Background
Schizophrenia is associated with large effect-size deficits in auditory sensory processing, as reflected in impaired delayed-tone matching performance. The deficit may reflect either impaired sensory precision, which would be indicative of neural dysfunction within auditory sensory (temporal) regions, or of increased distractibility, which would be indicative of impaired prefrontal function. The present study evaluates susceptibility of schizophrenic subjects to same-modality distraction to determine whether patients fit a "bitemporal" or "prefrontal" model of sensory dysfunction.
Methods
Tone-matching ability was evaluated in 15 first-episode patients, 18 outpatients with chronic illness, and 21 patients in long-term residential care, relative to 32 nonpsychiatric controls of a similar age. A staircase procedure determined individual thresholds for attaining criterion level correct performance.
Results
Tone-matching thresholds in the absence of distractors were significantly elevated in patients in long-term residential care relative to all other groups (P<.001). The effect size (d) of the difference relative to controls was extremely large (SD, 1.95). Schizophrenic patients, even those with elevated tone-matching thresholds, showed no increased susceptibility to auditory distraction (P = .42). Deficits in tone-matching performance in subjects with chronic illness could not be attributed to medication status or level of symptoms.
Conclusions
These findings suggest that sensory processing dysfunction in schizophrenia is particularly severe in a subgroup of patients who can be considered poor-outcome based on their need for long-term residential treatment. Furthermore, the absence of increased auditory distractibility argues against prefrontal dysfunction as an origin for auditory sensory imprecision in schizophrenia.
COGNITIVE dysfunction is a significant and enduring feature of schizophrenia and a primary predictor of poor outcome and long-term disability.1-3 Information-processing deficits are most commonly investigated in schizophrenia using complex stimuli and tasks that activate wide regions of cortex and subcortical systems.4-7 However, recent studies have demonstrated that schizophrenic subjects are significantly impaired even in routine processing of simple stimuli. Such processing may occur preattentively and require involvement only of limited areas of cortex. Thus, for example, chronic schizophrenic subjects show significant impairment in the generation of mismatch negativity (MMN),8-12 an event-related potential component that is generated within primary auditory cortex.13 Relatedly, patients are significantly impaired in the ability to perform the simple cognitive task of matching 2 similar tones separated by a brief delay.14-16 Similar deficits are present in other sensory modalities,17-19 indicating widespread dysfunction of sensory, along with cognitive, processing in schizophrenia.
Critical structures regulating tone-matching ability reside within both auditory sensory and prefrontal cortical regions. Lesions to both these areas may disrupt tone-matching performance20-28 and lead to impaired MMN generation.29,30 However, the contribution of these structures to tone-matching performance differs considerably. Monkeys with bilateral lesions of superior temporal plane involving auditory cortex show increased discrimination thresholds (ie, require greater pitch difference between tones to perform an auditory discrimination) even when only simple tone pairs are presented.20-24 In contrast, monkeys with prefrontal lesions show elevated discrimination thresholds only when distractor stimuli are interposed into the tone sequence.22 A similar dissociation is seen after bitemporal25,26 vs prefrontal27,28 cortical damage in humans. Thus, the relative contribution to sensory vs prefrontal cortex to tone-matching deficits can be assessed by analyzing tone-matching dysfunction in schizophrenia in the absence vs presence of same-modality distractor stimuli.
This study evaluates the hypothesis that tone-matching deficits in schizophrenia are caused by or reflect sensory, rather than prefrontal level dysfunction, and reflect decreased precision of tonal representations within auditory sensory regions. Tone-matching performance was assessed in the absence and presence of same-modality distractors. In addition, for this study, patients were recruited from first-episode, outpatient, and state-hospital inpatient and/or residential care settings to also evaluate between-measure relationships across a broad range of subject functionality.
This study included 54 individuals with schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder diagnosed according to DSM-IV31 criteria and 32 nonpsychiatric comparison subjects of similar age and sex (Table 1). All subjects provided voluntary informed consent. All patients were receiving antipsychotic medications (typical, atypical, or combination) at the time of testing (Table 1). One quarter of patients were also receiving anticholinergics. Diagnoses were established by a board-certified attending research psychiatrist (D.C.J.) or a licensed research psychologist (E.F.R. and R.G.) using a semistructured clinical and/or a Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders,32 medical record review, and discussion with mental health professionals familiar with the case. Subjects with a DSM-IV Axis I diagnosis other than schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, including psychotic mood disorder, alcoholism, or other substance abuse during the past year, were excluded from the study, as were patients with clinically apparent neurological abnormalities. Also excluded were any individuals with significant musical training or ability, as well as any controls who were receiving any psychiatric medications or who had a history of significant Axis I diagnosis. All subjects were comparably compensated at the completion of testing.
The patients were recruited from 3 sites (Table 1). First-episode (FE) patients (n = 15) were recruited from the Hillside Hospital–Long Island Jewish study of first-episode schizophrenia.33 This group included 9 patients who were considered stabilized (namely, without active psychotic symptoms), and who were either working or in school, as well as 6 patients who were persistently symptomatic and in day treatment. Outpatients with chronic illness (n = 18) were recruited from the Day Treatment Center of the Rockland County Robert Jeager Health facility, Pomona, NY. These patients lived either at home or in community residences, but were primarily responsible for their own daily living needs. Only 1 patient within this group had been hospitalized within a state hospital within the preceding 5 years (Table 1). The last patient group (n = 21) consisted of 13 individuals residing in a residential aftercare facility located on the premises of Rockland Psychiatric Center, along with 8 inpatients at the Bronx Psychiatric Center, a New York State long-term care facility. Eighteen of the 21 patients had been hospitalized within a state institution during the preceding 5 years (mean time in hospital, 872 days). All patients were receiving active treatment at the time of testing (Table 1).
Comparison subjects included 32 individuals who were associated personnel of the institutions from which the patient samples were drawn. Controls were divided by age into 2 groups. Young comparison subjects (n = 18) were 34 years or younger, corresponding to the age range of the FE patient group. Old comparison subjects (n = 14) were older than 34 years, and were comparable with the residential care subjects in age. Intelligence quotient (IQ) was assessed for all subjects by the Ammons and Ammons Quick Test.34
Tones were derived from 3 reference base frequencies (500, 1000, and 2000 Hz). Comparison tones were created that differed by a specified percentage change in frequency (Δf). Levels of Δf used for testing were 1%, 2%, 2.5%, 3%, 5%, 7.5%, 10%, 15%, 20%, 30%, 40%, 50%, 75%, and 100%. All tones were 100 milliseconds in duration and were presented at a nominal intensity level of 75 dB. Blocks of 24 tone pairs were presented for each level of Δf. Within each block, half of the tone pairs contained identical tones, and half contained pairs of tones that differed to a fixed degree. In half of the "different" trials, the lower-pitched tone was presented first; in half, the higher-pitched tone.
In both no-distractor and distractor conditions, the interval between reference and test tones was 2 seconds. In the no-distractor condition, the interval was silent. In the distractor condition, a composite 1000-millisecond distractor stimulus was presented starting 500 milliseconds after the reference tone and terminating 500 milliseconds before the test tone. The distractor stimulus consisted of a series (5 each) of alternating low- and high-pitched 100-millisecond tones. Distractor tones were chosen to fall outside the range of the reference and test tones to be compared.
Before the formal testing, subjects were acclimated to the testing apparatus and procedures. A brief screening hearing test was administered to ensure that all subjects could comfortably detect the presented tones and to familiarize subjects with the test tones. For this test, tones were presented at intensities 10 dB lower than those used in the experimental conditions. Twelve tones (500, 1000, or 2000 Hz) were presented at a nominal intensity of 65 dB. Subjects were instructed to respond with their hand or verbally when they heard each tone. All subjects were able to detect all 12 tones. For a subset of patients, 2 additional screening tests were employed to ensure their comprehension of the experimental procedures. One involved their correctly reporting the number of tones presented (1-3 tones) in a short sequence. The second consisted of discriminating tones from white noise. This task was formally similar to the tone discrimination task, except that stimuli consisted either of pairs of identical tones or identical noise bursts, or a tone paired with a 100-millisecond white-noise burst. Subjects had to respond "same" or "different" after each pair. Three patients from the residential care group were excluded because of an inability to comprehend the basic task requirements. Only subjects who successfully completed the pretests were retained for the study.
In the experimental conditions, tone-matching performance was first assessed under a baseline (no-distractor) condition. Testing was initiated at 20% Δf, and the level of between-tone pitch difference was adjusted upward or downward based on subject performance. For each subject, threshold was defined as the smallest level of Δf at which a criterion of 87.5% or more correct performance (at least 21 of 24 correct responses) was achieved. The Δf for the baseline condition was recorded and used as the primary measure of tone-matching ability within each group.
Testing in the distractor condition was initiated at the Δf that corresponded to the individual's tone-matching threshold obtained under no-distractor conditions. The percentage of correct responses under this condition was recorded, and used, relative to baseline performance, as the primary measure of susceptibility to distraction within each group. Since performance was typically worse in the distractor condition, subjects were then tested at ascending levels of Δf (descending levels of difficulty) until a level was reached at which criterion was again obtained (21 of 24 correct responses). After establishment of their distractor theshold, subjects were retested in the no-distractor condition at their previously obtained no-distractor threshold.
The primary dependent variables consisted of the level required for threshold performance under baseline (no-distractor) conditions and the change in performance with introduction of distractors. Threshold Δf level after introduction of distractors and change in threshold as a function of distraction were treated as additional analysis variables. Threshold Δf values were log-transformed before analysis. Between-group differences were analyzed using 1-way analysis of variance, with post hoc least significant difference testing using SPSS for Windows (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill). Differential between-group effects of distraction were analyzed using repeated-measures analysis of variance with within-group factor of distraction (absent or present). Potential contributions of demographic (eg, age, sex, IQ) or clinical (eg, length of illness, Clinical Global Impression) variables were evaluated by analysis of covariance. Relationship between primary dependent variables and other subject variables were also assessed by follow-up multiple regression analysis. Two-tailed tests with α level of significance of P<.05 were used throughout. Effect sizes and power values were interpreted according to the convention of Cohen.35,36 Values in text represent mean + SD.
Tone-matching performance under baseline (no-distractor) conditions
Tone-matching in the absence of distractor stimuli (Figure 1, open bars) varied significantly across groups (F4, 81 = 11.6, P<.001) with residential care patients showing substantially worse performance than subjects in other groups (least significant difference, P<.001). Patients in long-term residential care needed a 20% difference in pitch between reference and test tones to detect a between-tone pitch difference, whereas other groups required only a 3% to 5% difference. The effect size (d) of the difference between subjects in long-term residential care and controls was extremely large (SD, 1.95). Only 6% (2 of 32) of controls needed more than a 10% difference in tones to obtain criterion level performance, as compared with 71% (15 of 21) of patients in long-term residential care (χ21 = 26.6, P<.001).
First-episode and patients with chronic illness did not show significant threshold elevations, and effect sizes relative to controls were small to moderate (0.58 and 0.55, respectively). However, 33% of FE patients (5 of 15) required more than a 10% difference in tones to obtain criterion level performance (χ21 = 5.8, P = .02 vs control), indicating a greater number of outliers in this group.
Tone-matching performance was evaluated at individually determined Δf threshold at both the beginning and end of the test sessions (Table 2). Only patients in long-term residential care showed a significant change in performance from test to retest (paired t = 3.0, P = .007), indicating that initial testing may, in fact, have underestimated their degree of impairment on this measure.
Across-group susceptibility to distraction
Susceptibility to distraction was assessed first by evaluating the degree to which the introduction of distractors degraded tone-matching performance assessed at no-distractor threshold (Table 2), and, second, by evaluating the degree to which between-tone pitch separation had to be increased for subjects to regain threshold (Figure 1). Similar results were obtained from the 2 analyses.
All groups showed a significant degradation in performance with the introduction of distractors, indicating effectiveness of the distraction manipulation (F1, 80 = 176.3, P<.0001 across groups). For all groups, the correct performance rate in the presence of distractors was approximately 20 percentage points lower than in the absence of distractors (Table 2). Schizophrenia subjects showed no more degradation in performance than did controls with the introduction of distractor stimuli (Table 2) (F4, 80 = 0.98, P = .42). Power for rejecting a between-group difference was greater than 0.75. All groups continued to perform significantly above chance (50% correct) even in the distractor condition.
Once distractors were introduced, more than 75% of subjects required a greater level of Δf to attain threshold level performance than they had under baseline conditions. This included 8 subjects in long-term residential care who were unable to regain threshold even at the highest levels of Δf tested and were therefore considered to have increased thresholds. Patients who were unable to regain threshold in the presence of distractors had higher baseline thresholds than those who were able to regain threshold (mean Δf = 43% vs 22%). This finding may indicate that beyond a certain level, increasing between-tone pitch separation does not increase discriminability, as has previously been observed in the case of MMN.11
The percentage of subjects whose thresholds increased with distraction did not differ across groups (χ24 = 4.8, P = .31). The degree to which Δf levels needed to be increased in subjects for whom distractor thresholds could be obtained was also similar across groups (Figure 1, difference between open and solid bars). Tone-matching thresholds in the presence of distractors were significantly greater for inpatient subjects than for controls or other patient groups (F4, 73 = 8.8, P<.001). However, this difference was accounted for by the elevated tone-matching thresholds in this group obtained in the absence of distraction. No significant difference was observed between groups after covariation for no-distractor thresholds (F4, 71 = 1.0, P = .41).
Patient groups differed significantly in terms of Clinical Global Impression scores and duration of illness (Table 1). However, when these factors were entered as covariates into an analysis of covariance of tone-matching threshold vs patient group, the between-group differences in tone-matching performance remained significant (F4, 47 = 4.5, P = .004). Furthermore, in this analysis, neither Clinical Global Impression score nor duration of illness was independently predictive of impaired tone-matching performance. When age, Clinical Global Impression score, Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale symptom ratings, hospitalization length, and medication status were entered into a logistic regression equation vs residential care/outpatient status for patients with chronic illness, tone-matching performance emerged as the strongest correlate of hospitalization status (R = 0.30, P = .009). Negative, but not positive, symptom severity also correlated significantly with site of recruitment (R = 0.23, P = .03). No other variables were significantly correlated.
Correlational analyses were also performed to evaluate potential effects of medication. Antipsychotic dose varied significantly across groups (Table 1). However, there was no significant correlation between dose and performance either within or across groups (F1, 41 = 1.9, P = .20). Moreover, between-group differences in performance remained significant after covariation for antipsychotic dose (F2, 41 = 5.2, P = .01). Patients receiving atypical antipsychotics performed similarly to those using typical agents (F1, 31 = 0.45, P = .50).
Patients receiving anticholinergic medications performed significantly worse than those not receiving anticholinergics (F1, 39 = 5.44, P = .02). However, similar percentages of subjects were receiving anticholinergic medications in all patient groups and no significant group×anticholinergic treatment interaction was observed (F2, 39 = 1.42, P = .30). Neither antidepressant medications nor benzodiazepines were significantly associated with performance.
The present study investigated the degree to which auditory sensory deficits, as manifest by impaired delayed tone-matching performance, correspond to a sensory-level vs prefrontal pattern of dysfunction. There are 2 major findings from the present study. First, this study confirms previous observations of elevated tone-matching thresholds in chronic schizophrenic inpatients relative to controls.15,16,37 As in prior studies of tone matching, the effect size of the deficit is large, and at least comparable in magnitude to deficits observed in more complex tasks.7 Second, this study demonstrates that despite increases in tone-matching threshold, schizophrenic subjects are no more sensitive to the effects of same modality distractors than are controls. This latter finding contrasts with the pattern of deficit observed in patients with prefrontal damage,27,28,38 and strongly supports the concept that tone-matching deficits in schizophrenia are caused by or reflect dysfunction at the level of sensory cortex.
A second goal of this study was to evaluate the relationship between deficits in tone-matching performance in schizophrenia and clinical and demographic characteristics. We have previously observed deficits in tone-matching performance in several separate cohorts of chronic schizophrenic subjects recruited predominantly from a state-hospital inpatient setting.15,16,37 In these previous studies, effect sizes were large, and more than 80% of subjects could be correctly classified based on tone-matching performance alone. In contrast, a recent study in which patients were drawn primarily from an outpatient setting39 reported significant deficits in tone matching, but the percentage of patients with deficits, 34% (13 of 38), was relatively smaller than we had previously observed. Results of the present study clarify the apparent discrepancy between prior studies, and demonstrate that patients drawn from an inpatient and/or residential care setting are significantly more impaired in performance than those drawn from outpatient settings. The validity of subdividing patients according to recruitment site is supported by our observation that only 1 of the 18 subjects in the outpatient group had required long-term (state hospital) hospitalization during the 5 years before recruitment, as compared with 18 of the 21 subjects in long-term residential care.
It is unclear the degree to which sensory processing dysfunction, as reflected in impaired tone-matching performance (but presumably affecting other aspects of sensory processing as well), contributes to the need for persistent hospitalization vs the degree to which it indexes a form of schizophrenia associated with poorer functional outcome. In the present study, patients in long-term residential care showed significantly greater levels of negative symptoms than the outpatients with chronic illness but did not differ significantly in either global symptom ratings or in severity of positive symptoms. Tone-matching deficits, therefore, cannot be attributed simply to greater "severity" of illness, although such deficits may index a form of schizophrenia particularly associated with negative symptoms. The observation that roughly 33% of first-episode subjects showed elevated tone-matching thresholds may indicate etiological heterogeneity at first episode on the basis of either neurochemical or neuroanatomical differentiation. Whether tone-matching deficits at illness onset are predictive of poor outcome, however, requires longitudinal follow-up.
Even patients with significant elevation of tone-matching thresholds were no more affected by distractors than controls. The fact that resistance to distraction was preserved, even in subjects with elevated tone-matching thresholds, argues against this finding as being a manifestation of impaired attention, motivation, or cooperation.40 To the extent that the ability to maintain a tone representation in the presence of distraction represents a prefrontal function,28,41 inpatients with chronic illness were no more impaired in this aspect of prefrontal function than were other patient groups or controls. This finding does not imply that all aspects of prefrontal functioning are normal in schizophrenia. Rather, it indicates, first, that any study of higher-order processing in schizophrenia must be corrected for potential sensory level disturbance, and, second, that some elements of prefrontal function are preserved.
The present data suggest that information-processing deficits in schizophrenia may depend more on the types of processes that are involved than on the specific regions of cortex that are engaged. In support, it has recently been demonstrated that schizophrenialike deficits in both MMN generation (a sensory level process) and AX-type Continuous Performance Test performance (a prefrontal-type task)42 are induced by administration of the N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor antagonist ketamine.43 In both cases, the overall level of performance was diminished. However, rates of decay in performance as a function of interstimulus interval in the AX-type Continuous Performance Test were unaffected, as has previously been observed in schizophrenia.12,17,44-46 Thus, patients may have increased difficulty "constructing a representation of an object,"46 be it a sensory or cognitive construct, but be relatively unimpaired in retaining it if interfering stimuli are presented.
The present study is limited by the fact that all subjects were receiving medication. However, the mean dose of medication was highly similar for outpatients with chronic illness and long-term residential care subjects. Nevertheless, the 2 groups differed significantly in tone-matching threshold. Thus, the sensory processing deficits in the residential care group cannot be attributed to antipsychotic effect. Across groups, there was a significant deleterious effect of anticholinergic medication on performance. This finding is consistent with considerable research demonstrating anticholinergic-induced impairments in cognitive performance.47-49 However, the percentage of patients receiving anticholinergic medication was not significantly different across patient groups, indicating that anticholinergic effects were not responsible for the between-group differences in performance.
The present study confirms prior reports of impaired tone-matching ability in schizophrenic inpatients and demonstrates differential tone-matching performance in patients drawn from inpatient sites compared with those drawn from outpatient settings. Even schizophrenic subjects with impaired baseline tone-matching performance were no more susceptible to same-modality distraction than controls, indicating preserved performance of a specific domain of prefrontal processing. Deficits could not be attributed to medication or overall symptom severity. Heterogeneity of performance was seen in patients drawn from a first-episode study, indicating that for some individuals deficits may be present early in the course of illness. These findings indicate that information-processing dysfunction at the level of sensory cortex may contribute to poor outcome in schizophrenia or may index a subform of schizophrenia associated with poor psychosocial outcome.
Accepted for publication August 4, 2000.
This study was supported by grants R01 MH49334 and K02 MH01439, from the US Public Health Service, Washington, DC (Dr Javitt), a grant from the National Alliance for Research on Schizophrenia and Affective Disorders, Great Neck, NY (Dr Rabinowicz), and by the Zara and Bernad Jakubovitz Fund for Brain Research, New York, NY (Dr Javitt).
We thank Lisa Falbo, MA, program director, and the residential program counselors and staff at the Residential Care Center for Adults, Orangeburg, NY; G. Gail Neffinger, PhD, Mara Cohen, CSW, and the staff at the Young Adult Center, County of Rockland, Department of Mental Health, Pomona, NY; and Gail Reiter, MS, and the staff at Hillside Hospital, Glen Oaks, NY.
Corresponding author: Daniel C. Javitt, MD, PhD, or Esther Rabinowicz, PhD, Nathan Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, 140 Old Orangeburg Rd, Orangeburg, NY 10962 (e-mail: javitt@nki.rfmh.org or rabin@nki.rfhm.org).
1.Goldman
R Neuropsychological dysfunction and schizophrenia: implications for pharmacotherapy.
Dir Psychiatry. 1998;1835- 47
Google Scholar 2.Harvey
PDDoherty
NMSerper
MRRasmussen
M Cognitive deficits and thought disorder, II: an 8-month followup study.
Schizophr Bull. 1990;16147- 156
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Kay
SRMurrill
LM Predicting outcome of schizophrenia: significance of symptom profiles and outcome dimensions.
Compr Psychiatry. 1990;3191- 102
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Weinberger
DRBerman
KFZec
RF Physiologic dysfunction of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in schizophrenia, I: regional cerebral blood flow evidence.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1986;43114- 124
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Goldman-Rakic
PS Working memory dysfunction in schizophrenia.
J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994;6348- 357
Google Scholar 6.Green
MF Schizophrenia From a Neurocognitive Perspective. Boston, Mass Allyn & Bacon1998;
7.Bilder
RMGoldman
RSRobinson
DReiter
GBell
LPappadopulos
EWillson
DFAlvir
JMWoerner
MGGeisler
SKane
JMLieberman
JA Neuropsychology of first-episode schizophrenia: initial characterization and clinical correlates.
Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157549- 559
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Shelley
AMWard
PBCatts
SVMichie
PTAndrews
SMcConaghy
N Mismatch negativity: an index of a preattentive processing deficit in schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1991;301059- 1062
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Javitt
DCDoneshka
PZylberman
IRitter
WVaughan
HG
Jr Impairment of early cortical processing in schizophrenia: an event-related potential confirmation study.
Biol Psychiatry. 1993;33513- 519
Google ScholarCrossref 10.Javitt
DCDoneshka
PGrochowski
SRitter
W Impaired mismatch negativity generation reflects widespread dysfunction of working memory in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52550- 558
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Javitt
DCGrochowski
SShelley
AMRitter
W Impaired mismatch negativity (MMN) generation in schizophrenia as a function of stimulus deviance, probability, and interstimulus/interdeviant interval.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998;108143- 153
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Javitt
DCShelley
AMSilipo
GLieberman
JA Deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in schizophrenia: defining the pattern.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;571131- 1137
Google ScholarCrossref 13.Javitt
DC Intracortical mechanisms of mismatch negativity dysfunction in schizophrenia.
Audiol Neurootol. 2000;5207- 215
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Holcomb
HHRitzl
EKMedoff
DRNevitt
JGordon
BTamminga
CA Tone discrimination performance in schizophrenic patients and normal volunteers: impact of stimulus presentation levels and frequency differences.
Psychiatry Res. 1995;5775- 82
Google ScholarCrossref 15.Javitt
DCStrous
RDGrochowski
SRitter
WCowan
N Impaired precision, but normal retention, of auditory sensory ("echoic") memory information in schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106315- 324
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Strous
RDCowan
NRitter
WJavitt
DC Auditory sensory ("echoic") memory dysfunction in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry. 1995;1521517- 1519
Google Scholar 17.Javitt
DCLiederman
ECienfuegos
AShelley
AM Panmodal processing imprecision as a basis for dysfunction of transient memory storage systems in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1999;25763- 775
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Fleming
KGoldberg
TEBinks
SRandolph
CGold
JMWeinberger
DR Visuospatial working memory in patients with schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;4143- 49
Google ScholarCrossref 19.Rabinowicz
EFOpler
LAOwen
DRKnight
RA The Dot Enumeration Perceptual Organization Task (DEPOT): evidence for a short-term visual memory deficit in schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1996;105336- 348
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Cordeau
JPMahut
H Some long-term effects of temporal lobe resections on auditory and visual discrimination in monkeys.
Brain. 1964;87177- 188
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Heffner
HEHeffner
RS Effect of unilateral and bilateral auditory cortex lesions on the discrimination of vocalizations by Japanese macaques.
J Neurophysiol. 1986;56683- 701
Google Scholar 22.Iversen
SDMishkin
M Comparison of superior temporal and inferior prefrontal lesions on auditory and non-auditory tasks in rhesus monkeys.
Brain Res. 1973;55355- 367
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Massopust
LC
JrWolin
LRFrost
V Frequency discrimination thresholds following auditory cortex ablations in the monkey.
J Aud Res. 1971;11227- 233
Google Scholar 24.Colombo
MRodman
HRGross
CG The effects of superior temporal cortex lesions on the processing and retention of auditory information in monkeys
(Cebus apella).
J Neurosci. 1996;164501- 4517
Google Scholar 25.Kaga
KShindo
MTanaka
Y Central auditory information processing in patients with bilateral auditory cortex lesions.
Acta Otolaryngol Suppl. 1997;53277- 82
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Peretz
IKolinsky
RTramo
MLabrecque
RHublet
CDemeurisse
GBelleville
S Functional dissociations following bilateral lesions of auditory cortex.
Brain. 1994;1171283- 1301
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Chao
LLKnight
RT Human prefrontal lesions increase distractability to irrelevant sensory inputs.
Neuroreport. 1995;61605- 1610
Google ScholarCrossref 28.Chao
LLKnight
RT Contribution of human prefrontal cortex to delay performance.
J Cogn Neurosci. 1998;10167- 177
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Alho
KWoods
DLAlgazi
AKnight
RTNaatanen
R Lesions of frontal cortex diminish the auditory mismatch negativity.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994;91353- 362
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Alain
CWoods
DLKnight
RT A distributed cortical network for auditory sensory memory in humans.
Brain Res. 1998;81223- 37
Google ScholarCrossref 31.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
32.First
MBSpitzer
RLGibbon
MWilliams
JBW Structured Clinical Interview for Axis I DSM-IV Disorders: Patient Edition (SCID-I/P). New York Biometrics Research Dept, New York State Psychiatric Institute1994;
33.Lieberman
JAAlvir
JMWoerner
MDegreef
GBilder
RMAshtari
MBogerts
BMayerhoff
DIGeisler
SHLoebel
ALevy
DLHinrichsen
GSzymanski
SChakos
MKoreen
ABorenstein
MKane
JM Prospective study of psychobiology in first-episode schizophrenia at Hillside Hospital.
Schizophr Bull. 1992;18351- 371
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Ammons
RBAmmons
CH The Quick Test (QT): provisional manual.
Psychol Rep. 1962;11111- 162
Google Scholar 35.Cohen
J Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates1988;
36.Borenstein
MRothstein
HCohen
J Power and Precision. Teaneck, NJ Biostat1997;
37.March
LCienfuegos
AGoldbloom
LRitter
WCowan
NJavitt
DC Normal time course of auditory recognition in schizophrenia, despite impaired precision of the auditory sensory ("echoic") memory code.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1999;10869- 75
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Zatorre
RJSamson
S Role of the right temporal neocortex in retention of pitch in auditory short-term memory.
Brain. 1991;1142403- 2417
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Wexler
BEStevens
AABowers
AASernyak
MJGoldman-Rakic
PS Word and tone working memory deficits in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;551093- 1096
Google ScholarCrossref 41.Knight
RTGrabowecky
MFScabini
D Role of human prefrontal cortex in attention control.
Adv Neurol. 1995;6621- 34
Google Scholar 42.Cohen
JDBraver
TSO'Reilly
RC A computational approach to prefrontal cortex, cognitive control and schizophrenia: recent developments and current challenges.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 1996;3511515- 1527
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Umbricht
DSchmid
LKoller
RVollenweider
FXHell
DJavitt
DC Ketamine-induced deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in healthy volunteers: implications for models of cognitive deficits in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;571139- 1147
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Goldberg
TEPatterson
KJTaqqu
YWilder
K Capacity limitations in short-term memory in schizophrenia: tests of competing hypotheses.
Psychol Med. 1998;28665- 673
Google ScholarCrossref 45.Cohen
JDBarch
DMCarter
CServan-Schreiber
D Context-processing deficits in schizophrenia: converging evidence from three theoretically motivated cognitive tasks.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1999;108120- 133
Google ScholarCrossref 46.Elvevag
BWeinberger
DRSuter
JCGoldberg
TE Continuous Performance Test and schizophrenia: a test of stimulus-response compatibility, working memory, response readiness, or none of the above?
Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157772- 780
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Strauss
MEReynolds
KSJayaram
GTune
LE Effects of anticholinergic medication on memory in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res. 1990;3127- 129
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Spohn
HEStrauss
ME Relation of neuroleptic and anticholinergic medication to cognitive functions in schizophrenia.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1989;98367- 380
Google ScholarCrossref 49.Vitiello
BMartin
AHill
JMack
CMolchan
SMartinez
RMurphy
DLSunderland
T Cognitive and behavioral effects of cholinergic, dopaminergic, and serotonergic blockade in humans.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;1615- 24
Google ScholarCrossref