[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Table 1. 
Descriptive Characteristics of Mothers and Children in Kyiv, Ukraine, Assessed in 1997*
Descriptive Characteristics of Mothers and Children in Kyiv, Ukraine, Assessed in 1997*
Table 2. 
Differences in Well-being Between Evacuees and Classmates*
Differences in Well-being Between Evacuees and Classmates*
Table 3. 
Accounting for Differences Between Evacuees and Classmates Before and After Adjusting for Epidemiologic Risk Factors and Chornobyl Stress*
Accounting for Differences Between Evacuees and Classmates Before and After Adjusting for Epidemiologic Risk Factors and Chornobyl Stress*
1.
Bard  DVerger  PHubert  P Chernobyl, 10 years after: health consequences.  Epidemiol Rev. 1997;19187- 204Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Kroll-Smith  JSCouch  SR Technological hazards, adaptation and social change. Couch  SRKroll-Smith  JSeds Communities at Risk Collective Responses to Technological Hazards. New York, NY Peter Lang1991;293- 316Google Scholar
3.
Williams  D Thyroid cancer and the Chernobyl accident.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;816- 7Google Scholar
4.
Nikiforov  YGnepp  DRFagin  JA Thyroid lesions in children and adolescents after the Chernobyl disaster: implications for the study of radiation tumorigenesis.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;819- 14Google Scholar
5.
Bromet  E Psychological effects of radiation catastrophes. Peterson  LEAbrahamson  Seds Effects of Ionizing Radiation Atomic Bomb Survivors and Their Children (1945-1995). Washington, DC Joseph Henry Press1998;283- 294Google Scholar
6.
Lifton  RJ Death in Life: Survivors of Hiroshima.  New York, NY Random House1967;
7.
Misao  THattori  KShirakawa  MSuga  MOgawa  NOhara  YOhno  TFukuta  HHadama  TKuwahara  HFukamachi  KHitsumoto  SKamatani  T Characteristics in abnormalities observed in atom-bombed survivors.  J Radiat Res (Tokyo). 1961;285- 97Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Yamada  MKodama  KWong  FL The long-term psychological sequelae of atomic-bomb survivors in Hiroshima and Nagasaki. Ricks  RBerger  MEO'Hara  RMeds The Medical Basis for Radiation Preparedness III The Psychological Perspective. New York, NY Elsevier1991;155- 163Google Scholar
9.
Dohrenwend  BP Psychological implications of nuclear accidents: the case of Three Mile Island.  Bull N Y Acad Med. 1983;591060- 1076Google Scholar
10.
Bromet  EJParkinson  DKSchulberg  HCDunn  LOGondek  PC Mental health of residents near the Three Mile Island reactor: a comparative study of selected groups.  J Prev Psychiatry. 1982;1225- 275Google Scholar
11.
Baum  AFleming  I Implications of psychological research on stress and technological accidents.  Am Psychol. June1993;665- 672Google Scholar
12.
Davidson  LMBaum  A Chronic stress and posttraumatic stress disorders.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1986;54303- 308Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Dew  MABromet  EJSchulberg  HCDunn  LOParkinson  DK Mental health effects of the Three Mile Island nuclear reactor restart.  Am J Psychiatry. 1987;1441074- 1077Google Scholar
14.
Dew  MABromet  EJ Predictors of temporal patterns of psychiatric distress during 10 years following the nuclear accident at Three Mile Island.  Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1993;2849- 55Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Bromet  EJSchulberg  HC The TMI disaster: a search for high risk groups. Shore  JHed Disaster Stress Studies New Methods and Findings. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1986;2- 19Google Scholar
16.
Bromet  EHough  LConnell  M Mental health of children near the Three Mile Island reactor.  J Prev Psychiatry. 1984;2275- 301Google Scholar
17.
Cornely  PBromet  E Prevalence of behavior problems in three-year-old children living near Three Mile Island: a comparative analysis.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1986;27489- 498Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Cohen  SBromet  E Maternal predictors of behavioral disturbance in preschool children: a research note.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33941- 946Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Pass  J 18-Year Follow-Up of Children Living Near Three Mile Island: INTEL Science Report.  Setauket, NY Ward Melville High School1999;
20.
Cwikel  JAbdelgani  AGoldsmith  JRQuastel  MYevelson  II Two-year follow-up study of stress-related disorders among immigrants to Israel from the Chornobyl area.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;1051545- 1550Google Scholar
21.
Quastel  MRGoldsmith  JRCwikel  JMerkin  LWishkerman  VYPoljak  SAbdelgani  AKordysh  EDouvdevani  ALevy  JGorodisher  RBarki  YEmerit  IKramer  G Lessons learned from the study of immigrants to Israel from areas of Russia, Belarus, and Ukraine contaminated by the Chernobyl accident.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;1051523- 1527Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Havenaar  JMVan den Brink  Wvan den Bout  JKasyanenko  APPoelijoe  NWWholfarth  TMeijler-Iljina  LI Mental health problems in the Gomel region (Belarus): an analysis of risk factors in an area affected by the Chernobyl disaster.  Psychol Med. 1996;26845- 55Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Havenaar  JMRumyantzeva  GKasyanenko  AKaasjager  KWestermann  Avan den Brink  Wvan den Bout  JSavelkoul  J Health effects of the Chernobyl disaster: illness or illness behavior? a comparative general health survey in two former Soviet regions.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;1051533- 1537Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Havenaar  JMvan den Brink  W Psychological factors affecting health after toxicological disasters.  Clin Psychol Rev. 1997;17359- 374Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Viinamäki  HKumpusalo  EMyllykangas  MSalomaa  SKumpusalo  LKolmakov  SIlchenko  IZhukowsky  GNissinen  A The Chernobyl accident and mental wellbeing: a population study.  Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1995;91396- 401Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Rahu  MTekkel  MVeidebaum  TPukkala  EHakulinen  TAuvinen  ARytomaa  TInskip  PDBoice  JD  Jr The Estonian study of Chernobyl cleanup workers, II: incidence of cancer and mortality.  Radiat Res. 1997;147653- 657Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Baverstock  KF Perspectives in radiation and health: reflections on the International Conference in Beer Sheva.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;105(suppl 6)1611- 1617Google Scholar
28.
Havenaar  JMvan den Brink  W Psychological factors affecting health after toxicological disasters.  Clin Psychol Rev 1997;17359- 374Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Yevelson  IIAbdelgani  ACwikel  JYevelson  IS Bridging the gap in mental health approaches between East and West: the psychosocial consequences of radiation exposure.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;105(suppl 6)1551- 1556Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Havenaar  JMRumyantzeva  GMVandenbrink  WPoelijoe  NWVandenbout  JVanengeland  HKoeter  MWJ Long-term mental health effects of the Chernobyl disaster: an epidemiologic survey in two former Soviet regions.  Am J Psychiatry. 1997;1541605- 1607Google Scholar
31.
Stiehm  ER The psychologic fallout from Chernobyl.  AJDC. 1992;146761- 762Google Scholar
32.
Kolominsky  YIgumnov  SDrozdovitch  V The psychological development of children from Belarus exposed in the prenatal period to radiation from the Chernobyl atomic power plant.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40299- 305Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Aptekar  LBoore  J The emotional effects of disaster on children: a review of the literature.  Int J Ment Health. 1990;1977- 90Google Scholar
34.
Green  BKorol  MGrace  MVary  MLeonard  AGleser  GSmitson-Cohen  S Children and disaster: age, gender, and parental effects on PTSD symptoms.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991;30945- 951Google ScholarCrossref
35.
McFarlane  APolicansky  SIrwin  C A longitudinal study of the psychological morbidity in children due to a natural disaster.  Psychol Med. 1987;17727- 738Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Raphael  B When Disaster Strikes.  New York, NY Basic Books1986;
37.
Saylor  CFed Children and Disasters.  New York, NY Plenum Press1993;
38.
Vogel  JVernberg  EM Children's psychological responses to disasters: part 1.  J Clin Child Psychol. 1993;22464- 484Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Verhulst  FCKoot  HM Child Psychatric Epidemiology: Concepts, Methods, and Findings.  London, England Sage Publications1992;
40.
Reynolds  CRRichmond  BO What I think and feel: a revised measure of children's manifest anxiety.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1978;6271- 280Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Birleson  P The validity of depressive disorder in childhood and the development of a self-rating scale: a research report.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1981;2273- 88Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Scherer  MWNakamura  CY A Fear Survey Schedule for Children (FSS-FC): a factor analytic comparison with manifest anxiety (CMAS).  Behav Res Ther. 1968;6173- 182Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Garber  JWalker  LSZeman  J Somatization symptoms in a community sample of children and adolescents: further validation of the Children's Somatization Inventory.  Psychol Assess. 1991;3588- 595Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Harter  S The Perceived Competence Scale for Children.  Child Dev. 1982;5387- 97Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Pynoos  RSFrederick  CNader  KArroyo  WSteinberg  AEth  SNunez  FFairbanks  L Life threat and posttraumatic stress in school-age children.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;441057- 1063Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Carter  ASGrigorenko  ELPauls  DL A Russian adaptation of the Child Behavior Checklist: psychometric properties and associations with child and maternal affective symptomatology and maternal functioning.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1995;23661- 684Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Achenbach  TM Manual for the CBCL/4-18 and 1991 Profile.  Burlington University of Vermont Dept of Psychiatry1991;
48.
Loney  JMilich  R Hyperactivity, inattention, and aggression in clinical practice. Wolraich  MRouth  DKeds Advances in Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. Vol 3 Greenwich, Conn JAI Press1982;113- 147Google Scholar
49.
Weiss  DSMarmar  CR The Impact of Events Scale–Revised. Wilson  JPKeane  TMeds Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD A Handbook for Practitioners. New York, NY Guilford1997;399- 411Google Scholar
50.
Phelan  JSchwartz  JEBromet  EJDew  MAParkinson  DKSchulberg  HCDunn  LOBlane  HCurtis  EC Work stress, family stress and depression in professional and managerial employees.  Psychol Med. 1991;21999- 1012Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  MFirst  MB The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID), I: history, rationale, and description.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49624- 629Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Tarabrina  NLazebnaya  EZelenova  MLasko  N Chernobyl clean-up workers' perception of radiation threat.  Radiat Protection Dosimetry. 1996;68251- 255Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Derogatis  LR SCL-90-R: Administration, Scoring & Procedures Manual–II.  Towson, Md Psychometric Research1983;
54.
Turner  RJWheaton  BLloyd  DA The epidemiology of social stress.  Am Soc Rev. 1995;60104- 125Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Margolies  PJWeintraub  S The revised 56-item CRPBI as a research instrument: reliability and factor structure.  J Clin Psychol. 1977;33472- 476Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Bryk  ASRaudenbusch  SW Hierarchical Linear Models.  London, England Sage Publications1992;
57.
Litcher  LBromet  EJCarlson  GSquires  NGoldgaber  DPanina  NGolovakha  EGluzman  S School and neuropsychological performance of evacuated children in Kyiv eleven years after the Chornobyl disaster.  J Child Psychiatry Psychol. In press.Google Scholar
58.
Robbins  JSchneider  AB Radioiodine-induced thyroid cancer: studies in the aftermath of the accident at Chernobyl.  Trends Endocrinol Metab. 1998;987- 94Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Azarian  ASkriptchenko-Gregorian  VMiller  TWKraus  RF Childhood trauma in victims of the Armenian earthquake.  J Contemp Psychother. 1994;2477- 85Google ScholarCrossref
60.
Yule  WUdwin  RMurdoch  K The "Jupiter" sinking: effects on children's fears, depression and anxiety.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;311051- 1061Google ScholarCrossref
61.
Bromet  EJDew  MA Review of psychiatric epidemiologic research on disasters.  Epidemiol Rev. 1995;17113- 119Google Scholar
62.
Schwartz  SDohrenwend  BPLevav  I Nongenetic familial transmission of psychiatric disorders? evidence from children of Holocaust survivors.  J Health Soc Behav. 1994;35385- 402Google ScholarCrossref
63.
Rosenheck  RFontana  A Warrior fathers and warrior sons: intergenerational aspects of trauma. Danieli  Yed International Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York, NY Plenum Press1998;225- 242Google Scholar
64.
Briggs-Gowan  MJCarter  ASSchwab-Stone  M Discrepancies among mother, child, and teacher reports: examining the contributions of maternal depression and anxiety.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1996;24749- 764Google ScholarCrossref
65.
Spurgeon  AGompertz  DHarrington  JM Modifiers of non-specific symptoms in occupational and environmental syndromes.  Occup Environ Med. 1996;53361- 366Google ScholarCrossref
66.
Danieli  Y Introduction. Danieli  Yed International Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York, NY Plenum Press1998;1- 17Google Scholar
67.
Perrin  SLast  CG Do childhood anxiety measures measure anxiety?  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1992;20567- 578Google ScholarCrossref
68.
Slovic  P Perception of risk from radiation. Ricks  RBerger  MEO'Hara  RMeds The Medical Basis for Radiation Preparedness III The Psychological Perspective. New York, NY Elsevier1991;211- 227Google Scholar
Original Article
June 2000

Children's Well-being 11 Years After the Chornobyl Catastrophe

Author Affiliations

From the Departments of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science (Drs Bromet, Goldgaber, Carlson, and Schwartz) and Pathology (Dr Lyubsky), State University of New York at Stony Brook; Institutes of Sociology (Drs Panina and Golovakha) and Experimental Pathology, Oncology, and Radiobiology (Dr D. Gluzman), Academy of Sciences of the Ukraine, and Ukrainian-American Bureau for Protection of Human Rights and the Ukrainian Psychiatric Association (Dr S. Gluzman), Kyiv, Ukraine; Department of Family Medicine, Boston University, Boston, Mass (Dr Gilbert); and the Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Northport, NY (Dr Lyubsky).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(6):563-571. doi:10-1001/pubs.Arch Gen Psychiatry-ISSN-0003-990x-57-6-yoa9231
Abstract

Background  The psychological effects of technological disasters have rarely been studied in children. This study assessed the aftermath of the 1986 Chornobyl disaster in children evacuated to Kyiv from the contaminated zone surrounding the nuclear power facility.

Methods  In 1997, we evaluated three hundred 10- to 12-year-old children in Kyiv who were in utero or infants at the time of the disaster and who had resided near Chornobyl (evacuees) and 300 sex-matched homeroom classmates who had never lived in a radiation-contaminated area. Response rates were 92% (evacuees) and 85% (classmates). Data were obtained from children, mothers, and teachers using standard measures of well-being and risk factors for childhood psychopathology. The children also received physical examinations and basic blood tests.

Results  The evacuees and classmates perceived their mental health similarly except for Chornobyl-related anxiety symptoms and perceived scholastic competence. No differences were found on the Iowa Conners' Teacher Rating Scale. Although the physical examination and blood test results were normal, the evacuee mothers rated their children's well-being as significantly worse, especially with respect to somatic symptoms on the Children's Somatization Inventory and Child Behavior Checklist. The most important risk factors for these ratings were maternal somatization and Chornobyl-related stress.

Conclusions  Given the multiple stressful experiences to which evacuee families were exposed, the small differences in the children's self-reports suggest that there are protective factors in the lives of these children. The trauma experienced by the mothers was reflected in their perceptions of their children's well-being, particularly somatic symptoms, but was not transmitted to the children themselves.

THE ACCIDENT at the Chornobyl nuclear power plant began on April 26, 1986, and resulted in the permanent evacuation of 120,000 people primarily from the 30-km zone around the plant.1 The magnitude of the crisis was unprecedented. The secondary impacts included a sudden, disorderly evacuation, increased rate of abortions, difficulty obtaining official papers allowing evacuees to live in the relocation cities, conflicts over housing and benefits, being dismissed with labels such as "radiophobia," and inadequate medical care. Like other toxicological disasters, the Chornobyl accident created a subpopulation who live in a condition of threat.2 The one established consequence is an increase in thyroid cancer in youth,3 particularly in children who were between the ages of in utero to 2 years at the time of exposure.4

This is the first epidemiologic study of the psychological well-being of children caught up in the aftermath of the Chornobyl disaster. Our study represents a collaboration between independent scientists in Kyiv and an interdisciplinary US research team. We thus employ the original Ukrainian spelling for Chornobyl (Chernobyl) and Kyiv (Kiev).

Prior research on the psychological effects of radiation catastrophes focused primarily on adults.5 The evidence from studies of Hiroshima and Nagasaki indicates that generalized and health-focused anxiety, somatization, and depressive symptoms remained elevated 10 to 20 years later.6-8 Studies assessing the psychological aftermath of the 1979 accident at the Three Mile Island (TMI) nuclear power plant demonstrated similar acute,9-11 medium (5-6 years),12,13 and long-term (10 years) effects.14 Mothers of young children constituted a high-risk group,15 although the effects were not transmitted to their children.16-19

Several recent studies have addressed the psychosocial impact of the Chornobyl accident. Four Western-based studies with clearly explicated methodologies found that compared with controls, exposed adults, particularly women, had elevated somatization, anxiety, depressive, and posttraumatic stress symptoms during the first decade after the accident.20-25 In addition, a mortality study of Estonian liquidators (Chornobyl cleanup workers) found that suicide was the leading cause of death.26 Many Russian-based studies also reported long-term psychological effects, but the scientific validity of these reports is difficult to evaluate.1,27-29

Although exposed children have an elevated rate of thyroid cancer and are a major focus of humanitarian organizations, no systematic mental health data exist. Four sources of evidence suggest that exposed children constitute a high-risk group for psychological impairment. First, mothers of young children living in a contaminated region of Belarus were at high risk for psychopathology.30 Second, highly exposed children receive extensive annual checkups (some are hospitalized for up to 1 month for testing) and have a variety of diagnoses, including "vascular dystony" (also known as "vegetative dystonia"), that indicate symptoms such as headache, fatigue, dizziness, changes in blood pressure, and abdominal pain. This was the "official" diagnosis in Ukraine for Chornobyl-related conditions, based on tests not used in Western medicine.31 Third, although the assessment methods were unclear, one study reported greater emotional problems in exposed children in Belarus compared with controls.32 Finally, children studied after other disasters, particularly unexpected, severe traumatic events, are at increased risk for internalizing and externalizing symptoms.33-38

Our study examined whether, 11 years after the accident, the well-being of children evacuated to Kyiv was poorer than that of their classmates. We also considered the contributions of epidemiologic-based risk factors,39 including demographic characteristics, physical health, parental psychopathology, school performance, and quality of communication with parents, as well as maternal perceptions of Chornobyl-related stress.

Subjects and methods
Sample and setting

Chornobyl is located in northeastern Ukraine, 90 km north of Kyiv. When the accident occurred in 1986, the contamination was spotty but widespread. The 30-km zone surrounding the plant was permanently evacuated, although the undamaged reactors still function. This zone included small villages and the city of Pripyat (population approximately 50,000), built for the workers and their families. Many evacuees, including 20,000 people from Pripyat, were given temporary housing in Kyiv, which in time became their new home.

Kyiv received less radioactive contamination than other areas.1 Because residents of Kyiv were not informed about the magnitude of the accident, their children were outdoors for the May 1 celebrations. As news about Chornobyl spread, the Minister of Health appeared on television saying that the situation was under control, the accident was minor, and a little bit of radiation was "good for you." However, fear spread, and in the summer of 1986, many parents in Kyiv sent their children away.

Thus, while the parents of the evacuees and their classmates experienced a traumatic event in 1986, the evacuees underwent a series of traumas stemming from knowledge that they were exposed to high levels of radiation and to the tumultuous circumstances of the evacuation, relocation, and resettlement.

Our study focuses on children born between February 1, 1985, and January 31, 1987 (in utero to age 15 months when the accident occurred) now living in Kyiv. To identify these children, we merged the Ministry of Health's National Register of Persons Affected by Radiation as a Result of the Chornobyl Accident with lists of 2 large humanitarian organizations, Help for Families From Chornobyl, and Children of Chornobyl for Survival. We identified 721 children in the target age range, of whom 668 (92.6%) were on the National Register. In 1996 when our pilot work began, 693 of 721 were in Kyiv. Fifteen children were randomly selected for a pilot study. The remaining 678 names were randomized. Respondents were then selected sequentially until 300 interviews were completed.

The comparison group was composed of sex-matched, unexposed classmates from the same homeroom (up to 3 per evacuee were selected initially in case of refusal). These children had similar life experiences growing up in Kyiv. Access to schools was facilitated by the Director of Education of Kyiv.

The response rates were 92% (300/326) for evacuees (80.7% were from Pripyat) and 85% (300/352) for classmates.

Procedure

The study was conducted in 3 stages. Stage 1 entailed home interviews with the children and mothers during February through May 1997. The initial contact was made with mothers by telephone (or in person if the telephone was unavailable or shared). Written informed consent was obtained from mothers and children following a description of the study. The mothers were also informed that they would receive feedback about the findings when the study was completed.

The interviewers were employed by SOCIS-Gallup of Ukraine, an independent nongovernmental organization founded in 1992. Twenty interviewers conducted the assessments (10 for children and 10 for mothers). They had completed a general course on interviewing and worked on previous SOCIS projects. They were university graduates and were systematically trained for a period of 1 week.

Five checks were instituted to monitor the fieldwork: (1) completed interviews were immediately checked for missing data or inconsistencies; (2) a specially trained supervisor reviewed the response rate and interview content with each interviewer weekly; (3) a 10% random sample was recontacted and selected items verified; (4) mothers failing to participate in stage 2 were asked about potential problems during stage 1; and (5) the US investigators recontacted 20 randomly selected respondents. There was no evidence of field problems.

Stage 2 entailed basic physical examinations and blood tests and took place at the Center for the Rehabilitation of Victims of Torture and War approximately 2 to 4 weeks after the home interview. A total of 547 participants (91%) completed this stage, 275 evacuees and 272 classmates. Completers did not perceive their children's health differently from noncompleters. Three physicians were trained in the medical protocol by one of us (T.G.), who also confirmed the findings in 9 children. The blood samples were analyzed blindly at the Institute of Experimental Pathology, Oncology, and Radiobiology, Kyiv, and at the Veterans Affairs Medical Center in Northport, NY. The children were healthy except for minor seasonal problems (eg, swollen glands, inflamed tonsils). One evacuee girl had had her thyroid gland removed. The blood test results were in the normal range for both groups.

During stage 3, teacher ratings of child behavior and school record information were obtained in May (n=599).

Measures

The major concern about the measures was their cultural and conceptual appropriateness. In addition, because psychiatric epidemiology was unknown in Ukraine and clinicians were unavailable as interviewers, the decision was made to incorporate as much structure as possible. Standard translation and back-translation procedures were followed for US and Ukrainian measures. Eighty percent of mothers and 72% of children were interviewed in Russian and the remainder in Ukrainian.

Children's Self-reports

Six measures were included: (1) the Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale,40 the sum of 28 items rated 0=no and 1=yes (eg, I worry when I go to bed at night, I have trouble making up my mind) (α [Cronbach α]=.81); (2) the Depression Self-Rating Scale,41 the mean of 21 items rated 0=never to 3=all the time (eg, I look forward to things as much as I used to, I think life is not worth living) (α=.80); (3) the Fear Inventory, the mean of 47 current fears, adapted from the Fear Survey Schedule for Children,42 rated 0=not at all afraid to 4=very much (α=.94); (4) the Children's Somatization Inventory43 (CSI), the sum of 37 items rated 0=absent to 4=experienced a whole lot, such as headaches, sore muscles, and stomachaches (α=.94); (5) the short form of the Perceived Competence Scale for Children,44 the means for 6 subscales containing 4 items each, rated 1=low to 4=high level of competence (scholastic [α=.66]; social [α=.60]; athletic [α=.64]; physical [α=.57]; behavior [α=.64]; and self-worth [α=.50]); and (6) the Children's Chornobyl Anxiety Scale, the sum of 15 items modeled after Pynoos et al45 indicating degree of concern about Chornobyl, rated 0=absent to 4=always present (α=.86) (eg, Do you get scared or upset when you think about the Chornobyl disaster? Do you have bad dreams about Chornobyl?).

Maternal Reports of Child's Well-being

Two measures were included: (1) the Russian adaptation46 of the Child Behavior Checklist47 (CBCL) administered during stage 2 (n=546), composed of 118 items, rated 0=not at all true to 2=completely true, comprising 8 subscales—withdrawn (α=.72), somatic complaints (α=.80), anxious/depressed (α=.79), social problems (α=.64), thought problems (α=.51), attention problems (α=.75), delinquent behavior (α=.60), and aggressive behavior (α=.86); and (2) mothers' ratings on the CSI (M-CSI) (α=.91). The correlation between the M-CSI and the CBCL somatic complaints scale was 0.58 (P<.001).

Teacher Reports

The Iowa Conners' Teachers Rating Scale48 is the sum of 10 items, rated 0=not at all to 3=very much, covering behavioral symptoms, such as inattentive or uncooperative (α=.90).

Maternal Reports of Chornobyl-Related Stress

Three measures were analyzed: (1) the Chornobyl Health Stress Scale, an 8-item scale focused on the health effects of the Chornobyl disaster on mother and child, effects on future generations, and worries about eating or drinking contaminated food or milk (α=.77); (2) child diagnosed with vascular dystony (the official Chornobyl diagnosis) reflecting headaches, dizziness, blood pressure changes, and gastrointestinal tract upset; (3) the Impact of Events Scale–Revised49 (mean score) focused on Chornobyl-induced distress, assessing 22 avoidance, intrusion, and hyperarousal symptoms (α=.94) on a scale of 0=not at all to 4=very much.

Data analyses

Thirteen risk factors were analyzed: (1) child's age and (2) sex; (3) school performance (1=mostly Cs to 5=mostly As); (4) maternal education (1=less than high school; 6=advanced degree); (5) perceived standard of living (Likert scale developed in Kyiv: 0=lowest and 10=highest); (6) mother's and (7) teacher's ratings of the child's physical health (1=very bad to 5=excellent); (8) paternal alcohol abuse (mother's report of whether husband drinks too much [0=no; 1=yes, but not during the last 12 months; 2=yes, during the last 12 months]); (9) maternal lifetime depression (having the requisite symptoms for DSM-IV major depressive disorder based on a modified version of the depression module50 of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R51; (10) mother's Symptom Checklist-9052,53 somatization score (α=.89); (11) the sum of 23 life strain items rated by mothers as present ("somewhat" or "very true"), such as problems with finances, employment, or leisure time54 (α=.79); the child's perception of quality of communication with his or her mother (12) and father (13), based on the means of 10 items rated 1=never to 5=always (eg, How often do you talk things over with your mother/father? When you do something wrong, how long does your mother/father stay angry with you?)55 (α=.64 and .87 for mothers and fathers, respectively).

Means, medians, and percentages for the evacuees and matched classmates were compared using the 2-tailed paired t test, Wilcoxon signed rank test, and the McNemar test, respectively. P>.05 was considered nonsignificant. The effect size (difference in means divided by the pooled within-group SD) was used to evaluate the magnitude of group differences in well-being. Correlations were used to summarize the bivariate associations between Chornobyl-related stress and well-being. A series of mixed-level regression analyses56 was performed to estimate (1) the unique effects of the Chornobyl stress measures on well-being, controlling for the 13 putative epidemiologic risk factors, and (2) the group differences that remained after controlling for different sets of predictors. These analyses took account of the correlated residuals created by the matched design.

Results
Descriptive characteristics

The children were matched for sex; 48% were male. The median age of both groups was 11 years, although evacuees were slightly younger (10.7 ± 0.60 years vs 10.8 ± 0.79 years). Close to 40% of both groups had a grade average of B or better, and the quality of communication with parents was rated as positive (Table 1). Although the physical examination and blood test results were unremarkable, the teachers rated 24.7% of evacuees vs 10.7% of classmates as having "bad" or "very bad" health, and 36.7% of evacuee mothers compared with 14.0% of the classmate mothers rated their children's health as "bad" or "very bad."

The median ages of the mothers were late 30s and, like the children, the evacuee mothers were younger. The perceived standard of living was low in both groups. Most mothers were currently married, and the average number of children per family was 2. The evacuee mothers were less often university graduates or working outside the home; similarly, fewer evacuee fathers were university graduates (17.3% [46/266] vs 34.3% [91/266]; P<.001). The apartments given to the evacuees were somewhat larger (more rooms per person) than those of the classmates. Consistent with the children's age, relatively more evacuee mothers were pregnant at the time of the accident. More evacuee mothers met criteria for lifetime depression, and evacuee mothers had significantly higher Symptom Checklist-90 somatization scores. However, the classmate mothers reported more life strain overall.

Differences in child well-being and chornobyl stress

The groups were not significantly different on the Iowa Conners' Teachers Rating Scale or on 9 of the 11 children's self-report scales (Table 2). The 2 scales on which the evacuee children rated themselves more negatively (P≤.05) were scholastic competence and the Children's Chornobyl Anxiety Scale. Using the conventional criteria, these group differences were small to negligible (effect size approximately 0.2).

Both groups of mothers viewed their children similarly on several CBCL internalizing scales. However, the evacuee mothers perceived their children as substantially more symptomatic on the 2 somatization measures (effect size >.50), particularly the CSI. The evacuee mothers also reported higher CBCL thought problems scores, while the comparison mothers reported more problems on delinquent behavior, a difference that remained significant after controlling for age.

With respect to Chornobyl stress, the evacuee mothers had significantly higher scores on the Chornobyl Health Stress Scale (9.98 ± 2.23 vs 8.33 ± 2.65; paired t299=−6.41; P<.001; effect size=.48) and on the Impact of Events Scale (0.84 ± 0.67 vs 0.52 ± 0.55; paired t299=−8.38; P<.001; effect size=.37). In addition, 48% of evacuee vs 14% of comparison mothers reported that their child was diagnosed with vascular dystony (McNemar χ2=68.81; P<.001).

Bivariate relationships between chornobyl stress and child well-being

We next examined the relationships of the Chornobyl stress measures to child well-being in the evacuees and classmates. Given the number of correlations (3 stress variables × 22 measures × 2 groups), a Bonferroni-adjusted α of P<.001 was applied. The Chornobyl stress measures were only weakly related to the teacher rating scale and the child self-report scales. Among the latter, only 1 correlation coefficient approached statistical significance (that for the Chornobyl Health Stress Scale and the Children's Manifest Anxiety Scale [evacuees]; r=0.16; P=.007).

In contrast, for both groups, the Chornobyl Health Stress scale was significantly related (P<.001) to the CBCL somatic complaints (r=0.27 and 0.32 for evacuees and classmates, respectively) and the M-CSI (r=0.35 in both groups). The Impact of Events Scale was also significantly correlated with the somatization scales in both groups, particularly with the CSI (r=0.38 for evacuees and 0.25 for classmates; P<.001). Reports of vascular dystony were similarly correlated with the CSI (r=0.37 for evacuees and 0.26 for classmates; P<.001). In the evacuee (but not control) group, only 4 other correlations were significant (P<.001): Impact of Events Scale with the CBCL thought problems (r=0.22), anxious/depressed (r=0.28), and attention scales (r=0.21), and vascular dystony with the anxious/depressed scale (r=0.24).

Multivariate analysis

The final analysis considered the ability of the epidemiologic and Chornobyl risk factors to account for group differences in the measures of child well-being. Only the 5 variables on which the evacuee children were significantly more impaired than their classmates are presented.

A comparison of the unadjusted and adjusted group means (Table 3, rows 1 and 2) shows that the epidemiologic risk factors accounted for 50% to 75% of the observed group differences. For the 3 measures on which the unadjusted group differences were relatively small (effect size <.30), the group difference that remained after controlling for the risk factors was not statistically significant. However, the adjusted difference remained significant for the 2 somatization measures.

We note that in the epidemiologic multivariate models, the strongest predictors of (greater) scholastic competence (P<.001) were higher grades (t223=9.38), better quality of communication with mother (t223=4.89), and being male (t296=3.64). None of the variables was strongly associated (P<.001) with the Children's Chornobyl Anxiety Scale. The only significant predictor (P<.001) of CBCL thought problems was Symptom Checklist-90 somatization (t296=5.59). Finally, for children's somatization, the Symptom Checklist-90 somatization and mother's rating of her child's health were significant (for CBCL somatic complaints, t222=8.59 for the Symptom Checklist-90 and t222=4.94 for the health rating; for the CSI, t296=14.33 for the Symptom Checklist-90 and t296=7.16 for the health rating).

When the multivariate analysis was repeated controlling for Chornobyl stress (Table 3, row 3), the adjusted group differences in each of the well-being measures except scholastic competence were reduced substantially. In fact, the Chornobyl stress measures accounted for as much of the observed group difference in the child Chornobyl Anxiety Scale and more of the observed group difference in the 2 mother-rated somatic symptom measures than the epidemiologic risk factors. None of the group differences was statistically significant after controlling for Chornobyl-related stress.

Together the epidemiologic risk factors and the Chornobyl stress measures accounted for more than half of the observed group differences in the child reports and CBCL thought problems, and nearly all of the difference in the 2 somatization scales (Table 3, row 4 vs row 1). Furthermore, the remaining group differences on the 5 well-being measures were not significant. It should be noted that each Chornobyl stress measure was significantly associated with the somatization scales in the final models.

Comment

Our central objective was to determine whether the well-being of children evacuated to Kyiv from the contaminated area around Chornobyl was poorer than that of nonevacuee children. We found that 11 years after the accident, children who were infants when the explosion occurred perceived their well-being as similar to that of their classmates. This finding is consistent with our previous report showing no differences in neuropsychological test performance.57 The teachers and mothers perceived the children's behavior as similar in many respects. However, although the physical examination and blood test findings were similar, the evacuee mothers reported significantly more somatic symptoms in their children. Chornobyl stress was significantly related to these perceptions, even after adjusting for other risk factors. The strongest relationships were between the mother's somatization level and her reports of somatic symptoms in her child.

The project was modeled on our research on mothers of infants following the nuclear power plant accident at TMI 20 years ago. There too we found little difference between TMI and comparison site children despite significant differences in the mothers' mental health and persistent concerns about their children's health.16-18 Ten years after the TMI accident, when the children were about the same age as the Chornobyl sample, 49% of TMI mothers were worried about the effects of the accident on their children's health. In the present study, almost all of the women were at least somewhat worried, and 58% of evacuee mothers thought that their children's health was "very" much affected. Given the difference in the magnitude of these events, this result is not surprising. There were many other important differences besides the level of radiation exposure, of course, including the fact that the TMI families did not leave home permanently or experience the severe strains that occurred with the resettlement and breakup of the former Soviet Union. Thus, the parallels in the findings are all the more striking.

Like our TMI study, we had to select a single comparison group because of budgetary constraints. We note that although the contamination in Kyiv was far lower than in the areas adjacent to Chornobyl, the classmate group provided a conservative comparison since many Kyiv mothers sent their children away from May through August 1986 and worried about the radiation exposure their children received.

Several limitations should be noted. Because of difficulties in reconstructing radiation dose estimates,58 we could not determine the risk conferred by actual exposure level. On the other hand, the evacuees came from the 30-km zone, and 81% were from Pripyat. Second, the study design was cross-sectional, and the data were obtained 11 years after the explosion. We therefore do not know whether differences existed before the Chornobyl disaster happened or whether delayed effects will emerge as the children age. Third, families resettled in Kyiv are not representative of all residents of the 30-km zone. While we developed a reasonably complete enumeration of evacuees in Kyiv, it is impossible to specify the representativeness of our population. Fourth, the Chornobyl disaster unleashed a cascade of stressors, including harrowing experiences during the evacuation, arduous battles for residency permits in Kyiv and for government benefits, social stigma, and an irreversible loss of home, belongings, and lifestyle. The fact that there were multiple stressors is not unique to Chornobyl. As Azarian et al59 noted regarding the 1988 earthquake in Armenia: "The most distinctive characteristic of the children . . . is the multiplicity of stressors to which they were exposed as a result of the traumatization." However, it is difficult to disentangle the web of stress and pinpoint specific experiences that increased the vulnerability of the population. Finally, our study was a pioneering effort in Kyiv. Without prior experience doing epidemiologic research there, we attempted to impose as much structure as possible. Our measures appeared to have excellent internal consistency and concurrent validity. The CBCL scores were similar to those reported for a sample near Moscow, Russia.46 However, normative data for Ukraine are unavailable.

Based on previous disaster studies of children,33,38,60,61 we evaluated internalizing and behavioral symptoms. Unlike children exposed to acute disasters, the evacuee children seemed relatively unaffected by circumstances 11 years after Chornobyl. In some ways, the findings are consistent with studies of the offspring of trauma survivors that show very little effect on the next generation.62,63 However, while these children have no memory of witnessing the event, they are regularly exposed to the Chornobyl disaster by their parents and other circumstances (eg, being hospitalized every year for testing, being sent abroad in the summer for recuperation, and getting special lunches in school). It is common knowledge in Kyiv as to who the evacuee families are. The typical scene that occurred during our visits to families as we conducted the study was as follows: the child raced around the apartment while the parents made us comfortable; we discussed many topics amicably and with smiles; the subject of Chornobyl was brought up, and, in the child's presence, the mother related her worries about her child's health and showed us the child's medical record; the child witnessed the mother's sobbing about what the Chornobyl disaster had done. Thus, the children are keenly aware of their parents' reactions.

Carter et al46 showed that somatization was the one CBCL subscale that was elevated in a Russian sample relative to American norms. Somatic complaints are a sign of distress in Russia and Ukraine.28 Thus, it is not surprising that somatization symptoms were increased in the evacuee mothers and bore a strong relationship to perceptions of the children's well-being. In addition, the relationship between maternal somatization and mothers' reports of somatic symptoms in the children are consistent with other studies showing associations between maternal depression and mothers' reports of depressive symptoms in their children.64 An increase in perceived risk to health was reported after other technological disasters.65 While conceivably the mothers identified real symptoms that went undetected by our study physicians, it is also conceivable that they projected their own feelings on their reports about their children.

How do we account for the fact that the children were so resilient and that the differences in their self-ratings were so small? These children have grown up with consistent, loving parents. Indeed, the data on parent-child communication showed no significant differences between evacuees and classmates and, more importantly, showed scores indicating positive relationships with parents. Moreover, the correlation between the mothers' and children's perceptions of communication was moderately strong (r=0.30; P<.001). The trauma of Chornobyl was inflicted primarily on the parents and only secondarily on the children even though the children are the focus of their parents' anxiety. The way the trauma is handled in many households is by open discussion and grieving, as opposed to the "conspiracy of silence" that frequently occurred in families of Holocaust survivors.66 Conceivably, the open expression of emotion is a protective factor for these children. Finally, the children's reports of internalizing symptoms are subject to unreliability, and thus measurement error may have obscured real difficulties experienced by them.67 When the cohort enters adolescence, the evacuee children may indeed begin to be at greater risk for anxiety and depression, disorders whose rates increase with teenage years.

To conclude, while we enumerated the limitations of our study, the results should also be viewed against its strengths. These include a sample of evacuee families with no known bias, the classmate comparison group living in similar conditions, an excellent response rate, standard approaches to assessing risk factors and well-being, mental health data from the children, mothers, and teachers, and physical examinations and blood tests performed by experienced and knowledgeable physicians. Although radiation and nuclear power evoke deeply rooted fear and anxiety in adults,68 our study found that 11 years after the explosion, the trauma was not transmitted to children who were unborn or infants when their families were resettled in Kyiv.

Accepted for publication January 24, 2000.

This study was supported by a National Institute of Mental Health personal services contract in 1992 (Dr Bromet) and by grant MH51947 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md.

We are indebted to many professionals in Kyiv, Ukraine, for making this study a success, including N. Churilov, PhD, Director of SOCIS-Gallup in Kyiv, and the many interviewers and supervisory staff who ably performed the field work for the study; A. Gorbachik, PhD, biostatistician; and V. Grigorenko, MD, M. Abramova, MD, and the staff of the Center for the Rehabilitation of Victims of Torture and War who performed the physical examinations. We also wish to thank colleagues L. Jandorf, MA, L. Litcher, BA, R. Adams, PhD, and E. Polenova, MA, State University of New York at Stony Brook, for assisting with various aspects of the project, and B. Moscato, PhD, State University of New York at Buffalo, for invaluable advice and help throughout the fieldwork. We are very grateful to Ints Calitis from Riga, Latvia, and Sergey Fedorinchik from Kyiv for introducing our project to key professionals and organizations in Kyiv. Most of all, we are indebted to the participants of this study who graciously provided us with precious information and insights about their lives despite of the pain they endure on retelling their Chornobyl experiences.

Reprints: Evelyn J. Bromet, PhD, Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Science, State University of New York at Stony Brook, Putnam Hall–South Campus, Stony Brook, NY 11794-8790 (e-mail: ebromet@mail.psychiatry.sunysb.edu).

References
1.
Bard  DVerger  PHubert  P Chernobyl, 10 years after: health consequences.  Epidemiol Rev. 1997;19187- 204Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Kroll-Smith  JSCouch  SR Technological hazards, adaptation and social change. Couch  SRKroll-Smith  JSeds Communities at Risk Collective Responses to Technological Hazards. New York, NY Peter Lang1991;293- 316Google Scholar
3.
Williams  D Thyroid cancer and the Chernobyl accident.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;816- 7Google Scholar
4.
Nikiforov  YGnepp  DRFagin  JA Thyroid lesions in children and adolescents after the Chernobyl disaster: implications for the study of radiation tumorigenesis.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;819- 14Google Scholar
5.
Bromet  E Psychological effects of radiation catastrophes. Peterson  LEAbrahamson  Seds Effects of Ionizing Radiation Atomic Bomb Survivors and Their Children (1945-1995). Washington, DC Joseph Henry Press1998;283- 294Google Scholar
6.
Lifton  RJ Death in Life: Survivors of Hiroshima.  New York, NY Random House1967;
7.
Misao  THattori  KShirakawa  MSuga  MOgawa  NOhara  YOhno  TFukuta  HHadama  TKuwahara  HFukamachi  KHitsumoto  SKamatani  T Characteristics in abnormalities observed in atom-bombed survivors.  J Radiat Res (Tokyo). 1961;285- 97Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Yamada  MKodama  KWong  FL The long-term psychological sequelae of atomic-bomb survivors in Hiroshima and Nagasaki. Ricks  RBerger  MEO'Hara  RMeds The Medical Basis for Radiation Preparedness III The Psychological Perspective. New York, NY Elsevier1991;155- 163Google Scholar
9.
Dohrenwend  BP Psychological implications of nuclear accidents: the case of Three Mile Island.  Bull N Y Acad Med. 1983;591060- 1076Google Scholar
10.
Bromet  EJParkinson  DKSchulberg  HCDunn  LOGondek  PC Mental health of residents near the Three Mile Island reactor: a comparative study of selected groups.  J Prev Psychiatry. 1982;1225- 275Google Scholar
11.
Baum  AFleming  I Implications of psychological research on stress and technological accidents.  Am Psychol. June1993;665- 672Google Scholar
12.
Davidson  LMBaum  A Chronic stress and posttraumatic stress disorders.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1986;54303- 308Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Dew  MABromet  EJSchulberg  HCDunn  LOParkinson  DK Mental health effects of the Three Mile Island nuclear reactor restart.  Am J Psychiatry. 1987;1441074- 1077Google Scholar
14.
Dew  MABromet  EJ Predictors of temporal patterns of psychiatric distress during 10 years following the nuclear accident at Three Mile Island.  Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1993;2849- 55Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Bromet  EJSchulberg  HC The TMI disaster: a search for high risk groups. Shore  JHed Disaster Stress Studies New Methods and Findings. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Press1986;2- 19Google Scholar
16.
Bromet  EHough  LConnell  M Mental health of children near the Three Mile Island reactor.  J Prev Psychiatry. 1984;2275- 301Google Scholar
17.
Cornely  PBromet  E Prevalence of behavior problems in three-year-old children living near Three Mile Island: a comparative analysis.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1986;27489- 498Google ScholarCrossref
18.
Cohen  SBromet  E Maternal predictors of behavioral disturbance in preschool children: a research note.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33941- 946Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Pass  J 18-Year Follow-Up of Children Living Near Three Mile Island: INTEL Science Report.  Setauket, NY Ward Melville High School1999;
20.
Cwikel  JAbdelgani  AGoldsmith  JRQuastel  MYevelson  II Two-year follow-up study of stress-related disorders among immigrants to Israel from the Chornobyl area.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;1051545- 1550Google Scholar
21.
Quastel  MRGoldsmith  JRCwikel  JMerkin  LWishkerman  VYPoljak  SAbdelgani  AKordysh  EDouvdevani  ALevy  JGorodisher  RBarki  YEmerit  IKramer  G Lessons learned from the study of immigrants to Israel from areas of Russia, Belarus, and Ukraine contaminated by the Chernobyl accident.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;1051523- 1527Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Havenaar  JMVan den Brink  Wvan den Bout  JKasyanenko  APPoelijoe  NWWholfarth  TMeijler-Iljina  LI Mental health problems in the Gomel region (Belarus): an analysis of risk factors in an area affected by the Chernobyl disaster.  Psychol Med. 1996;26845- 55Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Havenaar  JMRumyantzeva  GKasyanenko  AKaasjager  KWestermann  Avan den Brink  Wvan den Bout  JSavelkoul  J Health effects of the Chernobyl disaster: illness or illness behavior? a comparative general health survey in two former Soviet regions.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;1051533- 1537Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Havenaar  JMvan den Brink  W Psychological factors affecting health after toxicological disasters.  Clin Psychol Rev. 1997;17359- 374Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Viinamäki  HKumpusalo  EMyllykangas  MSalomaa  SKumpusalo  LKolmakov  SIlchenko  IZhukowsky  GNissinen  A The Chernobyl accident and mental wellbeing: a population study.  Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1995;91396- 401Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Rahu  MTekkel  MVeidebaum  TPukkala  EHakulinen  TAuvinen  ARytomaa  TInskip  PDBoice  JD  Jr The Estonian study of Chernobyl cleanup workers, II: incidence of cancer and mortality.  Radiat Res. 1997;147653- 657Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Baverstock  KF Perspectives in radiation and health: reflections on the International Conference in Beer Sheva.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;105(suppl 6)1611- 1617Google Scholar
28.
Havenaar  JMvan den Brink  W Psychological factors affecting health after toxicological disasters.  Clin Psychol Rev 1997;17359- 374Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Yevelson  IIAbdelgani  ACwikel  JYevelson  IS Bridging the gap in mental health approaches between East and West: the psychosocial consequences of radiation exposure.  Environ Health Perspect. 1997;105(suppl 6)1551- 1556Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Havenaar  JMRumyantzeva  GMVandenbrink  WPoelijoe  NWVandenbout  JVanengeland  HKoeter  MWJ Long-term mental health effects of the Chernobyl disaster: an epidemiologic survey in two former Soviet regions.  Am J Psychiatry. 1997;1541605- 1607Google Scholar
31.
Stiehm  ER The psychologic fallout from Chernobyl.  AJDC. 1992;146761- 762Google Scholar
32.
Kolominsky  YIgumnov  SDrozdovitch  V The psychological development of children from Belarus exposed in the prenatal period to radiation from the Chernobyl atomic power plant.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40299- 305Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Aptekar  LBoore  J The emotional effects of disaster on children: a review of the literature.  Int J Ment Health. 1990;1977- 90Google Scholar
34.
Green  BKorol  MGrace  MVary  MLeonard  AGleser  GSmitson-Cohen  S Children and disaster: age, gender, and parental effects on PTSD symptoms.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1991;30945- 951Google ScholarCrossref
35.
McFarlane  APolicansky  SIrwin  C A longitudinal study of the psychological morbidity in children due to a natural disaster.  Psychol Med. 1987;17727- 738Google ScholarCrossref
36.
Raphael  B When Disaster Strikes.  New York, NY Basic Books1986;
37.
Saylor  CFed Children and Disasters.  New York, NY Plenum Press1993;
38.
Vogel  JVernberg  EM Children's psychological responses to disasters: part 1.  J Clin Child Psychol. 1993;22464- 484Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Verhulst  FCKoot  HM Child Psychatric Epidemiology: Concepts, Methods, and Findings.  London, England Sage Publications1992;
40.
Reynolds  CRRichmond  BO What I think and feel: a revised measure of children's manifest anxiety.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1978;6271- 280Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Birleson  P The validity of depressive disorder in childhood and the development of a self-rating scale: a research report.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1981;2273- 88Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Scherer  MWNakamura  CY A Fear Survey Schedule for Children (FSS-FC): a factor analytic comparison with manifest anxiety (CMAS).  Behav Res Ther. 1968;6173- 182Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Garber  JWalker  LSZeman  J Somatization symptoms in a community sample of children and adolescents: further validation of the Children's Somatization Inventory.  Psychol Assess. 1991;3588- 595Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Harter  S The Perceived Competence Scale for Children.  Child Dev. 1982;5387- 97Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Pynoos  RSFrederick  CNader  KArroyo  WSteinberg  AEth  SNunez  FFairbanks  L Life threat and posttraumatic stress in school-age children.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;441057- 1063Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Carter  ASGrigorenko  ELPauls  DL A Russian adaptation of the Child Behavior Checklist: psychometric properties and associations with child and maternal affective symptomatology and maternal functioning.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1995;23661- 684Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Achenbach  TM Manual for the CBCL/4-18 and 1991 Profile.  Burlington University of Vermont Dept of Psychiatry1991;
48.
Loney  JMilich  R Hyperactivity, inattention, and aggression in clinical practice. Wolraich  MRouth  DKeds Advances in Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics. Vol 3 Greenwich, Conn JAI Press1982;113- 147Google Scholar
49.
Weiss  DSMarmar  CR The Impact of Events Scale–Revised. Wilson  JPKeane  TMeds Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD A Handbook for Practitioners. New York, NY Guilford1997;399- 411Google Scholar
50.
Phelan  JSchwartz  JEBromet  EJDew  MAParkinson  DKSchulberg  HCDunn  LOBlane  HCurtis  EC Work stress, family stress and depression in professional and managerial employees.  Psychol Med. 1991;21999- 1012Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Spitzer  RLWilliams  JBWGibbon  MFirst  MB The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R (SCID), I: history, rationale, and description.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49624- 629Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Tarabrina  NLazebnaya  EZelenova  MLasko  N Chernobyl clean-up workers' perception of radiation threat.  Radiat Protection Dosimetry. 1996;68251- 255Google ScholarCrossref
53.
Derogatis  LR SCL-90-R: Administration, Scoring & Procedures Manual–II.  Towson, Md Psychometric Research1983;
54.
Turner  RJWheaton  BLloyd  DA The epidemiology of social stress.  Am Soc Rev. 1995;60104- 125Google ScholarCrossref
55.
Margolies  PJWeintraub  S The revised 56-item CRPBI as a research instrument: reliability and factor structure.  J Clin Psychol. 1977;33472- 476Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Bryk  ASRaudenbusch  SW Hierarchical Linear Models.  London, England Sage Publications1992;
57.
Litcher  LBromet  EJCarlson  GSquires  NGoldgaber  DPanina  NGolovakha  EGluzman  S School and neuropsychological performance of evacuated children in Kyiv eleven years after the Chornobyl disaster.  J Child Psychiatry Psychol. In press.Google Scholar
58.
Robbins  JSchneider  AB Radioiodine-induced thyroid cancer: studies in the aftermath of the accident at Chernobyl.  Trends Endocrinol Metab. 1998;987- 94Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Azarian  ASkriptchenko-Gregorian  VMiller  TWKraus  RF Childhood trauma in victims of the Armenian earthquake.  J Contemp Psychother. 1994;2477- 85Google ScholarCrossref
60.
Yule  WUdwin  RMurdoch  K The "Jupiter" sinking: effects on children's fears, depression and anxiety.  J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1990;311051- 1061Google ScholarCrossref
61.
Bromet  EJDew  MA Review of psychiatric epidemiologic research on disasters.  Epidemiol Rev. 1995;17113- 119Google Scholar
62.
Schwartz  SDohrenwend  BPLevav  I Nongenetic familial transmission of psychiatric disorders? evidence from children of Holocaust survivors.  J Health Soc Behav. 1994;35385- 402Google ScholarCrossref
63.
Rosenheck  RFontana  A Warrior fathers and warrior sons: intergenerational aspects of trauma. Danieli  Yed International Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York, NY Plenum Press1998;225- 242Google Scholar
64.
Briggs-Gowan  MJCarter  ASSchwab-Stone  M Discrepancies among mother, child, and teacher reports: examining the contributions of maternal depression and anxiety.  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1996;24749- 764Google ScholarCrossref
65.
Spurgeon  AGompertz  DHarrington  JM Modifiers of non-specific symptoms in occupational and environmental syndromes.  Occup Environ Med. 1996;53361- 366Google ScholarCrossref
66.
Danieli  Y Introduction. Danieli  Yed International Handbook of Multigenerational Legacies of Trauma. New York, NY Plenum Press1998;1- 17Google Scholar
67.
Perrin  SLast  CG Do childhood anxiety measures measure anxiety?  J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1992;20567- 578Google ScholarCrossref
68.
Slovic  P Perception of risk from radiation. Ricks  RBerger  MEO'Hara  RMeds The Medical Basis for Radiation Preparedness III The Psychological Perspective. New York, NY Elsevier1991;211- 227Google Scholar
×