ.  How meta-analysis increases statistical power.   Psychol Methods. 2003;8(3):243-253. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.3.243 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
McBee  MT, Brand  RJ, Dixon  WE  Jr.  Challenging the link between early childhood television exposure and later attention problems: a multiverse approach.   Psychol Sci. 2021;32(4):496-518. doi:10.1177/0956797620971650 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Stiglic  N, Viner  RM.  Effects of screentime on the health and well-being of children and adolescents: a systematic review of reviews.   BMJ Open. 2019;9(1):e023191. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2018-023191 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
de Jong  E, Visscher  TLS, HiraSing  RA, Heymans  MW, Seidell  JC, Renders  CM.  Association between TV viewing, computer use and overweight, determinants and competing activities of screen time in 4- to 13-year-old children.   Int J Obes (Lond). 2013;37(1):47-53. doi:10.1038/ijo.2011.244 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Rideout  V, Hamel  E.  The Media Family: Electronic Media in the Lives of Infants, Toddlers, Preschoolers and Their Parents. Kaiser Family Foundation;2006.
16.
Neville  RD, McArthur  BA, Eirich  R, Lakes  KD, Madigan  S. Bidirectional associations between screen time and children's externalizing and internalizing behaviors. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2021;62(12):1475-1484. doi:10.1111/jcpp.13425
17.
Zahn-Waxler  C, Shirtcliff  EA, Marceau  K.  Disorders of childhood and adolescence: gender and psychopathology.   Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2008;4:275-303. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091358 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Bado  P, Schafer  J, Simioni  AR,  et al.  Screen time and psychopathology: investigating directionality using cross-lagged panel models.   Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Published online November 1, 2020. doi:10.1007/s00787-020-01675-5 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Rideout  V, Robb MB. The common sense census: media use by kids age zero to eight. Common Sense Media; 2020. Accessed January 20, 2021. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2020_zero_to_eight_census_final_web.pdf
20.
Carson  V, Spence  JC, Cutumisu  N, Cargill  L.  Association between neighborhood socioeconomic status and screen time among pre-school children: a cross-sectional study.   BMC Public Health. 2010;10:367. doi:10.1186/1471-2458-10-367 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Tandon  PS, Zhou  C, Sallis  JF, Cain  KL, Frank  LD, Saelens  BE.  Home environment relationships with children’s physical activity, sedentary time, and screen time by socioeconomic status.   Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:88. doi:10.1186/1479-5868-9-88 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Rideout  V, Robb  MB. The common sense census: media use by tweens and teens. Common Sense Media; 2019. Accessed January 10, 2021. https://www.commonsensemedia.org/sites/default/files/uploads/research/2019-census-8-to-18-full-report-updated.pdf
23.
Ramirez  ER, Norman  GJ, Rosenberg  DE,  et al.  Adolescent screen time and rules to limit screen time in the home.   J Adolesc Health. 2011;48(4):379-385. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.07.013 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Marceau  K, Laurent  HK, Neiderhiser  JM,  et al.  Combined influences of genes, prenatal environment, cortisol, and parenting on the development of children’s internalizing versus externalizing problems.   Behav Genet. 2015;45(3):268-282. doi:10.1007/s10519-014-9689-z PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Achenbach  TM.  The classification of children’s psychiatric symptoms: a factor-analytic study.   Psychol Monogr. 1966;80(7):1-37. doi:10.1037/h0093906 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
McArthur  BA, Racine  N, Browne  D, McDonald  S, Tough  S, Madigan  S. Recreational screen time before and during COVID-19 in school-aged children. Acta Paediatr. Published online June 15, 2021. doi:10.1111/apa.15966
27.
Racine  N, McArthur  BA, Cooke  JE, Eirich  R, Zhu  J, Madigan  S.  Global prevalence of depressive and anxiety symptoms in children and adolescents during COVID-19: a meta-analysis.   JAMA Pediatr. 2021;175(11):1142-1150. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.2482 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
28.
National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. Study quality assessment tools: quality assessment tool for observational cohort and cross-sectional studies. National Institutes of Health. Accessed February 10, 2021. https://www.nhlbi.nih.gov/health-topics/study-quality-assessment-tools
29.
Cicchetti  DV. Guidelines, criteria, and rules of thumb for evaluating normed and standardized assessment instruments in psychology. Psychol Assess. 1994;6(4):284–290.
30.
Caldwell  DM, Davies  SR, Hetrick  SE,  et al.  School-based interventions to prevent anxiety and depression in children and young people: a systematic review and network meta-analysis.   Lancet Psychiatry. 2019;6(12):1011-1020. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(19)30403-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Brumariu  LE, Madigan  S, Giuseppone  KR, Movahed Abtahi  M, Kerns  KA.  The Security Scale as a measure of attachment: meta-analytic evidence of validity.   Attach Hum Dev. 2018;20(6):600-625. doi:10.1080/14616734.2018.1433217 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Witzel  TC, Eshun-Wilson  I, Jamil  MS,  et al.  Comparing the effects of HIV self-testing to standard HIV testing for key populations: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   BMC Med. 2020;18(1):381. doi:10.1186/s12916-020-01835-z PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Rosenthal  R.  Writing meta-analytic reviews.   Psychol Bull. 1995;118(2):183-192. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.118.2.183 Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Peterson  RA, Brown  SP.  On the use of beta coefficients in meta-analysis.   J Appl Psychol. 2005;90(1):175-181. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.90.1.175 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Borenstein  M, Hedges  L, Higgins  JPT, Rothstein  HR.  Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 3.0. Biostat; 2013.
36.
DerSimonian  R, Laird  N.  Meta-analysis in clinical trials.   Control Clin Trials. 1986;7(3):177-188. doi:10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Funder  DC, Ozer  DJ.  Evaluating effect size in psychological research: sense and nonsense.   Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2019;2(2):156-168. doi:10.1177/2515245919847202 Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Egger  M, Davey Smith  G, Schneider  M, Minder  C.  Bias in meta-analysis detected by a simple, graphical test.   BMJ. 1997;315(7109):629-634. doi:10.1136/bmj.315.7109.629 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Borenstein  M.  Introduction to Meta-Analysis. John Wiley & Sons; 2009. doi:10.1002/9780470743386
40.
Huesmann  LR.  The impact of electronic media violence: scientific theory and research.   J Adolesc Health. 2007;41(6)(suppl 1):S6-S13. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.09.005 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Christakis  DA, Zimmerman  FJ, DiGiuseppe  DL, McCarty  CA.  Early television exposure and subsequent attentional problems in children.   Pediatrics. 2004;113(4):708-713. doi:10.1542/peds.113.4.708 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Liu  M, Wu  L, Yao  S.  Dose-response association of screen time-based sedentary behaviour in children and adolescents and depression: a meta-analysis of observational studies.   Br J Sports Med. 2016;50(20):1252-1258. doi:10.1136/bjsports-2015-095084 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Guerrero  MD, Barnes  JD, Chaput  J-P, Tremblay  MS.  Screen time and problem behaviors in children: exploring the mediating role of sleep duration.   Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2019;16(1):105. doi:10.1186/s12966-019-0862-x PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Al-Ali  NM, Yaghy  HS, Shattnawi  KK, Al-Shdayfat  NM.  Parents’ knowledge and beliefs about the impact of exposure to media violence on children’s aggression.   Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2018;39(7):592-599. doi:10.1080/01612840.2017.1422201 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Allen  MS, Vella  SA.  Screen-based sedentary behaviour and psychosocial well-being in childhood: cross-sectional and longitudinal associations.   Ment Health Phys Act. 2015;9:41-47. doi:10.1016/j.mhpa.2015.10.002 Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Amawi  SO, Subki  AH, Khatib  HA,  et al.  Use of electronic entertainment and communication devices among a Saudi pediatric population: cross-sectional study.   Interact J Med Res. 2018;7(2):e13. doi:10.2196/ijmr.9103 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Barlett  ND, Gentile  DA, Barlett  CP, Eisenmann  JC, Walsh  DA.  Sleep as a mediator of screen time effects on US children’s health outcomes.   J Child Media. 2012;6(1):37-50. doi:10.1080/17482798.2011.633404 Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Byun  Y-H, Ha  M, Kwon  H-J,  et al.  Mobile phone use, blood lead levels, and attention deficit hyperactivity symptoms in children: a longitudinal study.   PLoS One. 2013;8(3):e59742. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0059742 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Cao  H, Yan  S, Gu  C,  et al.  Prevalence of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and their associations with sleep schedules and sleep-related problems among preschoolers in mainland China.   BMC Pediatr. 2018;18(1):70. doi:10.1186/s12887-018-1022-1 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Chonchaiya  W, Sirachairat  C, Vijakkhana  N, Wilaisakditipakorn  T, Pruksananonda  C.  Elevated background TV exposure over time increases behavioural scores of 18-month-old toddlers.   Acta Paediatr. 2015;104(10):1039-1046. doi:10.1111/apa.13067 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Christodoulou  G, Majmundar  A, Chou  C-P, Pentz  MA.  Anhedonia, screen time, and substance use in early adolescents: a longitudinal mediation analysis.   J Adolesc. 2020;78(C):24-32. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2019.11.007 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
52.
Coker  TR, Elliott  MN, Schwebel  DC,  et al.  Media violence exposure and physical aggression in fifth-grade children.   Acad Pediatr. 2015;15(1):82-88. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2014.09.008 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Corkin  MT, Peterson  ER, Henderson  AME, Waldie  KE, Reese  E, Morton  SMB.  Preschool screen media exposure, executive functions and symptoms of inattention/hyperactivity.   J Appl Dev Psychol. 2021;73:101237. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2020.101237 Google ScholarCrossref
54.
Davison  LL.  Media Viewing and the Neurological Development of ADHD: Voluntary and Involuntary Processing. Dissertation. Claremont Graduate University; 2004.
55.
Ebenegger  V, Marques-Vidal  P-M, Munsch  S,  et al.  Relationship of hyperactivity/inattention with adiposity and lifestyle characteristics in preschool children.   J Child Neurol. 2012;27(7):852-858. doi:10.1177/0883073811428009 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Erdogan  A, Kiran  S, Aydogan  G,  et al  Behavioral correlates of television viewing time in a Turkish sample of preschool children.   Neurol Psychiatr Brain Res. 2006;13(4):225-230.Google Scholar
57.
Froiland  JM, Davison  ML.  Home literacy, television viewing, fidgeting and ADHD in young children.   Educational Psychology. 2016;36(8):1337-1353. doi:10.1080/01443410.2014.963031 Google ScholarCrossref
58.
Gentile  DA, Berch  ON, Choo  H, Khoo  A, Walsh  DA.  Bedroom media: one risk factor for development.   Dev Psychol. 2017;53(12):2340-2355. doi:10.1037/dev0000399 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Gialamas  A, Haag  DG, Mittinty  MN, Lynch  J.  Which time investments in the first 5 years of life matter most for children’s language and behavioural outcomes at school entry?   Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49(2):548-558. doi:10.1093/ije/dyz192 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
Guxens  M, Vermeulen  R, Steenkamer  I,  et al.  Radiofrequency electromagnetic fields, screen time, and emotional and behavioural problems in 5-year-old children.   Int J Hyg Environ Health. 2019;222(2):188-194. doi:10.1016/j.ijheh.2018.09.006 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Hastings  EC, Karas  TL, Winsler  A, Way  E, Madigan  A, Tyler  S.  Young children’s video/computer game use: relations with school performance and behavior.   Issues Ment Health Nurs. 2009;30(10):638-649. doi:10.1080/01612840903050414 PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Hefner  HL.  Risks of Television Viewing Among Children With and Without the ADHD Diagnosis. Dissertation. Walden University; 2013.
63.
Hill  MM, Gangi  D, Miller  M, Rafi  SM, Ozonoff  S.  Screen time in 36-month-olds at increased likelihood for ASD and ADHD.  Infant Behav Dev. 2020;61:101484.
64.
Holton  KF, Nigg  JT.  The association of lifestyle factors and ADHD in children.   J Atten Disord. 2020;24(11):1511-1520. doi:10.1177/1087054716646452 PubMedGoogle Scholar
65.
Hong  GCC, Conduit  R, Wong  J, Di Benedetto  M, Lee  E.  Diet, physical activity, and screen time to sleep better: multiple mediation analysis of lifestyle factors in school-aged children with and without attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.   J Atten Disord. Published July 14, 2020.PubMedGoogle Scholar
66.
Hosokawa  R, Katsura  T.  Association between mobile technology use and child adjustment in early elementary school age.   PLoS One. 2018;13(7):e0199959. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0199959 PubMedGoogle Scholar
67.
Huesmann  LR, Eron  LD.  Cognitive processes and the persistence of aggressive behavior.   Aggress Behav. 1984;10(3):243-251. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1984)10:3<243::AID-AB2480100308>3.0.CO;2-6 Google Scholar
68.
Jia  S, Wang  L, Shi  Y, Li  P.  Family risk factors associated with aggressive behavior in Chinese preschool children.   J Pediatr Nurs. 2016;31(6):e367-e374. doi:10.1016/j.pedn.2016.08.001 PubMedGoogle Scholar
69.
Kahn  M, Schnabel  O, Gradisar  M,  et al. Sleep, screen time and behaviour problems in preschool children: an actigraphy study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. Published online October 1, 2020.
70.
Kuhhirt  M, Klein  M. Parental education, television exposure, and children's early cognitive, language and behavioral development. Soc Sci Res. 2020;86:102391.
71.
Linebarger  DL.  Contextualizing video game play: the moderating effects of cumulative risk and parenting styles on the relations among video game exposure and problem behaviors.   Psychol Pop Media Cult. 2015;4(4):375-396. doi:10.1037/ppm0000069 Google Scholar
72.
Levelink  B, van der Vlegel  M, Mommers  M,  et al.  The longitudinal relationship between screen time, sleep and a diagnosis of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in childhood.   J Atten Disord. 2020.PubMedGoogle Scholar
73.
Lin  H-P, Chen  K-L, Chou  W,  et al.  Prolonged touch screen device usage is associated with emotional and behavioral problems, but not language delay, in toddlers.  Infant Behav Dev. 2020;58:101424.
74.
Lobel  A, Engels  RCME, Stone  LL, Burk  WJ, Granic  I.  Video gaming and children’s psychosocial wellbeing: a longitudinal study.   J Youth Adolesc. 2017;46(4):884-897. doi:10.1007/s10964-017-0646-z PubMedGoogle Scholar
75.
Manganello  JA, Taylor  CA.  Television exposure as a risk factor for aggressive behavior among 3-year-old children.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2009;163(11):1037-1045. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.193 PubMedGoogle Scholar
76.
Martin  A, Razza  R, Brooks-Gunn  J.  Specifying the links between household chaos and preschool children’s development.   Early Child Dev Care. 2012;182(10):1247-1263. doi:10.1080/03004430.2011.605522 PubMedGoogle Scholar
77.
Martins  N, Wilson  BJ.  Social aggression on television and its relationship to children’s aggression in the classroom.   Hum Commun Res. 2012;38(1):48-71. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.2011.01417.x Google Scholar
78.
McNeill  J, Howard  SJ, Vella  SA, Cliff  DP.  Longitudinal associations of electronic application use and media program viewing with cognitive and psychosocial development in preschoolers.   Acad Pediatr. 2019;19(5):520-528. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2019.02.010 PubMedGoogle Scholar
79.
Miller  CJ, Marks  DJ, Miller  SR,  et al.  Brief report: television viewing and risk for attention problems in preschool children.   J Pediatr Psychol. 2007;32(4):448-452. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsl035 PubMedGoogle Scholar
80.
Miller  LE, Grabell  A, Thomas  A, Bermann  E, Graham-Bermann  SA.  The associations between community violence, television violence, intimate partner violence, parent–child aggression, and aggression in sibling relationships of a sample of preschoolers.   Psychol Violence. 2012;2(2):165-178. doi:10.1037/a0027254 Google Scholar
81.
Mistry  KB, Minkovitz  CS, Strobino  DM, Borzekowski  DLG.  Children’s television exposure and behavioral and social outcomes at 5.5 years: does timing of exposure matter?   Pediatrics. 2007;120(4):762-769. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3573 PubMedGoogle Scholar
82.
Moyer  VS.  The Role of User Motivations in Moderating the Relation Between Video Game Playing and Children's Adjustment. Dissertation. Bowling Green State University; 2008.
83.
Mundy  LK, Canterford  L, Olds  T, Allen  NB, Patton  GC.  The association between electronic media and emotional and behavioral problems in late childhood.   Acad Pediatr. 2017;17(6):620-624. doi:10.1016/j.acap.2016.12.014 PubMedGoogle Scholar
84.
Niiranen  J, Kiviruusu  O, Vornanen  R, Saarenpää-Heikkilä  O, Paavonen  EJ.  High-dose electronic media use in five-year-olds and its association with their psychosocial symptoms: a cohort study.   BMJ Open. 2021;11(3):e040848. doi:10.1136/bmjopen-2020-040848 PubMedGoogle Scholar
85.
Nikkelen  SWC, Vossen  HGM, Valkenburg  PM.  Children’s television viewing and ADHD-related behaviors: evidence from The Netherlands.   J Child Media. 2015;9(4):399-418. doi:10.1080/17482798.2015.1088872 Google Scholar
86.
Obel  C, Henriksen  TB, Dalsgaard  S,  et al.  Does children’s watching of television cause attention problems? retesting the hypothesis in a Danish cohort.   Pediatrics. 2004;114(5):1372-1373. doi:10.1542/peds.2004-0954 PubMedGoogle Scholar
87.
Özmert  E, Toyran  M, Yurdakök  K.  Behavioral correlates of television viewing in primary school children evaluated by the child behavior checklist.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2002;156(9):910-914. doi:10.1001/archpedi.156.9.910 PubMedGoogle Scholar
88.
Parkes  A, Sweeting  H, Wight  D, Henderson  M.  Do television and electronic games predict children’s psychosocial adjustment? Longitudinal research using the UK Millennium Cohort Study.   Arch Dis Child. 2013;98(5):341-348. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2011-301508 PubMedGoogle Scholar
89.
Patel  C. Co-occurrence and Non-overlap Among Behavioral and Health-Related Problems in Preschool Children. Dissertation. University of South Carolina; 2019.
90.
Paulus  FW, Sinzig  J, Mayer  H, Weber  M, von Gontard  A.  Computer gaming disorder and ADHD in young children—a population-based study.   Int J Ment Health Addict. 2018;16(5):1193-1207. doi:10.1007/s11469-017-9841-0 Google Scholar
91.
Peralta  GP, Forns  J, García de la Hera  M,  et al.  Sleeping, TV, cognitively stimulating activities, physical activity, and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptom incidence in children: a prospective study.   J Dev Behav Pediatr. 2018;39(3):192-199. doi:10.1097/DBP.0000000000000539 PubMedGoogle Scholar
92.
Poulain  T, Vogel  M, Neef  M,  et al.  Reciprocal associations between electronic media use and behavioral difficulties in preschoolers.   Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2018;15(4):814. doi:10.3390/ijerph15040814 PubMedGoogle Scholar
93.
Reynolds  JE. An Assessment of Variables Associated With Television Viewing and Their Influence on Aggressive Behavior of Third Grade Children. Dissertation. Southern Illinois University. ProQuest Dissertations; 1978.
94.
Rosen  LD, Lim  AF, Felt  J,  et al.  Media and technology use predicts ill-being among children, preteens and teenagers independent of the negative health impacts of exercise and eating habits.   Comput Human Behav. 2014;35:364-375. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2014.01.036 PubMedGoogle Scholar
95.
Sanders  W, Parent  J, Forehand  R, Sullivan  ADW, Jones  DJ.  Parental perceptions of technology and technology-focused parenting: associations with youth screen time.   J Appl Dev Psychol. 2016;44:28-38. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2016.02.005 PubMedGoogle Scholar
96.
Schaefer  WG.  The Effects of Television Viewing on the Academic Performance of Elementary School Children With Attention Deficit Disorder. Dissertation. Western Michigan University; 1991.
97.
Schmiedeler  S, Niklas  F, Schneider  W.  Symptoms of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and home learning environment (HLE): findings from a longitudinal study.   Eur J Psychol Educ. 2014;29(3):467-482. doi:10.1007/s10212-013-0208-z Google Scholar
98.
Séguin  D, Klimek  V.  Just five more minutes please: electronic media use, sleep and behaviour in young children.   Early Child Dev Care. 2016;186(6):981-1000. doi:10.1080/03004430.2015.1071528 Google Scholar
99.
Sheehan  PW.  Age trends and the correlates of children’s television viewing.   Aust J Psychol. 1983;35(3):417-431. doi:10.1080/00049538308258753 Google Scholar
100.
Shmukler  D.  A descriptive analysis of television viewing in South African Preschoolers and its relationship to their spontaneous play.   S Afr J Psychol. 1981;11(3):106-110. doi:10.1177/008124638101100303 Google Scholar
101.
Stenseng  F, Hygen  BW, Wichstrøm  L.  Time spent gaming and psychiatric symptoms in childhood: cross-sectional associations and longitudinal effects.   Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;29(6):839-847. doi:10.1007/s00787-019-01398-2 PubMedGoogle Scholar
102.
Stevens  T, Mulsow  M.  There is no meaningful relationship between television exposure and symptoms of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.   Pediatrics. 2006;117(3):665-672. doi:10.1542/peds.2005-0863 PubMedGoogle Scholar
103.
Sugawara  M, Matsumoto  S, Murohashi  H, Sakai  A, Isshiki  N.  Trajectories of early television contact in Japan: relationship with preschoolers’ externalizing problems.   J Child Media. 2015;9(4):453-471. doi:10.1080/17482798.2015.1089298 Google Scholar
104.
Swing  EL, Gentile  DA, Anderson  CA, Walsh  DA.  Television and video game exposure and the development of attention problems.   Pediatrics. 2010;126(2):214-221. doi:10.1542/peds.2009-1508 PubMedGoogle Scholar
105.
Tamana  SK, Ezeugwu  V, Chikuma  J,  et al; CHILD study Investigators.  Screen-time is associated with inattention problems in preschoolers: results from the CHILD birth cohort study.   PLoS One. 2019;14(4):e0213995. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0213995 PubMedGoogle Scholar
106.
Tansriratanawong  S, Louthrenoo  O, Chonchaiya  W, Charnsil  C.  Screen viewing time and externalising problems in pre-school children in Northern Thailand.   J Child Adolesc Ment Health. 2017;29(3):245-252. doi:10.2989/17280583.2017.1409226 PubMedGoogle Scholar
107.
Teramoto  S, Soeda  A, Hayashi  Y, Saito  K, Urashima  M.  Problematic behaviours of 3-year-old children in Japan: relationship with socioeconomic and family backgrounds.   Early Hum Dev. 2005;81(6):563-569. doi:10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.12.011 PubMedGoogle Scholar
108.
Verlinden  M, Tiemeier  H, Hudziak  JJ,  et al.  Television viewing and externalizing problems in preschool children: the Generation R Study.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(10):919-925. doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2012.653 PubMedGoogle Scholar
109.
Viemerö  V, Paajanen  S.  The role of fantasies and dreams in the TV viewing—aggression relationship.   Aggress Behav. 1992;18(2):109-116. doi:10.1002/1098-2337(1992)18:2<109::AID-AB2480180204>3.0.CO;2-1 Google Scholar
110.
Wan  MW, Fitch-Bunce  C, Heron  K, Lester  E.  Infant screen media usage and social-emotional functioning.  Infant Behav Dev. 2021;62:101509. doi:10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101509
111.
Wimbarti  S.  Children’s Aggression in Indonesia: The Effects of Culture, Familial Factors, Peers, TV Violence Viewing, and Temperament. Dissertation. University of Southern California; 2002.
112.
Woodfield  DL. Mass Media Viewing Habits and Toleration of Real-life Aggression. Dissertation. In: ProQuest Dissertations; 1987.
113.
Wu  X, Kirk  SFL, Ohinmaa  A, Veugelers  P.  Health behaviours, body weight and self-esteem among grade five students in Canada.   Springerplus. 2016;5(1):1099. doi:10.1186/s40064-016-2744-x PubMedGoogle Scholar
114.
Wu  X, Tao  S, Rutayisire  E, Chen  Y, Huang  K, Tao  F.  The relationship between screen time, nighttime sleep duration, and behavioural problems in preschool children in China.   Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;26(5):541-548. doi:10.1007/s00787-016-0912-8 PubMedGoogle Scholar
115.
Xie  G, Deng  Q, Cao  J, Chang  Q.  Digital screen time and its effect on preschoolers’ behavior in China: results from a cross-sectional study.   Ital J Pediatr. 2020;46(1):9. doi:10.1186/s13052-020-0776-x PubMedGoogle Scholar
116.
Yousef  S, Eapen  V, Zoubeidi  T, Mabrouk  A.  Behavioral correlation with television watching and videogame playing among children in the United Arab Emirates.   Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. 2014;18(3):203-207. doi:10.3109/13651501.2013.874442 PubMedGoogle Scholar
117.
Zimmerman  FJ, Christakis  DA.  Associations between content types of early media exposure and subsequent attentional problems.   Pediatrics. 2007;120(5):986-992. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3322 PubMedGoogle Scholar
118.
Zimmerman  FJ, Glew  GM, Christakis  DA, Katon  W.  Early cognitive stimulation, emotional support, and television watching as predictors of subsequent bullying among grade-school children.   Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159(4):384-388. doi:10.1001/archpedi.159.4.384 PubMedGoogle Scholar
119.
Brubaker  MM.  Exploring the Relationship Between Social Support and Social Behavior: Moderating Effects of Video Game Use. Dissertation. Fielding Graduate University. ProQuest Dissertations Publishing; 2020.
120.
Cooper  NS. The Identification of Psychological and Social Correlates of internet Use in Children and Teenagers. Dissertation. Alliant International University; 2004.
121.
Dunkeld  C, Wright  ML, Banerjee  RA, Easterbrook  MJ, Slade  L.  Television exposure, consumer culture values, and lower well-being among preadolescent children: the mediating role of consumer-focused coping strategies.   Br J Soc Psychol. 2020;59(1):26-48. doi:10.1111/bjso.12325 PubMedGoogle Scholar
122.
Lam  MW, Burgoon  E, Jin  C.  5.37 Associations of screen time and depression in children in a US nationally representative sample.   J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58 (10)(suppl):S257. doi:10.1016/j.jaac.2019.08.351 Google Scholar
123.
Lin  S-Y, Eaton  NR, Schleider  JL.  Unpacking associations between mood symptoms and screen time in preadolescents: a network analysis.   J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2020;48(12):1635-1647. doi:10.1007/s10802-020-00703-x PubMedGoogle Scholar
124.
Zhang  J, Liu  M-W, Yu  H-J,  et al. Associations of health-risk behaviors with mental health among Chinese children. Psychol Health Med. Published December 9, 2020.
125.
Zhu  X, Haegele  JA, Healy  S.  Movement and mental health: behavioral correlates of anxiety and depression among children of 6-17 years old in the US.   Ment Health Phys Act. 2019;16:60-65. doi:10.1016/j.mhpa.2019.04.002 Google Scholar
126.
Madigan  S, McArthur  BA, Anhorn  C, Eirich  R, Christakis  DA.  Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(7):665-675. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2020.0327 PubMedGoogle Scholar
127.
Adelantado-Renau  M, Moliner-Urdiales  D, Cavero-Redondo  I, Beltran-Valls  MR, Martínez-Vizcaíno  V, Álvarez-Bueno  C.  Association between screen media use and academic performance among children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis.   JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(11):1058-1067. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.3176 PubMedGoogle Scholar
128.
Hentges  RF, Devereux  C, Graham  SA, Madigan  S.  Child language difficulties and internalizing and externalizing symptoms: a meta-analysis.   Child Dev. 2021;92(4):e691-e715. doi:10.1111/cdev.13540 PubMedGoogle Scholar
129.
Peverill  M, Dirks  MA, Narvaja  T, Herts  KL, Comer  JS, McLaughlin  KA.  Socioeconomic status and child psychopathology in the United States: a meta-analysis of population-based studies.   Clin Psychol Rev. 2021;83:101933. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101933 PubMedGoogle Scholar
130.
Rutledge  T, Loh  C.  Effect sizes and statistical testing in the determination of clinical significance in behavioral medicine research.   Ann Behav Med. 2004;27(2):138-145. doi:10.1207/s15324796abm2702_9 PubMedGoogle Scholar
131.
McArthur  BA, Volkova  V, Tomopoulos  S, Madigan  S. Global prevalence of meeting screen time guidelines among children 5 years and younger: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. Published online February 14, 2022. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.6386
132.
Groh  AM, Roisman  GI, van Ijzendoorn  MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg  MJ, Fearon  RP.  The significance of insecure and disorganized attachment for children’s internalizing symptoms: a meta-analytic study.   Child Dev. 2012;83(2):591-610. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01711.x PubMedGoogle Scholar
133.
Hoyos Cillero  I, Jago  R.  Sociodemographic and home environment predictors of screen viewing among Spanish school children.   J Public Health (Oxf). 2011;33(3):392-402. doi:10.1093/pubmed/fdq087 PubMedGoogle Scholar
134.
Mesman  J, Bongers  IL, Koot  HM.  Preschool developmental pathways to preadolescent internalizing and externalizing problems.   J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(5):679-689. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00763 PubMedGoogle Scholar
135.
Tomopoulos  S, Dreyer  BP, Valdez  P,  et al.  Media content and externalizing behaviors in Latino toddlers.   Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7(3):232-238. doi:10.1016/j.ambp.2007.02.004 PubMedGoogle Scholar
136.
Funk  JB, Baldacci  HB, Pasold  T, Baumgardner  J.  Violence exposure in real-life, video games, television, movies, and the internet: is there desensitization?   J Adolesc. 2004;27(1):23-39. doi:10.1016/j.adolescence.2003.10.005 PubMedGoogle Scholar
137.
Bandura  A.  Social Learning Theory. Prentice-Hall; 1977.
138.
World Health Organization. Guidelines on Physical Activity, Sedentary Behaviour and Sleep for Children Under 5 Years of Age. World Health Organization; 2019. Accessed February 9, 2022. https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/311664
139.
Madigan  S, Racine  N, Tough  S.  Prevalence of preschoolers meeting vs exceeding screen time guidelines.   JAMA Pediatr. 2020;174(1):93-95. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.4495 PubMedGoogle Scholar
140.
Sigman  A.  Time for a view on screen time.   Arch Dis Child. 2012;97(11):935-942. doi:10.1136/archdischild-2012-302196 PubMedGoogle Scholar
141.
Harvey  DL, Milton  K, Jones  AP, Atkin  AJ.  International trends in screen-based behaviours from 2012 to 2019.   Prev Med. 2022;154:106909. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2021.106909 PubMedGoogle Scholar
142.
Paulo  MS, Nauman  J, Abdulle  A,  et al.  Results from the United Arab Emirates’ 2018 report card on physical activity for children and youth.   J Phys Act Health. 2018;15(S2):S419-S421. doi:10.1123/jpah.2018-0543 PubMedGoogle Scholar
143.
Ernest  JM, Causey  C, Newton  AB, Sharkins  K, Summerlin  J, Albaiz  N.  Extending the global dialogue about media, technology, screen time, and young children.   Childhood Education. 2014;90(3):182-191. doi:10.1080/00094056.2014.910046 Google Scholar
144.
De Los Reyes  A, Kazdin  AE.  Informant discrepancies in the assessment of childhood psychopathology: a critical review, theoretical framework, and recommendations for further study.   Psychol Bull. 2005;131(4):483-509. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.4.483 PubMedGoogle Scholar
145.
De Los Reyes  A.  Introduction to the special section: more than measurement error: discovering meaning behind informant discrepancies in clinical assessments of children and adolescents.   J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2011;40(1):1-9. doi:10.1080/15374416.2011.533405 PubMedGoogle Scholar
146.
Achenbach  TM, Edelbrock  CS.  The classification of child psychopathology: a review and analysis of empirical efforts.   Psychol Bull. 1978;85(6):1275-1301. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.85.6.1275 PubMedGoogle Scholar
147.
Greenbaum  PE, Dedrick  RF.  Hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18.   Psychol Assess. 1998;10(2):149-155. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.10.2.149 Google Scholar
148.
Radesky  JS, Weeks  HM, Ball  R,  et al.  Young children’s use of smartphones and tablets.   Pediatrics. 2020;146(1):e20193518. doi:10.1542/peds.2019-3518 PubMedGoogle Scholar
149.
Ioannidis  JP.  Why most discovered true associations are inflated.   Epidemiology. 2008;19(5):640-648. doi:10.1097/EDE.0b013e31818131e7 PubMedGoogle Scholar
150.
Schooler  J. Unpublished results hide the decline effect. Nature. 2011;470(7335):437.
151.
de Bruin  A, Della Sala  S.  The decline effect: how initially strong results tend to decrease over time.   Cortex. 2015;73:375-377. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.025 PubMedGoogle Scholar
152.
Chen  W, Adler  JL.  Assessment of screen exposure in young children, 1997 to 2014.   JAMA Pediatr. 2019;173(4):391-393. doi:10.1001/jamapediatrics.2018.5546 PubMedGoogle Scholar
153.
van IJzendoorn  MH, Bakermans-Kranenburg  MJ.  Differential susceptibility experiments: going beyond correlational evidence: comment on beyond mental health, differential susceptibility articles.   Dev Psychol. 2012;48(3):769-774. doi:10.1037/a0027536 PubMedGoogle Scholar
154.
Sudan  M, Olsen  J, Arah  OA, Obel  C, Kheifets  L.  Prospective cohort analysis of cellphone use and emotional and behavioural difficulties in children.   J Epidemiol Community Health. 2016;70(12):1207-1213. doi:10.1136/jech-2016-207419 PubMedGoogle Scholar
155.
Christakis  DA.  The challenges of defining and studying “digital addiction” in children.   JAMA. 2019;321(23):2277-2278. doi:10.1001/jama.2019.4690 PubMedGoogle Scholar
156.
Shah  RR, Fahey  NM, Soni  AV, Phatak  AG, Nimbalkar  SM.  Screen time usage among preschoolers aged 2-6 in rural Western India: a cross-sectional study.   J Family Med Prim Care. 2019;8(6):1999-2002. doi:10.4103/jfmpc.jfmpc_206_19 PubMedGoogle Scholar
157.
Tang  L, Darlington  G, Ma  DWL, Haines  J; Guelph Family Health Study.  Mothers’ and fathers’ media parenting practices associated with young children’s screen-time: a cross-sectional study.   BMC Obes. 2018;5(1):37. doi:10.1186/s40608-018-0214-4 PubMedGoogle Scholar
158.
Korhonen  L.  The good, the bad and the ugly of children´s screen time during the COVID-19 pandemic.   Acta Paediatr. 2021;110(10):2671-2672. doi:10.1111/apa.16012 PubMedGoogle Scholar
159.
Xiang  M, Zhang  Z, Kuwahara  K.  Impact of COVID-19 pandemic on children and adolescents’ lifestyle behavior larger than expected.   Prog Cardiovasc Dis. 2020;63(4):531-532. doi:10.1016/j.pcad.2020.04.013 PubMedGoogle Scholar
160.
Hamaker  EL, Kuiper  RM, Grasman  RPPP.  A critique of the cross-lagged panel model.   Psychol Methods. 2015;20(1):102-116. doi:10.1037/a0038889 PubMedGoogle Scholar
×