Background
Neuroanatomic studies of schizophrenia have reported temporolimbic abnormalities. Most magnetic resonance imaging studies have evaluated small samples of primarily men with chronic schizophrenia. Our goal was to evaluate sex differences in segmented temporal lobe subregions with reliable parcellation methods, relating volume with clinical and neurocognitive parameters.
Methods
Magnetic resonance imaging was performed in 100 patients with schizophrenia (58 men, 42 women; 39 neuroleptic naive, 61 previously treated) and 110 healthy controls (51 men, 59 women). Gray and white matter volumes of temporolimbic (hippocampus and amygdala) and neocortical regions (superior temporal gyrus and temporal pole) were examined. Symptoms, functioning, and neurocognition were assessed concurrently.
Results
Hippocampal gray matter volume was reduced in men (7%) and women (8.5%) with schizophrenia. In the amygdala, however, decreased volume was evident for men (8%) whereas women (10.5%) had increased volume. Magnetic resonance imaging of the temporal pole showed decreased gray matter in men (10%) and women (8.5%). For the superior temporal gyrus, the decrease exceeded that of whole-brain only in men (11.5%). Volumes were largely uncorrelated with clinical measures, but higher hippocampal volumes were associated with better memory performance for all groups. Cortical volumes were associated with better memory performance in healthy women.
Conclusions
Schizophrenia is associated with reduced gray matter volume in temporolimbic structures. In men, reduction was manifested in all regions, whereas women showed decreased hippocampal volumes but increased amygdala volumes. The abnormalities are evident in patients with first-episode schizophrenia and correlate more strongly with cognitive performance than with symptom severity.
TEMPORAL LOBE structures and circuitry modulate cognition and emotion, prompting their neuroanatomic examination in schizophrenia. Investigations with magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) have evaluated lobar volumes1-5 and specific subregions6-15 implicated in neurobehavioral domains aberrant in schizophrenia, such as memory. Regional volumes have also been linked to clinical features, including thought disorder and auditory hallucinations.7,8 This research complements neuropathological findings documenting abnormal morphometry, cytoarchitecture, and neuroreceptors in temporolimbic structures.16-18
Studies have reported volume reduction in the global temporal lobe,1 gray matter (GM), but not white matter (WM),2,11 with lateralization to the left hemisphere.3 Regional analysis revealed decreased volume in temporolimbic structures including the hippocampus,6,7,9,11,12,19-22 parahippocampal gyrus,7,23 and amygdala.19,20 However, some studies did not find differences in these regions.2,13,14,24-27 A meta-analysis28 examined 18 studies measuring hippocampal volumes, sometimes combined with amygdala. Bilateral hippocampal reduction had a significant effect on size, and adding amygdala further increased it. Laterality effects were inconclusive.28
Cortical temporal regions, especially the superior temporal gyrus (STG), have also been examined.29 Reduced volume has been observed in anterior,8,30 posterior,7,31 and total STG21,32 and is related to the severity of auditory hallucinations8 and thought disorder.7 However, other studies have not noted STG decrease.33-35 It is also unclear whether STG reduction is differential compared with other temporal lobe association cortices.
Conclusions are limited by variation in imaging parameters affecting resolution, variation in regional boundary definitions, and small sample sizes that include primarily men. Sex differences have been noted clinically, with women having later onset, milder course, and more affective symptoms.36,37 Neurocognitively, however, reports vary.38-40 Our goal was to evaluate GM and WM volumes of temporolimbic and neocortical areas in men and women with schizophrenia. We hypothesized that: (1) schizophrenia is associated with lower GM volumes across temporolimbic regions; (2) the reduction is more evident in men than women; (3) volume reduction is observed at initial clinical presentation; (4) higher volume is associated with better memory performance in patients and controls; and (5) no directional hypothesis is offered relating volume with symptom severity, but we expect higher volumes to be associated with better functioning.
The sample included 100 patients with schizophrenia (58 men, 42 women) and 110 healthy controls (51 men, 59 women) from the Schizophrenia Research Center (Philadelphia, Pa). Participants were right-handed and aged 18 to 45 years. They represented a subsample for whom we reported whole-brain data41 and an expanded sample of participants in the prefrontal study.42 The current sample does not differ from the other samples in demographic or clinical characteristics (Table 1 and Table 2). Patients had a DSM-IV43 diagnosis of schizophrenia established by research psychiatrists as previously detailed.44,45 The healthy controls also underwent comprehensive assessment including medical, neurological, and psychiatric evaluations with laboratory tests.46,47 Participants had no history of a disorder or event that might affect brain function (eg, substance use or dependence, hypertension, cerebrovascular disease, seizure disorder, head trauma with loss of consciousness, or endocrine disorder). After complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained prior to participation. Clinical assessments, neurocognitive testing, and MRI studies were conducted within a week.
Assessments, conducted by research psychiatrists with established procedures,44 included the Scale for Assessment of Negative Symptoms48 (SANS), Scale for Assessment of Positive Symptoms49 (SAPS), the Hamilton Depression Scale50 (HAM-D), Premorbid Adjustment Scale51 (PAS), and Quality of Life Scale.52 The sample was mildly to moderately impaired (Table 3).
A standardized battery and procedures provided measures of abstraction-flexibility, attention, verbal memory, spatial memory, verbal abilities, and spatial abilities.53,54 Testing was done by trained fellows supervised by investigators.
Magnetic resonance imaging scans were acquired as detailed41,42: a GE Signa (General Electric Co, Milwaukee, Wis) 1.5-T system was used, along with a spoiled gradient-recalled echo sequence; we used a 35° flip angle; repetition time was 35 milliseconds; echo time was 6 milliseconds; field of view was 24 cm; there was 1 repetition, a 1-mm slice thickness, no gaps, transaxial images, and 0.9375 × 0.9375-mm resolution. Images were realigned as in the prefrontal analyses, resliced along the anterior commissure–posterior commissure axis for head tilt, and none had parenchymal lesions or skull abnormalities. The brain volume was extracted semiautomatically and segmented into GM and WM using the optimal thresholding and morphological operations previously detailed.55,56
The temporal lobe was divided into limbic (hippocampus and amygdala) and cortical regions (STG and temporal pole [TP]). Regions were drawn on the realigned sagittal series with the exception of the medial aspect of STG, which uses the realigned coronal series (Figure 1).
The GM structure lying on the lateral ventricle is bounded inferiorly by the WM, separating it from the parahippocampal cortex. Laterally and posteriorly it is bounded by a WM region, the alveus, which separates the tail of the hippocampus from the atrium of the lateral ventricle, thus excluding the choroid plexus. On the more lateral slices, the anterior border is defined by the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle. On the more medial slices, a small strip of WM separates the hippocampus from the amygdala. The coordinates of the border established by the WM are maintained on those slices where there is no white strip present. The outline of the hippocampus is traced as it appears on each slice.
The drawing for the amygdala is performed on the sagittal plane but all 3 planes are used to determine the borders. The superior border is determined in 2 steps. First, the coronal slice cutting through the most inferior-anterior point of the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle is chosen from the sagittal plane. Then, on the coronal plane, the most lateral point of cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) where the chiasmatic cistern meets the amygdala provides the linear superior border on the sagittal plane. The anterior border is determined by the caudal coronal slice in which the anterior commissure disappears and the third ventricle becomes continuous. This coronal slice is used as the anterior border on the sagittal plane. The inferior border of the amygdala is determined by the axial slice on which the tip of the inferior horn of the lateral ventricle first appears. The posterior border of the amygdala is drawn adjacent to the anterior border of the hippocampus.
The anterior border is defined by a vertical line representing the most posterior CSF pixel in front of the limen insula. This anterior point remains consistent on the sagittal slices cutting through the insular cortex. The posterior border is determined by a vertical line where the lateral fissure is capped by the supramarginal gyrus at the slice lateral to the appearance of the insular gyri. The superior and inferior borders are determined by the CSF of the lateral fissure and the superior temporal sulcus, respectively. Tracing of the region on the sagittal plane ends when the inferior temporal gyrus becomes discontinuous. The drawings from the sagittal plane are displayed on the coronal plane using 3-dimensional imaging software, whereupon the remaining medial portion of the gyrus is drawn. The gyrus is still bounded inferiorly by the superior temporal sulcus and superiorly by the CSF of the lateral fissure. The medial border is defined by a line connecting the most inferior point of the insular cortex to the most medial point of the superior temporal sulcus.
The posterior border of this region is defined as the anterior border of STG. Anteriorly it is bounded by the sphenoid bone, inferiorly by the temporal bone (articular tubercle), and superiorly by the lateral fissure. The region is then drawn in both the lateral and medial directions until there is no longer brain anterior to the established posterior border. Based on the defining boundaries, the TP includes the anterior middle and inferior temporal gyri. It may also include the most anterior segment of STG that is difficult to separate reliably.
Two raters independently completed the temporal region drawings on the same 10 randomly selected cases of 5 controls and 5 patients. The intraclass correlations for the 4 subfields in each hemisphere for GM and WM ranged from 0.90 to 0.96.
Brain volumes in milliliters were the dependent measures in the analyses. Since only GM was present for the hippocampus and amygdala, they were each analyzed using univariate analyses of covariance (ANCOVA), with diagnosis and sex as grouping factors, hemisphere as a repeated-measures (within-group) factor, and total cranial, brain, GM volumes, and age as sequential covariates. For temporal cortex (STG and TP), where both GM and WM were measured, a multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was conducted where a compartment (GM, WM) was added as a repeated-measures factor. These analyses tested hypotheses 1 and 2. Because covariates other than total cranial volume, necessary for equating men and women, did not alter the effects, we report the results of the MANCOVAs with cranial volume covaried. Analyses of variance were performed within the patient group, contrasting first-episode neuroleptic-naive to previously treated patients (hypothesis 3) and comparing deficit with nondeficit subtypes.57 As these analyses did not show group effects or interactions, these results are not detailed.
To examine the relationship between volumes and neurocognitive functioning, we computed the correlations between GM volume in the above subregions and performance on the 6 neurocognitive domains. Two domains, verbal and spatial memory, are hypothesized to relate to temporal lobe functioning (hypothesis 4). This was tested with a Pearson correlation coefficient with α level set at 0.05. The other 4 correlations were considered exploratory and the P value was Bonferroni adjusted so that a P value of .01 (0.05/4) was considered significant at P = .05. Similarly, the possible link between volumes and clinical variables was examined by correlating the temporal subregions' GM with global measures of function (PAS and Quality of Life Scale), where positive correlations are expected with volumes (hypothesis 5) and severity of symptoms (SANS, SAPS, HAM-D), where we make no directional hypothesis. Here P values were Bonferroni adjusted using the 5 measures in the denominator, so that a P value of .01 was considered significant at P = .05. All P values were 2 tailed.
Magnetic resonance imaging
The ANCOVA for the hippocampus showed a main grouping effect of diagnosis (F2,191 = 3.53; P = .03), indicating that patients had overall smaller hippocampal volumes (Table 4). No other main effects or interactions were significant. The ANCOVA for amygdala showed no main effects of diagnosis or sex, but a diagnosis × sex interaction was significant (F1,192 = 4.21; P = .04). This reflected reduced volume in men relative to increased volume in women with schizophrenia, compared with their healthy counterparts (Figure 2). The MANCOVA for STG showed a main effect of diagnosis (F4,189 = 5.47; P<.001), with patients having lower volumes than controls. There was a main effect of compartment (F1,192 = 4.12; P = .04) showing overall higher GM than WM, and a diagnosis × compartment interaction (F1,192 = 21.70; P = .001), indicating that the reduction in STG volume seen in patients was specific to GM. No other main effects or interactions involving diagnosis were significant. For TP, the MANCOVA showed a main effect of diagnosis (F4,189 = 2.78; P = .03) with patients having lower volumes than controls. A compartment × diagnosis interaction (F1,192 = 9.50; P = .002) reflected that the reduced parenchymal volume in patients was specific to GM. Again, no other effects or interactions were significant.
Covarying age in the data analyses was justified by volume correlations with age, within the limited range. These correlations were significant only for cortical GM, and not for WM or subcortical regions (amygdala and hippocampus). For healthy men, increased age was associated with decreased GM volume in STG (r39 = −0.39; P = .01). In healthy women, the corresponding correlations were r54 = −0.26; P = .05 and r = −0.30, P = .03, respectively. In men with schizophrenia, only TP volume correlated with age (r56 = −0.35; P = .008) and for women with schizophrenia the correlation was significant only for STG (r40 = −0.31, P = .05). Correlations partialling age were not different from the raw correlations reported below.
Correlation of mri with assessment measures
Because the differences between patients and controls were in GM, only GM volumes were correlated with the clinical symptoms. In men with schizophrenia, lower hippocampus volume correlated with poorer PAS (r56 = −0.34; P = .02). No other correlations were significant after Bonferroni adjustment. There were no correlations between volumes and illness duration.
The hypothesized correlations between volumes and memory were significant for the hippocampus in healthy men (verbal, r39 = 0.30; P = .05; spatial, r = 0.37; P = .02), healthy women (spatial, r54 = 0.35; P<.007), men with schizophrenia (verbal, r56 = 0.35; P<.02), and women with schizophrenia (verbal, r40 = 0.26; P = .05; spatial, r = 0.29; P = .05). Amygdala volume did not correlate significantly with performance on any neurocognitive domain. Superior temporal gyrus volume correlated significantly with spatial memory in healthy women (r = 0.36; P = .005). Temporal pole volume correlated with verbal and spatial memory in healthy women (r = 0.27; P = .04 and r = 0.36; P = .005, respectively). Other correlations that withstood Bonferroni adjustment included STG with attention in healthy men (r = 0.38; P = .05) and TP with abstraction (r = 0.45; P = .009) and spatial abilities (r = 0.38; P = .004) in healthy women.
Examination of cortical and subcortical temporal regions supported the hypothesized volume reduction in schizophrenia and its specificity to GM. This is consistent with findings of whole-brain41,58,59 and temporal subregion volume decrease.1 We previously reported an average decrease in GM volume of 6% for men and 3% for women with schizophrenia, relative to healthy controls.41 By evaluating both neocortical and temporolimbic structures, we could assess the nature and extent of volume change for specific temporal regions. The decreased volume is evident across the sample for the hippocampus, where both men (7%) and women (8.5%) with schizophrenia manifest comparable decrease in volume. For the amygdala, however, sex differences were observed. While men (8%) with schizophrenia show decrease in volume, women (10.5%) display increased volume. For the cortical regions, STG was substantially decreased in men (11.5%) but for women (4%), the decrease did not exceed that seen for the whole brain. Both patient groups showed decreased volume of TP (10% for men, 8.5% for women). Thus, it appears that hippocampal and TP volume are reduced in both men and women with schizophrenia. However, consistent with the hypothesis, volume reduction was more diffuse in men.
Our finding on the hippocampus supports the conclusion of the meta-analysis of Nelson et al28 that hippocampal volume is reduced in schizophrenia. However, we did not find generalized reduction in amygdala volume as implied by their observation that the inclusion of amygdala enhanced the effect size. Most patients in previous studies were men. Our results indicate that the hippocampal finding is also evident in women, but reduced amygdala volume is seen only in men with schizophrenia. Our results also support the hypothesis that temporal lobe abnormalities are not correlated with duration of illness and are observed in neuroleptic-naive patients. Therefore, they do not reflect treatment effects or chronicity.
An association between hippocampal volume and clinical measures was noted only in men, where lower volume was related to poorer premorbid functioning. The sparse and modest correlations between volumes and clinical measures are noteworthy and consistent with other studies41,42 that suggest that the neuroanatomic abnormalities may underlie more enduring disease features than those reflected in cross-sectional clinical ratings.
The hypothesized association between temporal volumes and memory was confirmed for the hippocampus, where volume correlated with better performance in all groups. In healthy women, volume was associated with better memory also for cortical temporal regions. Patients overall showed similar correlations to controls, as they did for the prefrontal regions.42 This may suggest that while volume reduction is associated with poorer neurocognitive abilities, it does not alter the association between neuronal integrity and performance. It is noteworthy that amygdala volume did not correlate with neurocognitive measures in any group. This supports the notion that hippocampal and cortical temporal regions have a greater role in cognition60,61 than the amygdala that has been associated with emotions.62,63 In the prefrontal subregions we observed correlations with cognitive measures for dorsal and not orbital cortex. There is considerable evidence that the amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex play a role in mediating emotion processing.64,65 The connectivity among regions can be examined by correlating anatomic and physiologic features with combined use of structural and functional imaging.
No laterality effects were observed for temporal subregions. Some studies have reported lateralized volume reduction in subcortical7,28 and cortical areas.29 However, this has not been a consistent finding.28 We noted relative increase in glucose metabolism for the left medial temporal region in schizophrenia,66 which was associated with poorer memory.67 The symmetric temporal lobe volume reduction may suggest that relative increased left hemispheric activity is not secondary to lateralized tissue loss. However, as with connectivity, testing such hypotheses requires combined imaging of structure and function.68
The study has several limitations. Our sample included young adult patients with mild to moderate symptoms and without comorbidity. The generalizability of findings to older adults with a broader range of symptoms merits further investigation. The neuroanatomic sectors contain functionally distinct subdivisions, which could be differentially affected. More advanced procedures for automated feature analysis are needed. Given the sex differences in the amygdala, measures of emotion processing are lacking.
The anatomic data on frontal and temporal subregions indicate volume reductions in GM that generally exceed whole-brain changes. Lower volumes are evident at first presentation and are unrelated to illness duration. However, the effects in some regions are moderated by sex. The main difference between men and women with schizophrenia is that in men regions related to cognitive processing are predominantly reduced, whereas in women the abnormalities include parts of the neural system related to emotion processing. We also noted that neuroanatomic measures were unrelated to symptom severity assessed cross-sectionally. However, modest associations were established with neurocognitive performance, and these showed some specificity. Thus, prefrontal volumes correlated with attention and abstraction performance, while temporolimbic volume correlated with memory measures. These correlations were seen in patients and controls, suggesting similar coupling between brain volume and performance.
Accepted for publication March 24, 2000.
This research was supported by grants MH-42191, MH-43880, MH-01336, and MO1RR0040 from the National Institute of Health, Bethesda, Md.
We thank Tamara Kostick for assistance in manuscript preparation.
Corresponding author: Raquel E. Gur, MD, PhD, Neuropsychiatry Program, University of Pennsylvania, 3400 Spruce St, 10th Floor, Gates Building, Philadelphia, PA 19104.
1.Dauphinais
DDelisi
LECrow
TJAlexandropolous
KColter
NTuma
IGerson
ES Reduction in temporal lobe size in siblings with schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study.
Psychiatry Res. 1990;35137- 147
Google ScholarCrossref 2.Zipursky
RBMarsh
LLim
KODeMent
SShear
PKSullivan
EVGreer
MMCsernansky
JGPfefferbaum
A Volumetric MRI assessment of temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1994;35501- 516
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Turetsky
BTCowell
PEGur
RCGrossman
RIShtasel
DLGur
RE Frontal and temporal lobe brain volumes in schizophrenia: relationship to symptomatology and clinical subtype.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;521061- 1070
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Cowell
PEKostianovsky
DJGur
RCTuretsky
BIGur
RE Sex differences in neuroanatomical and clinical correlations in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1996;153799- 805
Google Scholar 5.Sullivan
EVLim
KOMathalon
DHMarsh
LBeal
DMHarris
DHoff
ALFaustman
WOPfefferbaum
A A profile of cortical gray matter volume deficits characteristic of schizophrenia.
Cereb Cortex. 1998;8117- 124
Google ScholarCrossref 6.Bogerts
BAshtari
MDegreef
GAlvir
JMJBilder
RMLieberman
JA Reduced temporal limbic structure volumes on magnetic resonance images in first episode schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res. 1990;351- 13
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Shenton
MEKikinis
RJolesz
FAPollak
SDLeMay
MWible
CGHokama
HMartin
JMetcalf
DColeman
MMcCarley
RW Abnormalities of the left temporal lobe and thought disorder in schizophrenia: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.
N Engl J Med. 1992;327604- 612
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Barta
PEPearlson
GDPowers
RERichards
SSTune
LE Auditory hallucinations and smaller superior temporal gyral volume in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1990;1471457- 1462
Google Scholar 9.Bogerts
BLieberman
JAAshtari
MBilder
RMDegreef
GLerner
GJohns
CMasiar
S Hippocampus-amygdala volumes and psychopathology in chronic schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1993;33236- 246
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Fukuzako
HFukuzako
THashiguchi
THokazono
YTakeuchi
KHirakawa
KUeyama
KTakigawa
MKajiya
YNakgo
MFujimoto
T Reduction in hippocampal formation volume is caused mainly by its shortening in chronic schizophrenia: assessment by MRI.
Biol Psychiatry. 1996;39938- 945
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Suddath
RLChristison
GWTorrey
EFCasanova
MFWeinberger
DR Anatomical abnormalities in the brains of monozygotic twins discordant for schizophrenia.
N Engl J Med. 1990;322789- 794
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Becker
TElmer
KMechela
BSchneider
FTaubert
SSchroth
GGrodd
WBartels
MBeckmann
H MRI findings in medial temporal lobe structures in schizophrenia.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 1990;183- 86
Google ScholarCrossref 13.DeLisi
LEHoff
ALSchwartz
JEShields
GWHalthore
SNGupta
SMHenn
FAAnand
AK Brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenic-like psychotic patients: a quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry. 1991;29159- 175
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Swayze
VW
IIAndreasen
NCAlliger
RJYuh
WTCEhrhardt
JC Subcortical and temporal structures in affective disorder and schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry. 1992;31221- 240
Google ScholarCrossref 15.Whitworth
ABHoneder
MKremser
CKemmler
GFelber
SHausmann
AWanko
CWechdorn
HAichner
FStuppaeck
CHFleischhacker
WW Hippocampal volume reduction in male schizophrenic patients.
Schizophr Res. 1998;3173- 81
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Shapiro
RM Regional neuropathology in schizophrenia: where are we? where are we going?
Schizophr Res. 1993;10187- 239
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Bogerts
B The temporolimbic system theory of positive schizophrenic symptoms.
Schizophr Bull. 1997;23423- 435
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Arnold
SETrojanowski
JQ Recent advances in the neuropathology of schizophrenia.
Acta Neuropathol. 1996;92217- 231
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Breier
ABuchanan
RWElkashef
AMunson
RCKirkpatrick
BGellad
F Brain morphology and schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging study of limbic, prefrontal cortex, and caudate structures.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49921- 926
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Rossi
AStratta
PMancini
FGallucci
MMattel
PCore
LDiMichelle
VCasacchia
M Magnetic resonance imaging findings of amygdala-anterior hippocampus shrinkage in male patients with schizophrenia.
Psychiatry Res. 1994;5243- 53
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Flaum
MSwayze
VW
IIO'Leary
DSYuh
WTCEhrhardt
JCArndt
SVAndreasen
NC Effects of diagnosis, laterality, and gender on brain morphology in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1995;152704- 714
Google Scholar 22.Buchanan
RWBreier
AKirkpatrick
BElkashef
AMunson
RCGellad
FCarpenter
WT Structural abnormalities in deficit and nondeficit schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1993;15059- 65
Google Scholar 23.Brown
RColter
NCorsellis
JACrow
TJFrith
CDJagoe
RJohnstone
ECMarsh
L Postmortem evidence of structural brain changes in schizophrenia: differences in brain weight, temporal horn area, and parahippocampal gyrus compared with affective disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;4336- 42
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Kelsoe
JRCadet
JLPickar
DWeinberger
DR Quantitative neuroanatomy in schizophrenia: a controlled magnetic resonance imaging study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;45533- 541
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Blackwood
DHRYoung
AHMcQueen
JKMartin
MJRoxborough
HMMuir
WJSt Clair
DMKean
DM Magnetic resonance imaging in schizophrenia: altered brain morphology associated with P300 abnormalities and eye tracking dysfunction.
Biol Psychiatry. 1991;30753- 769
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Young
AHBlackwood
DHRRoxborough
HMcQueen
JKMartin
MJKean
M A magnetic resonance imaging study of schizophrenia: brain structure and clinical symptoms.
Br J Psychiatry. 1991;158158- 164
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Torres
IJFlashman
LAO'Leary
DSSwayze
VAndreasen
NC Lack of an association between delayed memory and hippocampal and temporal lobe size in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls.
Biol Psychiatry. 1992;31221- 240
Google Scholar 28.Nelson
MDSaykin
AJFlashman
LARiordan
HJ Hippocampal volume reduction in schizophrenia as assessed by magnetic resonance imaging: a meta-analytic study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55433- 440
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Petty
RG Structural asymmetries of the human brain and their disturbance in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1999;25121- 139
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Pearlson
GDBarta
PEPowers
REMenon
RRRichards
SSAylward
EHFederman
EBChase
GAPetty
RGTien
AY Medial and superior temporal gyral volumes and cerebral asymmetry in schizophrenia versus bipolar disorder.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;411- 14
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Menon
RRBarta
PEAylward
DH Posterior superior temporal gyrus in schizophrenia: gray matter changes and clinical correlates.
Schizophr Res. 1995;16121- 126
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Marsh
LLim
KOHoff
ALHarris
DBeal
MMinn
KFaustman
WOCsernansky
JGSullivan
EVPfefferbaum
A Severity of schizophrenia and magnetic resonance imaging abnormalities: a comparison of state and veterans hospital patients.
Biol Psychiatry. 1999;4549- 61
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Vita
ADieci
MGiobbio
GMCaputo
AGhiringhelli
LComazzi
MGarbarini
MMendin
APMorganti
CTenconi
FCesana
BInvernizzi
G Language and thought disorder in schizophrenia: brain morphological correlates.
Schizophr Res. 1995;15243- 251
Google ScholarCrossref 34.DeLisi
LEHoff
ALChance
NKushner
M Asymmetries in the superior temporal lobe in male and female first-episode schizophrenic patients: measures of the planum temporale and superior temporal gyrus by MRI.
Schizophr Res. 1994;1219- 28
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Kulynych
JJVladar
KJones
DWWeinberger
DR Superior temporal gyrus volume in schizophrenia: a study using MRI morphometry assisted by surface rendering.
Am J Psychiatry. 1996;15350- 56
Google Scholar 36.Gur
REPetty
RGTuretsky
BIGur
RC Schizophrenia throughout life: sex differences in severity and profile of symptoms.
Schizophr Res. 1996;211- 12
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Hafner
HMaurer
KLoffler
WRiecher-Rossler
A The influence of age and sex on the onset and early course of schizophrenia.
Br J Psychiatry. 1993;16280- 86
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Goldstein
JMSeidman
LJGoodman
JMKoren
DLee
HWeintraub
STsuang
MT Are there sex differences in neuropsychological functions among patients with schizophrenia?
Am J Psychiatry. 1998;1551358- 1364
Google Scholar 39.Hoff
ALWieneke
MFaustman
WOHoron
RSakuma
MBlankfeld
HEspinoza
SDeLisi
LE Sex differences in neuropsychological functioning of first-episode and chronically ill schizophrenic patients.
Am J Psychiatry. 1998;1551437- 1439
Google Scholar 40.Lewine
RJWalker
EFShurett
RCaudle
JHaden
C Sex differences in neuropsychological functioning among schizophrenic patients.
Am J Psychiatry. 1996;1531178- 1184
Google Scholar 41.Gur
RETuretsky
BIBilker
WBGur
RC Reduced gray matter volume in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1999;56905- 911
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Gur
RECowell
PELatshaw
ATuretsky
BIGrossman
RIArnold
SEBilker
WBGur
RC Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57761- 768
Google ScholarCrossref 43.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
44.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JBWGibbon
M
Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV. Patient Version (SCID-P). New York New York State Psychiatric Institute1994;
Google Scholar 45.Gur
REMozley
DResnick
SMLevick
SErwin
RSaykin
AJGur
RC Relations among clinical scales in schizophrenia: overlap and subtypes.
Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148472- 478
Google Scholar 46.First
MBSpitzer
RLGibbon
MWilliams
JBW Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders: Non-Patient Edition (SCID-NP). New York New York State Psychiatric Institute/Biometrics Research Dept1995;
47.Shtasel
DLGur
REMozley
PDRichards
JTaleff
MMHeimberg
CGallacher
FGur
RC Volunteers for biomedical research: recruitment and screening of normal controls.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;481022- 1025
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Andreasen
NC The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City The University of Iowa1984;
49.Andreasen
NC The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City The University of Iowa1984;
51.Harris
JG An abbreviated form of the Phillips Rating Scale of Premorbid Adjustment in schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1975;84129- 137
Google ScholarCrossref 52.Henrichs
DWHanlon
TECarpenter
WT The quality of life scale: an instrument for rating the schizophrenic deficit syndrome.
Schizophr Bull. 1984;10388- 398
Google ScholarCrossref 53.Saykin
AJShtasel
DLGur
REKester
DBMozley
LHStafiniak
PGur
RC Neuropsychological deficits in neuroleptic naive, first episode schizophrenic patients.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51124- 131
Google ScholarCrossref 54.Ragland
JDGur
REKlimas
BCMcGrady
NGur
RC Neuropsychological laterality induces of schizophrenia: interactions with gender.
Schizophr Bull. 1999;2579- 89
Google ScholarCrossref 55.Yan
MXHKarp
JS Segmentation of 3D MR using an adaptive K-means clustering algorithm.
Proc IEEE Med Imaging Conf. 1994;41529- 1533
Google Scholar 56.Gur
RCTuretsky
BIMatsui
MYan
MBilker
WHughett
PGur
RE Sex differences in brain gray and white matter in healthy young adults: correlations with cognitive performance.
J Neurosci. 1999;194065- 4072
Google Scholar 57.Carpenter
WTHeinrichs
DWWagman
AMI Deficit and nondeficit forms of schizophrenia: the concept.
Am J Psychiatry. 1988;145578- 583
Google Scholar 58.Lim
KOHarris
DBeal
MHoff
ALMinn
KCsernansky
JGFaustman
WOMarsh
LSullivan
EVPfefferbaum
A Gray matter deficits in young onset schizophrenia are independent of age of onset.
Biol Psychiatry. 1996;404- 13
Google ScholarCrossref 59.Zipursky
RBLambe
EKKapur
SMikulis
DJ Cerebral gray matter deficits in first episode psychosis.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55540- 546
Google ScholarCrossref 61.Squire
LRZola
SM Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1996;9313515- 13522
Google ScholarCrossref 65.Damasio
AR Emotion in the perspective of an integrated nervous system.
Brain Res Brain Res Rev. 1998;2683- 86
Google ScholarCrossref 66.Gur
REResnick
SMAlavi
AGur
RCCaroff
SDann
RSilver
FSaykin
AJChawluk
JBKushner
MReivich
M Regional brain function in schizophrenia, I: a positron emission tomography study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44119- 125
Google ScholarCrossref 67.Harper
LGur
RCGur
REMozley
PDAlavi
A Relationships between verbal memory performance and the cerebral distribution of fluorodeoxyglucose in patients with schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1996;40443- 451
Google ScholarCrossref 68.Weinberger
DRBerman
KFSuddath
RLTorrey
EF Evidence of dysfunction of a prefrontal-limbic network in schizophrenia: a magnetic resonance imaging and regional cerebral blood flow study of discordant monozygotic twins.
Am J Psychiatry. 1992;149890- 897
Google Scholar