[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Mean scores on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) at baseline, after 6 treatments, within 2 days of the last electroconvulsive therapy treatment, and 1 week after the last treatment in the randomized phase for the 4 groups (n = 20 for each group).

Mean scores on the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD) at baseline, after 6 treatments, within 2 days of the last electroconvulsive therapy treatment, and 1 week after the last treatment in the randomized phase for the 4 groups (n = 20 for each group).

Figure 2. 
Initial and final response rates for the 4 treatment groups (n=20 for each group) as a function of whether patients did not respond to an adequate trial prior to either right unilateral (RUL) or bilateral (BL) electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) (medication resistant) or did not receive an adequate trial (not resistant).

Initial and final response rates for the 4 treatment groups (n=20 for each group) as a function of whether patients did not respond to an adequate trial prior to either right unilateral (RUL) or bilateral (BL) electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) (medication resistant) or did not receive an adequate trial (not resistant).

Figure 3. 
Kaplan-Meier estimates of the proportion of patients who remained well after electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), for patients classified as medication resistant (n=34), and for patients who did not have an adequate medication trial prior to ECT (n=28).

Kaplan-Meier estimates of the proportion of patients who remained well after electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), for patients classified as medication resistant (n=34), and for patients who did not have an adequate medication trial prior to ECT (n=28).

Table 1. 
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Sample*
Demographic and Clinical Characteristics of the Sample*
Table 2. 
Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) Parameters*
Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) Parameters*
Table 3. 
Initial and Final Clinical Responses*
Initial and Final Clinical Responses*
Table 4. 
Continuation and Maintenance Treatments Received During Follow-up*
Continuation and Maintenance Treatments Received During Follow-up*
Table 5. 
Effects of Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) on Recovery of Orientation and Memory Functions Assessed Immediately After Seizure Termination at Each Treatment*
Effects of Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) on Recovery of Orientation and Memory Functions Assessed Immediately After Seizure Termination at Each Treatment*
Table 6. 
Short-term Effects of Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) on Cognitive Measures*
Short-term Effects of Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) on Cognitive Measures*
1.
Weiner  RDFink  MHammersley  DMoench  LSackeim  HASmall  IAmerican Psychiatric Association, The Practice of ECT: Recommendations for Treatment, Training and Privileging.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1990;
2.
Sackeim  HAPortnoy  SNeeley  PSteif  BLDecina  PMalitz  S Cognitive consequences of low-dosage electroconvulsive therapy.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986;462326- 340Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Sackeim  HA The cognitive effects of electroconvulsive therapy. Moos  WHGamzu  ERThal  LJeds Cognitive Disorders: Pathophysiology and Treatment New York, NY Marcel Dekker1992;183- 228Google Scholar
4.
Weiner  RDRogers  HJDavidson  JRSquire  LR Effects of stimulus parameters on cognitive side effects.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986;462315- 325Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Sackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPKiersky  JEFitzsimons  LMoody  BJMcElhiney  MCColeman  EASettembrino  JM Effects of stimulus intensity and electrode placement on the efficacy and cognitive effects of electroconvulsive therapy.  N Engl J Med. 1993;328839- 846Google ScholarCrossref
6.
McElhiney  MCMoody  BJSteif  BLPrudic  JDevanand  DPNobler  MSSackeim  HA Autobiographical memory and mood: effects of electroconvulsive therapy.  Neuropsychology. 1995;9501- 517Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Strömgren  LS Is bilateral ECT ever indicated?  Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1984;69484- 490Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Ottosson  JO Is unilateral nondominant ECT as efficient as bilateral ECT? a new look at the evidence.  Convuls Ther. 1991;7190- 200Google Scholar
9.
Abrams  R Electroconvulsive Therapy. 3rd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1997;
10.
d'Elia  GRaotma  H Is unilateral ECT less effective than bilateral ECT?  Br J Psychiatry. 1975;12683- 89Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Abrams  R Is unilateral electroconvulsive therapy really the treatment of choice in endogenous depression?  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986;46250- 55Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Sackeim  HA Optimizing unilateral electroconvulsive therapy.  Convulsive Ther. 1991;7201- 212Google Scholar
13.
Farah  AMcCall  WV Electroconvulsive therapy stimulus dosing: a survey of contemporary practices.  Convulsive Ther. 1993;990- 94Google Scholar
14.
Sackeim  HADecina  PKanzler  MKerr  BMalitz  S Effects of electrode placement on the efficacy of titrated, low-dose ECT.  Am J Psychiatry. 1987;1441449- 1455Google Scholar
15.
Abrams  RSwartz  CMVedak  C Antidepressant effects of high-dose right unilateral electroconvulsive therapy.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48746- 748Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Krystal  ADCoffey  CEWeiner  RDHolsinger  T Changes in seizure threshold over the course of electroconvulsive therapy affect therapeutic response and are detected by ictal EEG ratings.  J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;10178- 186Google Scholar
17.
Letemendia  FJDelva  NJRodenburg  MLawson  JSInglis  JWaldron  JJLywood  DW Therapeutic advantage of bifrontal electrode placement in ECT.  Psychol Med. 1993;23349- 360Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Ottosson  JO Experimental studies of the mode of action of electroconvulsive therapy.  Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1960;1451- 141Google Scholar
19.
Fink  M Convulsive Therapy: Theory and Practice.  New York, NY Raven Press1979;
20.
Not Available, NIH Consensus Conference: electroconvulsive therapy.  JAMA. 1985;2542103- 2108Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Endicott  JSpitzer  RL A diagnostic interview: the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35837- 844Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Spitzer  RLEndicott  JRobins  E Research diagnostic criteria: rationale and reliability.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35773- 782Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Hamilton  M Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness.  Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6278- 296Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Rackowski  DKalat  JWNebes  R Reliability and validity of some handedness questionnaire items.  Neuropsychologia. 1976;8523- 526Google Scholar
25.
Sackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPDecina  PKerr  BMalitz  S The impact of medication resistance and continuation pharmacotherapy on relapse following response to electroconvulsive therapy in major depression.  J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1990;1096- 104Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Prudic  JHaskett  RFMulsant  BMalone  KMPettinati  HMStephans  SGreenburg  RRifas  SLSackeim  HA Resistance to antidepressant medications and short-term clinical response to ECT.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153985- 992Google Scholar
27.
Fleiss  JL The Design and Analysis of Clinical Experiments.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1986;
28.
d'Elia  G Unilateral electroconvulsive therapy.  Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1970;2151- 98Google Scholar
29.
Sackeim  HDecina  PProhovnik  IMalitz  S Seizure threshold in electroconvulsive therapy: effects of sex, age, electrode placement, and number of treatments.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44355- 360Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Guy  W ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology.  Washington, DC National Institute of Health1976;Dept of Health, Education, and Welfare publication 76-338
31.
Prudic  JSackeim  HADevanand  DP Medication resistance and clinical response to electroconvulsive therapy.  Psychiatry Res. 1990;31287- 96Google ScholarCrossref
32.
Sobin  CSackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPMoody  BJMcElhiney  MC Predictors of retrograde amnesia following ECT.  Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152995- 1001Google Scholar
33.
Mayeux  RStern  YRosen  JLeventhal  J Depression, intellectual impairment, and Parkinson disease.  Neurology. 1981;31645- 650Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR "Mini-Mental state": a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients the clinician.  J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12189- 198Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Stern  YSano  MPauson  JMayeux  R Modified Mini-Mental Status Examination: validity and reliability.  Neurology. 1987;37(suppl 1)S179Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Sackeim  HAProhovnik  IMoeller  JRMayeux  RStern  YDevanand  DP Regional cerebral blood flow in mood disorders, II: comparison of major depression and Alzheimer's disease.  J Nucl Med. 1993;341090- 1101Google Scholar
37.
Rey  A L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique.  Arch Psychol. 1941;28286- 340Google Scholar
38.
Lezak  MD Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1995;
39.
Buschke  H Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning.  J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 1973;12543- 550Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Hannay  HJLevin  HS Selective reminding test: an examination of the equivalence of four forms.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1985;7251- 263Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Steif  BLSackeim  HAPortnoy  SDecina  PMalitz  S Effects of depression and ECT on anterograde memory.  Biol Psychiatry. 1986;21921- 930Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Randt  CTBrown  EROsbourne  DP A memory test for longitudinal measurement of mild to moderate deficits.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2184- 194Google Scholar
43.
Randt  CTBrown  RE Randt Memory Test.  Bayport, NY Life Science1983;
44.
Goldberg  EBarnett  J The Goldberg-Barnett Remote Memory Questionnaire.  New York, NY Albert Einstein Medical College1985;
45.
Squire  LRWetzel  CDSlater  PC Memory complaint after electroconvulsive therapy: assessment with a new self-rating instrument.  Biol Psychiatry. 1979;14791- 801Google Scholar
46.
McCall  WVShelp  FEWeiner  RDAustin  SNorris  J Convulsive threshold differences in right unilateral and bilateral ECT.  Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34606- 611Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Coffey  CELucke  JWeiner  RDKrystal  ADAque  M Seizure threshold in electroconvulsive therapy, I: initial seizure threshold.  Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37713- 720Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Pettinati  HMRosenberg  J Memory self-ratings before and after electroconvulsive therapy: depression versus ECT induced.  Biol Psychiatry. 1984;19539- 548Google Scholar
49.
Coleman  EASackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPMcElhiney  MCMoody  BJ Subjective memory complaints before and after electroconvulsive therapy.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39346- 356Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Nobler  MSSackeim  HAMoeller  JRPrudic  JPetkova  EWaternaux  C Quantifying the speed of symptomatic improvement with electroconvulsive therapy: comparison of alternative statistical methods.  Convuls Ther. 1997;13208- 221Google Scholar
51.
Sackeim  HA Continuation therapy following ECT: directions for future research.  Psychopharmacol Bull. 1994;30501- 521Google Scholar
52.
Shapira  BGorfine  MLerer  B A prospective study of lithium continuation therapy in depressed patients who have responded to electroconvulsive therapy.  Convuls Ther. 1995;1180- 85Google Scholar
53.
Sackeim  HADevanand  DPPrudic  J Stimulus intensity, seizure threshold, and seizure duration: impact on the efficacy and safety of electroconvulsive therapy.  Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1991;14803- 843Google Scholar
54.
Boylan  LSHaskett  RFMulsant  BFGreenburg  RPrudic  JSpignall  KLisanby  SHSackeim  HA Determinants of seizure threshold in ECT: benzodiazepine use, anesthetic dosage, and other factors.  J ECT. Google Scholar
Original Article
May 2000

A Prospective, Randomized, Double-blind Comparison of Bilateral and Right Unilateral Electroconvulsive Therapy at Different Stimulus Intensities

Author Affiliations

From the Department of Biological Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute and the Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Sackeim, Prudic, Devanand, Nobler, and Lisanby and Mss Peyser and Moody), Radiology (Dr Sackeim), and Neurology (Dr Devanand), Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, New York, NY.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(5):425-434. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.5.425
Abstract

Background  Controversy persists about the use of right unilateral (RUL) and bilateral (BL) electroconvulsive therapy (ECT). While RUL ECT results in less severe short-term and long-term cognitive effects, there is concern that it is less efficacious than BL ECT.

Methods  In a double-blind study, 80 depressed patients were randomized to RUL ECT, with electrical dosages 50%, 150%, or 500% above the seizure threshold, or BL ECT, with an electrical dosage 150% above the threshold. Depression severity and cognitive functioning were assessed before, during, immediately after, and 2 months after ECT. Compared with baseline, responders had at least a 60% reduction in symptom scores 1 week after ECT, and were monitored for relapse for 1 year.

Results  High-dosage RUL and BL ECT were equivalent in response rate (65%) and approximately twice as effective as low-dosage (35%) or moderate-dosage (30%) unilateral ECT. During the week after the randomized phase, BL ECT resulted in greater impairment than any dosage of unilateral ECT in several measures of anterograde and retrograde memory. Two months after ECT, retrograde amnestic deficits were greatest among patients treated with BL ECT. Thirty-three (53%) of the 62 patients who responded to ECT relapsed, without treatment group differences. The relapse rate was greater in patients who had not responded to adequate pharmacotherapy prior to ECT and who had more severe depressive symptoms after ECT.

Conclusion  Right unilateral ECT at high dosage is as effective as a robust form of BL ECT, but produces less severe and persistent cognitive effects.

FOR DECADES there has been a controversy concerning the use of right unilateral (RUL) or bilateral (BL) electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) in major depression.1 It has been established that RUL ECT causes less severe cognitive adverse effects than BL ECT.2-6 However, despite 40 comparative trials, the relative efficacy of both RUL and BL ECT remains uncertain.1,7-9 When efficacy differences have been found, they consistently favored BL ECT.10-12 Most patients in the United States receive BL treatment. Farah and McCall13 conducted a survey of US ECT practitioners and found that 52% initiated ECT with the BL placement. We recently conducted a survey of ECT directors at 59 facilities in the tristate New York metropolitan area (H.A.S. and J.P., unpublished data, January 1, 1997, to July 1, 1997). The mean percentage of patients receiving BL ECT was 79%.

Recently, it has been shown that the efficacy of RUL ECT is contingent on electrical dosage.5,14-17 In a 4-group study randomizing patients to RUL and BL placement and to an electrical dosage just above (0%) or 150% above the initial seizure threshold, we found that higher-dosage RUL ECT was considerably more effective than low-dosage RUL ECT and produced less severe cognitive effects than either form of BL ECT.5 Nonetheless, the higher-dosage RUL ECT did not match the efficacy of either type of BL ECT. This study contradicted the standard view that producing a generalized seizure of sufficient duration is necessary and sufficient for efficacy,18-20 and raised questions about the breadth of the dose-response function for RUL ECT.5

We report on a double-blind trial in which patients were randomized to either RUL ECT delivered at 50%, 150%, or 500% above the initial seizure threshold or to BL ECT at 150% above the threshold. We tested the hypothesis that markedly suprathreshold RUL ECT (500% above threshold) is more effective than lower-dosage RUL ECT and equal in efficacy to a robust form of BL ECT, while retaining advantages with respect to short-term and long-term cognitive adverse effects.

Subjects and methods
Subjects

Patients were referred for ECT and participation in this protocol by mental health professionals in the New York metropolitan region and, in some cases, nationally. Using the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia,21 administered by a senior research social worker (S.P.), and other diagnostic information, patients met the Research Diagnostic Criteria22 for major depressive disorder. They had a pretreatment score of 18 or greater on the 24-item Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD),23 and provided written informed consent. Patients with a history of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, other functional psychosis, rapid-cycling bipolar illness, neurological illness or insult, alcohol or other drug abuse within the past year, ECT within the past 6 months, or severe medical illness were excluded. Three participants completed the protocol as outpatients. All others were inpatients at the New York State Psychiatric Institute, whose institutional review board approved the study.

Of 84 patients admitted to the protocol, 4 were considered dropouts because they received fewer than 5 treatments (withdrawal of consent [n = 2], need for psychotropic treatment [n=1], and intercurrent illness [n = 1]). Except for lorazepam (up to 3 mg/d, as needed), psychotropic medications were discontinued at least 5 days before ECT (mean ± SD, 15.0 ± 7.3 days [maximum, 30 days]) until 1 week after the end of ECT. The mean ± SD dosage of lorazepam during ECT was 1.2 ± 1.0 mg/d and did not differ among the treatment groups. Thirteen patients did not receive lorazepam, and the mean ± SD dosage in the remaining 67 patients (1.4 ± 1.0 mg/d) also did not differ among the treatment groups. Seventy-four (93%) of the 80 patients were right-handed,24 with no difference among the treatment groups.

Electroconvulsive therapy

Patients were randomly assigned to the 4 treatment conditions, stratified by whether they had received an adequate antidepressant medication trial during the index episode.25,26 The randomization used a permuted block procedure,27 with equal distribution of the treatment conditions within each stratum. At the first treatment, the ECT psychiatrist opened a sealed envelope containing the treatment condition for the next patient in the stratum. Patients and all staff not involved in ECT administration were masked to the type and dosage of ECT.

Atropine (0.4 mg), methohexital sodium (0.75 mg/kg), and succinylcholine chloride (0.75 mg/kg) were the anesthetic medications. The standard bifrontotemporal1 and the d'Elia28 placements were used for BL and RUL ECT, respectively. Electroconvulsive therapy was administered 3 times per week with a customized MECTA SR1 device (MECTA Corp, Lake Oswego, Ore). Compared with the commercial device, the maximum train duration of the customized device was increased from 2 to 4 seconds and pulse frequency was extended from 40 through 90 Hz to 20 through 140 Hz. The seizure threshold was quantified at the first and last treatments using the empirical titration procedure.29 At all other treatments, stimulus intensity was 50%, 150%, or 500% above the initial seizure threshold for the low-, moderate-, and high-dosage RUL groups, respectively, and 150% above the threshold in the high-dosage BL group. The frequency and train duration of brief pulses were the electrical parameters manipulated, keeping pulse width (1.5 milliseconds) and amplitude (0.8 A) constant. The BL group was classified as a high-dosage group since the efficacy of this treatment is established, and adverse effects are likely to increase at higher dosage without improving efficacy.5 To be considered adequate, minimal seizure duration was 20 seconds of motor or 25 seconds of electroencephalographic manifestation.1

Clinical evaluations

A blinded clinical evaluation team (research psychiatrist [J.P.] and senior social worker [S.P.]) completed HRSD ratings 2 days before the first treatment, twice weekly during ECT, and within 1 or 2 days and 1 week after the ECT course. Interrater reliability coefficients for HRSD scores exceeded 0.98. The Clinical Global Improvement30 scale was completed 1 week after ECT.

Patients were initial responders if they had a decrease of at least 60% in HRSD scores from pretreatment to 1 or 2 days after the final treatment and a maximum posttreatment score of 16. Final responders maintained this level of improvement 1 week after ECT, while free of psychotropic medication. At least 10 treatments were required before classification as a nonresponder, with this minimum lowered to 8 treatments for patients who had HRSD reductions of 20% or less. Patients were classified as initial remitters if they met the criteria for initial response and had an HRSD score of 10 or less 1 or 2 days after the final treatment. Final remitters met the criteria for final response and had HRSD scores of 10 or less 1 week after ECT, while free of psychotropic medication.

Nonresponders were offered an open course of high-dosage (150% above the retitrated threshold) BL ECT. Efficacy evaluations for this crossover phase followed the procedures used in the randomized phase. Patients who responded in either phase were evaluated until relapse, as defined elsewhere,25 or for 1 year. Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression interviews were conducted every 2 weeks for the first 2 months after ECT and monthly thereafter. During the follow-up period, continuation/maintenance treatment was naturalistic. Strength (0-5 scale) and adequacy (categorical) of continuation treatments were rated using the Antidepressant Treatment History Form with respect to the type and dosage of treatment, as well as compliance.25,26,31

Cognitive evaluations

Acute neuropsychological effects were assessed at each treatment during the randomized phase by blinded, trained technicians. Before each treatment, patients memorized sets of words, geometric and nonsense shapes, and either neutral or emotionally expressive faces. After presentation of each set, immediate learning was tested.2,5 After the seizure, recovery of orientation was assessed continuously for 90 minutes. Patients who failed to meet the orientation recovery criteria2,32 were given scores of 100 minutes. Five minutes after the orientation criteria were met, retrograde memory was tested for the previously learned material. Twelve equivalent stimuli sets were available for each task.2

Neurocognitive tests focusing on anterograde and retrograde memory were administered before ECT, the day after the sixth or seventh treatment, 2 to 7 days after the end of the randomized phase, and 2 months after completion of all ECT. The battery included the modified Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE),33 an expanded version (range, 0-57) of the original MMSE34 with established reliability and validity.35,36 Other tasks included complex figure copying and reproduction,37,38 randomizing the Rey-Osterreith, Taylor, and Richie figures across testing occasions. Scores for copying and 20-minute delayed reproduction were examined. The Buschke Selective Reminding Test (SRT)39,40 used 12 words and 10 trials. Free recall was assessed after a 2-hour delay and then the full SRT procedure was repeated. The dependent measures were total recall across the 10 trials at first administration, first trial delayed free recall, and total recall across the 10 trials at the second administration. Recognition memory for verbal and nonverbal material was assessed with paired-word and paired-face tasks.5,41 The paired-word task involved presentation of 30 word pairs. After presentation, learning was assessed by presenting a target and 4 foils (including the paired word). The paired-face task involved presentation of 20 face pairs, followed by immediate recognition testing, using a target and 3 foils (including the paired face). After 4 hours, delayed recognition memory was assessed for both tasks. The complete Randt Memory Test was also administered,42,43 using the immediate and delayed memory for verbal paired associates, story recall, and picture recognition subtests. The dependent measures were scores for immediate acquisition and 24-hour delayed recall.

Assessments of retrograde amnesia included a test of memory for famous events occurring between 1950 and 1990,44 and the Columbia University Autobiographical Memory Interview (AMI).6,32 Autobiographical Memory Interview retrograde amnesia scores were the percentage of factual items reported at retesting that were inconsistent with baseline reports, and the percentage of "don't remember" responses for factual items reported at baseline. Except for the famous event test and AMI, alternative versions of all cognitive tasks were used at each assessment. Patients also rated their subjective memory functioning with the Squire Subjective Memory Questionnaire.45

At the evaluations after the randomized phase, the primary dependent measures were delayed recall and reacquisition scores on the SRT (anterograde amnesia) and the 2 AMI retrograde amnesia scores. Since retrograde memory is the domain known to show persistent deficits after ECT,4,6,32 analyses at the 2-month follow-up were restricted to scores on the retrograde famous events test and the AMI. The primary measures were selected because of their established sensitivity to ECT effects3-6 and functional significance (ie, degree of anterograde and retrograde amnesia).

Statistical analyses

The results are expressed as mean ± SD. All statistical tests were 2-tailed, with a level of significance of α=.05. The comparability of the 4 treatment groups in baseline demographic and clinical features and treatment parameters was tested with analyses of variance (ANOVAs) for continuous variables and χ2 analyses for dichotomous variables. As an omnibus test of efficacy differences, a repeated-measures ANOVA, was conducted on HRSD scores, with the randomized ECT modality (4 levels) and medication resistance classification (2 levels) as between-subject factors, and time point (before ECT, after 6 treatments, immediately after treatment, and 1 week after treatment) as the repeated-measures factor. This was followed by analyses of covariance (ANCOVAs) on HRSD scores at the last 3 points, with baseline HRSD scores as the covariate. With all ANCOVAs, post hoc t tests of covariate-adjusted means were used to identify pairwise group differences. Log-linear analysis was used to compare groups in rates of response and remission (modality by medication resistance). Multiple regression analysis was used to determine whether dosage relative to threshold, or the absolute electrical dose administered, contributed to efficacy in the RUL ECT conditions. Percentage change from baseline in HRSD scores was predicted on the basis of treatment group assignment and absolute electrical dosage. Survival analyses, using both Kaplan-Meier and regression models, provided tests of treatment group differences in likelihood and speed of relapse. The regression model used the Weibull distribution, with medication resistance classification, HRSD scores 1 week after ECT, and strength of post-ECT continuation/maintenance treatment as covariates.

Similar methods were used to compare the treatment conditions in cognitive measures. For the acute measures of orientation recovery and retrograde memory, the scores for each patient were averaged across all treatments. We compared the treatment groups' scores using ANCOVAs, with age as the covariate. For measures of short-term and long-term cognitive effects, ANCOVAs were performed using the ECT modality as the between-subject factor and baseline performance as the covariate. Differences in cognitive performance among the ECT conditions at the 2-month follow-up examination were assessed using ANCOVAs to compare the groups that received a single course of RUL ECT, a single course of BL ECT, or crossover BL ECT. When significant differences were obtained, ANCOVAs were repeated comparing the groups that received only high-dosage RUL ECT, a single course of BL ECT, and crossover treatment.

Results
Characteristics of patients and treatment measures

The treatment groups did not differ in demographic and clinical characteristics (Table 1) or in doses of anesthetic agents or seizure duration (Table 2) (P>.10 for each comparison). Replicating an established finding,29,46,47 the initial seizure threshold was higher with BL than RUL ECT (t78=2.84, P<.006). Across the sample, the range of the initial seizure threshold was 14-fold (24-336 millicoulombs [mC]).

Efficacy

The treatment groups differed in clinical outcome (Figure 1). The repeated-measures ANOVA on the serial HRSD scores yielded a main effect of time point (F3,216=145.82, P<.001) and interactions between treatment group and time point (F9,216=2.20, P=.02) and between medication resistance classification and time point (F3,216=2.83, P=.04). Follow-up ANCOVAs indicated that the treatment groups differed in HRSD scores at all time points after baseline (after sixth ECT: F3,71=2.99, P=.04; 1-2 days after ECT: F3,71=3.20, P=.03; 1 week after ECT: F3,71=2.84, P=.04). Post hoc comparisons indicated that the low- and moderate-dosage RUL ECT groups did not differ from each other at any point. Likewise, the high-dosage RUL and BL groups did not differ at any point. Using ANCOVA (with baseline HRSD scores as a covariate), we discovered that after the sixth treatment, the high-dosage RUL and BL groups had superior antidepressant response compared with the low- and moderate-dosage RUL groups (F1,77=9.38, P=.003). This difference was maintained immediately after the ECT course (F1,77=10.65, P=.002), and 1 week later (F1,77=5.21, P=.03).

The log-linear analysis conducted on the initial response rate produced a main effect of treatment group (χ23=11.68, P=.009) (Table 3). In the analysis of final response rate, there were main effects of both treatment group (χ23=8.39, P=.04) and the medication resistance classification (χ21=4.99, P=.03). The rates of initial and final response were identical in the high-dosage RUL and BL groups. Patients in these groups were almost twice as likely to be final responders as patients treated with low- or moderate-dosage RUL ECT (χ21=8.46, P=.004). Independent of treatment condition, patients classified as medication resistant (n=46 [37%]) had a lower final response rate than patients for whom an adequate medication trial had not failed prior to ECT (n = 34 [65%]) (χ21=6.03; P=.01) (Figure 2). Immediately and 1 week after ECT, only 1 final responder had an HRSD score greater than 10. Consequently, analyses of response and remitter rates produced identical results (Table 3). For the 39 final responders, the mean ± SD HRSD scores immediately and 1 week after ECT were 4.6 ± 3.0 and 5.5 ± 3.4, respectively. Among final responders, there was no difference among the treatment groups in HRSD scores 1 week after ECT (F3,35=0.09, P=.97).

The ANOVA on Clinical Global Impressions improvement scores produced main effects of treatment group (F3,72=2.82, P=.04) and medication resistance (F1,72=4.15, P=.045). The high-dosage RUL and BL groups had greater improvement than the low- and moderate-dosage RUL groups (F1,76=7.27, P=.009).

The ANOVA on the number of treatments in the randomized phase did not produce an effect of treatment group (F3,72=2.05, P=.11). Despite the fact that the rate of early termination because of lack of efficacy was highest with low- and moderate-dosage RUL ECT, both high-dosage conditions averaged 1 fewer treatment than the less effective conditions (Table 3).

Absolute electrical dose or dose relative to threshold

Among the 60 patients treated with RUL ECT, regression analyses indicated that improvement in HRSD scores was associated with dosage condition after the sixth treatment (t57=2.58, P=.01), immediately after treatment, (t57=2.39, P=.02), and 1 week later (t57=1.78, P=.08), and not with absolute electrical dose at any point. Thus, efficacy was influenced by dosage relative to seizure threshold, and not by absolute electrical dose.

Crossover phase

Thirty-six (88%) of the 41 patients who did not respond in the randomized phase completed the crossover phase. They received a mean ± SD of 7.6 ± 2.1 high-dosage BL treatments. Their mean ± SD HRSD scores were 31.9 ± 7.1 before ECT, 24.8 ± 8.1 after the randomized phase, 8.3 ± 6.9 immediately after crossover treatment, and 11.3 ± 6.7 one week after the crossover phase; 28 (78%) and 25 (69%) were initial and final responders, respectively. Symptomatic improvement, response, and remission rates were equivalent regardless of the prior randomized assignment.

Relapse

Sixty-two of the 64 patients who responded to randomized or crossover treatment (39 and 25 patients, respectively) were monitored for relapse. Their continuation/maintenance treatments are described in Table 4. In both the Kaplan-Meier and regression survival analyses, ECT modality was unrelated to relapse. This held regardless of whether patients who responded during the crossover phase were treated as a separate group or combined with patients who responded to high-dosage BL ECT in the randomized phase. Thirty-three (53%) of the 62 patients relapsed during the year; 31 (94%) of the relapses occurred during the first 6 months. The relapse rates were 62% (15/24) for patients who received crossover treatment, 54% (7/13) for the high-dosage BL ECT group, 33% (4/12) for the high-dosage RUL ECT group, and 54% (7/13) for patients treated with either low- or moderate-dosage RUL ECT. Relapse was almost twice as likely among medication-resistant patients (23 [68%] of 34) as among patients who had not received an adequate medication trial before ECT (10 [36%] of 28) (likelihood ratio, χ21=4.46; P=.03) (Figure 3). Higher HRSD scores at the end of ECT were also associated with a higher rate of relapse (likelihood ratio, χ21=8.54; P=.004). Strength of continuation/maintenance treatment was not related to relapse (likelihood ratio, χ21=0.34; P=.56). The relapse rate was 56% (25/45) for patients who received adequate continuation/maintenance treatment and 47% (8/17) for patients whose treatment was below the threshold for adequacy. However, the relapse rate was only 35% (6/17) for patients who received adequate treatment with the combination of a tricyclic antidepressant and lithium, compared with 68% (19/28) for adequate treatment with all other regimens (likelihood ratio, χ21=5.33; P=.02).

Cognitive adverse effects

At the postictal assessments, cognition was more impaired after BL ECT compared with any other treatment (Table 5). The rate of prolonged disorientation was more than 6 times greater with BL ECT than with any other treatment. Bilateral ECT resulted in a significantly longer time to recovery of orientation than all other treatments, and high-dosage RUL ECT resulted in a longer recovery time than low- or moderate-dosage RUL ECT. Compared with each of the other 3 groups, BL ECT resulted in greater amnesia for the recall and recognition of words and greater amnesia than the low- and moderate-dosage RUL groups for the recognition of geometric shapes.

At the baseline assessment, before ECT, the treatment groups did not differ in any neuropsychological measure. After completion of the randomized phase, the BL ECT group had greater impairment than each of the other 3 groups on a variety of measures, including all the primary cognitive variables (Table 6). This pattern held for post-ECT scores on the MMSE; initial acquisition, delayed recall, and reacquisition on the SRT; immediate recognition in the anterograde paired-words task; and the Randt subtests of delayed paired-word recall and delayed picture recall. Across every test of the retention of new information over a delay (anterograde amnesia), the BL ECT group had the poorest absolute performance. For the measures of retrograde amnesia, memory of famous events and both scores on the AMI, the BL ECT group showed greater impairment than each of the other 3 groups. Bilateral and high-dosage RUL ECT could not be distinguished and produced greater impairment than low- and moderate-dosage RUL ECT in the post-ECT measures of delayed reproduction of the complex figure and delayed short story recall. Despite clear objective amnestic effects, the patient sample reported subjective improvement in memory after ECT (mean ± SD, 48.5% ± 81.2%) (t71=5.1, P<.001), as seen in a number of recent studies,4,48,49 with no difference among the treatment groups.

Fifty-five patients participated in the 2-month follow-up. The ANCOVA comparing RUL ECT (n=20), a single course of BL ECT (n=10), or BL ECT as a crossover treatment (n=25) produced differences among the groups in retrograde memory for famous events (F2,51=4.26; P=.02). Post hoc comparisons indicated that patients who had been treated only with RUL ECT had better scores than patients treated with a single course of high-dosage BL ECT or patients who received crossover BL ECT. These differences were significant when restricting the RUL ECT group to those who had received high-dosage treatment (n=10). For the AMI retrograde amnesia scores, the groups differed in the percentage of responses inconsistent with baseline (F2,51=4.26, P=.02) and the percentage of "don't remember" responses (F2,51=6.06, P=.004). Patients treated only with RUL ECT had superior scores compared with patients treated with BL ECT; these differences were maintained when restricting the RUL ECT group to those who received high-dosage treatment.

Comment

This study suggests that RUL ECT delivered with a high stimulus intensity relative to seizure threshold is equivalent in efficacy to a criterion standard form of BL ECT, yet retains important advantages with respect to cognitive adverse effects.

At all time points and for all efficacy measures, high-dosage RUL and BL ECT could not be distinguished in antidepressant effects. Both were considerably more effective than either low- or moderate-dosage RUL ECT. These findings confirm earlier reports that the efficacy of RUL ECT is influenced by electrical dosage,5,14-17 and that fixed high-dosage RUL ECT can be as effective as BL ECT.15 Specifically, this study indicates that, at sufficiently high stimulus intensity above the threshold, RUL matches BL ECT in efficacy.

In previous studies,5,14 we found that RUL ECT administered just above the seizure threshold (0%) was remarkably ineffective (eg, 17% final response rate), while RUL ECT 150% above the threshold was more efficacious (44% final response rate). In this study, there was no difference in efficacy between the low (50% above the threshold) and moderate (150% above the threshold) RUL groups. This may indicate that a dosage increment between 50% and 150% relative to the threshold is insufficient to influence outcome.

Despite the use of high electrical intensity with high-dosage RUL ECT, this treatment produced less severe and persistent cognitive adverse effects than BL ECT. In the postictal period, recovery of orientation was prolonged with BL ECT, and BL treatment produced the greatest retrograde amnesia in selective measures. During the week after treatment, BL ECT resulted in more severe impairment than any of the RUL conditions in each of the primary cognitive measures. These effects were of clinical consequence. Capacity to recall words after a 2-hour delay on the SRT improved by 20% relative to baseline in patients treated with high-dosage RUL ECT, but decreased by 22% in patients treated with BL ECT. At this point, those in the BL group were 71% more likely than those in the high-dosage RUL group not to remember facts about their lives that they had reported at baseline. During the week after treatment, BL ECT also resulted in more severe cognitive adverse effects than any of the RUL treatment conditions in a variety of secondary measures assessing global cognitive status, anterograde learning and memory, and retrograde memory. At 2-month follow-up, BL ECT, either as a single course or as crossover treatment, resulted in greater retrograde amnesia than high-dosage RUL ECT. This long-term effect held for all primary measures. Thus, despite high stimulus intensity relative to seizure threshold, RUL ECT retained important cognitive advantages relative to BL ECT.

It may be questioned whether the choice of dosage for the BL ECT group biased the comparisons of cognitive effects. Previously, we reported no difference in degree of clinical improvement when BL ECT was administered just above (0%) or 150% above the threshold, although the response was slower with low-dosage BL ECT.5,50 However, we did not detect differences between the 2 forms of BL ECT in short-term or long-term cognitive effects. Thus, it is unlikely that the differences in cognitive effects were caused by excessive doses of BL ECT. On the other hand, the use of a robust form of BL ECT should allay doubt that the equivalence with high-dosage RUL ECT in efficacy and relapse rate was caused by use of a handicapped form of BL ECT.

As in several recent studies (see Sackeim51 for a review), the rate of relapse after response to ECT was high despite adequate continuation treatment. With the possible exception of the combination of a tricyclic antidepressant and lithium, strength or adequacy of continuation/maintenance treatment had no relationship to relapse, a finding previously reported.25 Also in line with previous results,5,25 the type or dosage of ECT was independent of relapse. In contrast, medication-resistant patients were less likely to respond to ECT regardless of the type of ECT used (Figure 2), and if they did respond, were more than twice as likely to relapse (Figure 3). These findings confirm earlier reports that medication resistance predicts both ECT short-term outcome and relapse.25,26,31,52 Since resistance to antidepressant medications is the leading indication for the use of ECT,1 research is needed on methods to improve ECT response and prevent relapse in this subgroup.

In previous studies, the differences in cognitive effects between RUL and BL ECT2,4-6 and the impact of electrical dosage on the efficacy of RUL ECT5,14 had large effect sizes. This study was powered to detect effects of this magnitude in the potential impact of high-dosage RUL and BL ECT on cognition and the potential impact of the dosage conditions within RUL ECT on efficacy. However, since each treatment group contained only 20 patients, the lack of difference in efficacy between high-dosage RUL and BL ECT might reflect insufficient power. This concern was mitigated by the finding that these conditions had identical initial and final response rates.

Only 55 patients (69%) were evaluated at the 2-month follow-up. This raises the possibility of selection bias caused by selective loss to follow-up of patients with more pronounced cognitive deficits. However, the patients who did and did not participate in the 2-month evaluation did not differ in cognitive measures after the randomized phase (data not shown).

This study used a customized ECT device with an extended output range. Given the marked variability in seizure threshold,53,54 it will not be possible to treat some patients with RUL ECT at 6 times the threshold using standard devices in the United States (504-576 mC maximal output). In this study, 14 of 20 patients in the high-dosage RUL group were treated at the conventional maximum (576 mC). Two patients (1 BL and 1 high-dosage RUL) had substantial threshold increases during the ECT course, resulting in inadequate seizures and subsequent stimulation above conventional levels (768 and 840 mC). In a study of 267 patients treated at 3 sites with RUL ECT, we found that 10.9% had an initial threshold greater than 100 mC.54 The findings of this study support consideration of higher electrical output for standard ECT devices.

Accepted for publication December 1, 1999.

This study was supported in part by grants R37 MH35636 and R10 MH57009 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md (Dr Sackeim).

We thank Rene Doolity, RN, Judith E. Kiersky, PhD, Richard Krueger, MD, Martin C. McElhiney, PhD, Barbara McGuire, PhD, and the staff of the General Clinical Research Unit, New York State Psychiatric Institute, New York, NY, without whose help this study could not have been carried out.

Reprints: Harold A. Sackeim, PhD, Department of Biological Psychiatry, New York State Psychiatric Institute, 1051 Riverside Dr, New York, NY 10032 (e-mail: has1@columbia.edu).

References
1.
Weiner  RDFink  MHammersley  DMoench  LSackeim  HASmall  IAmerican Psychiatric Association, The Practice of ECT: Recommendations for Treatment, Training and Privileging.  Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1990;
2.
Sackeim  HAPortnoy  SNeeley  PSteif  BLDecina  PMalitz  S Cognitive consequences of low-dosage electroconvulsive therapy.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986;462326- 340Google ScholarCrossref
3.
Sackeim  HA The cognitive effects of electroconvulsive therapy. Moos  WHGamzu  ERThal  LJeds Cognitive Disorders: Pathophysiology and Treatment New York, NY Marcel Dekker1992;183- 228Google Scholar
4.
Weiner  RDRogers  HJDavidson  JRSquire  LR Effects of stimulus parameters on cognitive side effects.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986;462315- 325Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Sackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPKiersky  JEFitzsimons  LMoody  BJMcElhiney  MCColeman  EASettembrino  JM Effects of stimulus intensity and electrode placement on the efficacy and cognitive effects of electroconvulsive therapy.  N Engl J Med. 1993;328839- 846Google ScholarCrossref
6.
McElhiney  MCMoody  BJSteif  BLPrudic  JDevanand  DPNobler  MSSackeim  HA Autobiographical memory and mood: effects of electroconvulsive therapy.  Neuropsychology. 1995;9501- 517Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Strömgren  LS Is bilateral ECT ever indicated?  Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1984;69484- 490Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Ottosson  JO Is unilateral nondominant ECT as efficient as bilateral ECT? a new look at the evidence.  Convuls Ther. 1991;7190- 200Google Scholar
9.
Abrams  R Electroconvulsive Therapy. 3rd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1997;
10.
d'Elia  GRaotma  H Is unilateral ECT less effective than bilateral ECT?  Br J Psychiatry. 1975;12683- 89Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Abrams  R Is unilateral electroconvulsive therapy really the treatment of choice in endogenous depression?  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1986;46250- 55Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Sackeim  HA Optimizing unilateral electroconvulsive therapy.  Convulsive Ther. 1991;7201- 212Google Scholar
13.
Farah  AMcCall  WV Electroconvulsive therapy stimulus dosing: a survey of contemporary practices.  Convulsive Ther. 1993;990- 94Google Scholar
14.
Sackeim  HADecina  PKanzler  MKerr  BMalitz  S Effects of electrode placement on the efficacy of titrated, low-dose ECT.  Am J Psychiatry. 1987;1441449- 1455Google Scholar
15.
Abrams  RSwartz  CMVedak  C Antidepressant effects of high-dose right unilateral electroconvulsive therapy.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48746- 748Google ScholarCrossref
16.
Krystal  ADCoffey  CEWeiner  RDHolsinger  T Changes in seizure threshold over the course of electroconvulsive therapy affect therapeutic response and are detected by ictal EEG ratings.  J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1998;10178- 186Google Scholar
17.
Letemendia  FJDelva  NJRodenburg  MLawson  JSInglis  JWaldron  JJLywood  DW Therapeutic advantage of bifrontal electrode placement in ECT.  Psychol Med. 1993;23349- 360Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Ottosson  JO Experimental studies of the mode of action of electroconvulsive therapy.  Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1960;1451- 141Google Scholar
19.
Fink  M Convulsive Therapy: Theory and Practice.  New York, NY Raven Press1979;
20.
Not Available, NIH Consensus Conference: electroconvulsive therapy.  JAMA. 1985;2542103- 2108Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Endicott  JSpitzer  RL A diagnostic interview: the schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35837- 844Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Spitzer  RLEndicott  JRobins  E Research diagnostic criteria: rationale and reliability.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35773- 782Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Hamilton  M Development of a rating scale for primary depressive illness.  Br J Soc Clin Psychol. 1967;6278- 296Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Rackowski  DKalat  JWNebes  R Reliability and validity of some handedness questionnaire items.  Neuropsychologia. 1976;8523- 526Google Scholar
25.
Sackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPDecina  PKerr  BMalitz  S The impact of medication resistance and continuation pharmacotherapy on relapse following response to electroconvulsive therapy in major depression.  J Clin Psychopharmacol. 1990;1096- 104Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Prudic  JHaskett  RFMulsant  BMalone  KMPettinati  HMStephans  SGreenburg  RRifas  SLSackeim  HA Resistance to antidepressant medications and short-term clinical response to ECT.  Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153985- 992Google Scholar
27.
Fleiss  JL The Design and Analysis of Clinical Experiments.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1986;
28.
d'Elia  G Unilateral electroconvulsive therapy.  Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl. 1970;2151- 98Google Scholar
29.
Sackeim  HDecina  PProhovnik  IMalitz  S Seizure threshold in electroconvulsive therapy: effects of sex, age, electrode placement, and number of treatments.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44355- 360Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Guy  W ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology.  Washington, DC National Institute of Health1976;Dept of Health, Education, and Welfare publication 76-338
31.
Prudic  JSackeim  HADevanand  DP Medication resistance and clinical response to electroconvulsive therapy.  Psychiatry Res. 1990;31287- 96Google ScholarCrossref
32.
Sobin  CSackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPMoody  BJMcElhiney  MC Predictors of retrograde amnesia following ECT.  Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152995- 1001Google Scholar
33.
Mayeux  RStern  YRosen  JLeventhal  J Depression, intellectual impairment, and Parkinson disease.  Neurology. 1981;31645- 650Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Folstein  MFFolstein  SEMcHugh  PR "Mini-Mental state": a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients the clinician.  J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12189- 198Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Stern  YSano  MPauson  JMayeux  R Modified Mini-Mental Status Examination: validity and reliability.  Neurology. 1987;37(suppl 1)S179Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Sackeim  HAProhovnik  IMoeller  JRMayeux  RStern  YDevanand  DP Regional cerebral blood flow in mood disorders, II: comparison of major depression and Alzheimer's disease.  J Nucl Med. 1993;341090- 1101Google Scholar
37.
Rey  A L'examen psychologique dans les cas d'encephalopathie traumatique.  Arch Psychol. 1941;28286- 340Google Scholar
38.
Lezak  MD Neuropsychological Assessment. 3rd ed. New York, NY Oxford University Press1995;
39.
Buschke  H Selective reminding for analysis of memory and learning.  J Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 1973;12543- 550Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Hannay  HJLevin  HS Selective reminding test: an examination of the equivalence of four forms.  J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 1985;7251- 263Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Steif  BLSackeim  HAPortnoy  SDecina  PMalitz  S Effects of depression and ECT on anterograde memory.  Biol Psychiatry. 1986;21921- 930Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Randt  CTBrown  EROsbourne  DP A memory test for longitudinal measurement of mild to moderate deficits.  Clin Neuropsychol. 1980;2184- 194Google Scholar
43.
Randt  CTBrown  RE Randt Memory Test.  Bayport, NY Life Science1983;
44.
Goldberg  EBarnett  J The Goldberg-Barnett Remote Memory Questionnaire.  New York, NY Albert Einstein Medical College1985;
45.
Squire  LRWetzel  CDSlater  PC Memory complaint after electroconvulsive therapy: assessment with a new self-rating instrument.  Biol Psychiatry. 1979;14791- 801Google Scholar
46.
McCall  WVShelp  FEWeiner  RDAustin  SNorris  J Convulsive threshold differences in right unilateral and bilateral ECT.  Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34606- 611Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Coffey  CELucke  JWeiner  RDKrystal  ADAque  M Seizure threshold in electroconvulsive therapy, I: initial seizure threshold.  Biol Psychiatry. 1995;37713- 720Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Pettinati  HMRosenberg  J Memory self-ratings before and after electroconvulsive therapy: depression versus ECT induced.  Biol Psychiatry. 1984;19539- 548Google Scholar
49.
Coleman  EASackeim  HAPrudic  JDevanand  DPMcElhiney  MCMoody  BJ Subjective memory complaints before and after electroconvulsive therapy.  Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39346- 356Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Nobler  MSSackeim  HAMoeller  JRPrudic  JPetkova  EWaternaux  C Quantifying the speed of symptomatic improvement with electroconvulsive therapy: comparison of alternative statistical methods.  Convuls Ther. 1997;13208- 221Google Scholar
51.
Sackeim  HA Continuation therapy following ECT: directions for future research.  Psychopharmacol Bull. 1994;30501- 521Google Scholar
52.
Shapira  BGorfine  MLerer  B A prospective study of lithium continuation therapy in depressed patients who have responded to electroconvulsive therapy.  Convuls Ther. 1995;1180- 85Google Scholar
53.
Sackeim  HADevanand  DPPrudic  J Stimulus intensity, seizure threshold, and seizure duration: impact on the efficacy and safety of electroconvulsive therapy.  Psychiatr Clin North Am. 1991;14803- 843Google Scholar
54.
Boylan  LSHaskett  RFMulsant  BFGreenburg  RPrudic  JSpignall  KLisanby  SHSackeim  HA Determinants of seizure threshold in ECT: benzodiazepine use, anesthetic dosage, and other factors.  J ECT. Google Scholar
×