[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Scatterplot of the relationship between the number of criticisms expressed by family members during a family interaction and the number of unusual thoughts expressed by patients with recent-onset schizophrenia during the family transaction, as a function of the memory-load Continuous Performance Test (CPT) performance of the patient. Those patients scoring in the bottom one third (n = 13) on the memory-load CPT are on the left side; those scoring in the top one third (n = 13) on the memory-load CPT are on the right side. The presence of memory-load CPT deficits interacts with interpersonal criticisms to predict expression of unusual thinking by patients with schizophrenia.

Scatterplot of the relationship between the number of criticisms expressed by family members during a family interaction and the number of unusual thoughts expressed by patients with recent-onset schizophrenia during the family transaction, as a function of the memory-load Continuous Performance Test (CPT) performance of the patient. Those patients scoring in the bottom one third (n = 13) on the memory-load CPT are on the left side; those scoring in the top one third (n = 13) on the memory-load CPT are on the right side. The presence of memory-load CPT deficits interacts with interpersonal criticisms to predict expression of unusual thinking by patients with schizophrenia.

Table 1. 
Continuous Performance Test (CPT) Measures, Interpersonal Criticism, and Unusual Thinking by 41 Patients*†
Continuous Performance Test (CPT) Measures, Interpersonal Criticism, and Unusual Thinking by 41 Patients*†
Table 2. 
Hierarchical Multiple Regression Using Degraded-Stimulus Continuous Performance Test (CPT) d′ and Interpersonal Criticism to Predict Unusual Thinking by 41 Patients*
Hierarchical Multiple Regression Using Degraded-Stimulus Continuous Performance Test (CPT) d′ and Interpersonal Criticism to Predict Unusual Thinking by 41 Patients*
Table 3. 
Hierarchical Multiple Regression Using Memory-Load Continuous Performance Test (CPT), d′, and Interpersonal Criticism to Predict Unusual Thinking by 40 Patients*
Hierarchical Multiple Regression Using Memory-Load Continuous Performance Test (CPT), d′, and Interpersonal Criticism to Predict Unusual Thinking by 40 Patients*
1.
Gottesman  IIShields  J Schizophrenia: The Epigenetic Puzzle.  New York, NY Cambridge University Press1982;
2.
Kendler  KSEaves  LJ Models for the joint effect of genotype and environment on liability to psychiatric illness.  Am J Psychiatry. 1986;143279- 289Google Scholar
3.
Meehl  P Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia.  Am Psychol. 1962;17827- 838Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Meehl  P Schizotaxia revisited.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46935- 944Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Rosenthal  D Genetic Theory and Abnormal Behavior.  New York, NY McGraw-Hill Co1970;
6.
Zubin  JSpring  B Vulnerability: a new view of schizophrenia.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1977;86103- 126Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Nuechterlein  KHDawson  ME A heuristic vulnerability/stress model of schizophrenic episodes.  Schizophr Bull. 1984;10300- 312Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Nuechterlein  KHDawson  MEGitlin  MVentura  JGoldstein  MJSnyder  KSYee  CMMintz  J Developmental processes in schizophrenic disorders: longitudinal studies of vulnerability and stress.  Schizophr Bull. 1992;18387- 425Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Nuechterlein  KHDawson  MEVentura  JFogelson  DGitlin  MMintz  J Testing vulnerability models: stability of potential vulnerability indicators across clinical state. Hafner  HGattaz  WFJanzarik  Weds. Search for the Causes of Schizophrenia Vol 2. Heidelberg, Germany Springer-Verlag1991;177- 191Google Scholar
10.
Servan-Schreiber  DCohen  JDSteingard  S Schizophrenic deficit in the processing of context: a test of a theoretical model.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;531105- 1112Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Brown  GWBirley  JLWing  JK Influence of family life on the course of schizophrenic disorders: a replication.  Br J Psychiatry. 1972;121241- 258Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Vaughn  CELeff  JP The influence of family and social factors on the course of psychiatric illness: a comparison of schizophrenic and neurotic patients.  Br J Psychiatry. 1976;129125- 137Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Vaughn  CESnyder  KSJones  SFreeman  WBFalloon  IR Family factors in schizophrenic relapse: replication in California of the British research on expressed emotion.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1984;411169- 1177Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Butzlaff  RLHooley  JM Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse: a meta-analysis.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55547- 552Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Spitzer  RLEndicott  JRobins  E Research Diagnostic Criteria for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders. 3rd ed. New York, NY Biometrics Research Division, New York State Psychiatric Institute1977;
16.
Wing  JKCooper  JESartorius  N The Description and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms: An Instructional Manual for the Use of the PSE and Catego System.  Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press1974;
17.
Hollingshead  AB Two Factor Index of Social Status.  New Haven, Conn Dept of Sociology, Yale University1957;
18.
Rosvold  HEMirsky  ASarason  IBransome  EDBeck  LH A continuous performance test of brain damage.  J Consult Psychol. 1956;20343- 350Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Nuechterlein  KHEdell  WSNorris  MDawson  ME Attentional vulnerability indicators, thought disorder, and negative symptoms.  Schizophr Bull. 1986;12408- 426Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Green  DMSwets  JA Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1966;
21.
Nuechterlein  KH Signal detection in vigilance tasks and behavioral attributes among offspring of schizophrenic mothers and among hyperactive children.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1983;924- 28Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Doane  JAWest  KLGoldstein  MJRodnick  EHJones  JE Parental communication deviance and affective style: predictors of subsequent schizophrenia spectrum disorders in vulnerable adolescents.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38679- 685Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Doane  JAFalloon  IRGoldstein  MJMintz  J Parental affective style and the treatment of schizophrenia: predicting course of illness and social functioning.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;4234- 42Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Rosenfarb  ISGoldstein  MJMintz  JNuechterlein  KH Expressed emotion and subclinical psychopathology observable within the transactions between schizophrenic patients and their family members.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104259- 267Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Lukoff  DNuechterlein  KHVentura  J Manual for expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS).  Schizophr Bull. 1986;12594- 602Google Scholar
26.
Endicott  JSpitzer  RL A diagnostic interview: the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35837- 844Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Rosenfarb  ISGoldstein  MJMiklowitz  DJHarmon  LHNuechterlein  KHRea  MM Patient subclinical psychopathology during family transactions in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.  Presented at: the 13th Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychopathology Research November 12, 1998 Cambridge, Mass.
28.
Rosenfarb  ISMiklowitz  DJGoldstein  MJHarmon  LHNuechterlein  KH Family transactions and relapse in bipolar disorder.  Fam Process. In press.Google Scholar
29.
Addington  JAddington  D Attentional vulnerability indicators in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.  Schizophr Res. 1997;23197- 204Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Bowen  LWallace  CJGlynn  SMNuechterlein  KHLutzker  JRKuehnel  TG Schizophrenic individuals' cognitive functioning and performance in interpersonal interactions and skills training procedures.  J Psychiatr Res. 1994;28289- 30Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Braff  DLSaccuzzo  DPGeyer  MA Information processing dysfunctions in schizophrenia: studies of visual backward masking, sensorimotor gating, and habituation. Steinhauser  SRGruzelier  JHZubin  Jeds. Handbook of Schizophrenia Vol 5. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1991;303- 334Google Scholar
32.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHMintz  J Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania, I: specifying a mechanism.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51939- 944Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  B Backward masking performance in unaffected siblings of schizophrenic patients: evidence for a vulnerability indicator.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54465- 472Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Grove  WMLebow  BSClementz  BACerri  AMedus  CIacono  WG Familial prevalence and coaggregation of schizotypy indicators: a multitrait family study.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100115- 121Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Nuechterlein  KHAsarnow  RFSubotnik  KLFogelson  DLVentura  JTorquato  RDDawson  ME Neurocognitive vulnerability factors for schizophrenia: convergence across genetic risk studies and longitudinal trait/state studies. Lenzenweger  MFDworkin  RHeds. Origins and Development of Schizophrenia Advances in Experimental Psychopathology Washington, DC American Psychological Association1998;299- 327Google Scholar
Original Article
December 2000

Neurocognitive Vulnerability, Interpersonal Criticism, and the Emergence of Unusual Thinking by Schizophrenic Patients During Family Transactions

Author Affiliations

From the Department of Psychology and Family Studies, United States International University, San Diego, Calif (Dr Rosenfarb); and the Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Nuechterlein, Goldstein, and Subotnik) and Psychology (Drs Nuechterlein and Goldstein), University of California, Los Angeles. Dr Goldstein is deceased.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57(12):1174-1179. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.57.12.1174
Abstract

Background  This study examined whether the combination of patients' neurocognitive deficits and criticism by others would predict the emergence of patients' unusual thinking during stressful family transactions.

Methods  When clinically stable, 41 patients with recent-onset schizophrenia completed 2 versions of a visual vigilance task, the Continuous Performance Test (CPT). One CPT emphasized early perceptual processing, while the other stressed immediate, working memory. On a separate occasion, patients and family members participated in a 20-minute interaction in which the number of relatives' criticisms and patients' unusual thoughts was assessed.

Results  In a hierarchical regression model, after entering performance on the CPT demanding immediate, working memory, and the number of criticisms by family members, the interaction of CPT performance and criticism significantly predicted the number of patients' unusual thoughts during the family session (r2 change = 0.09; P = .03). Post hoc analyses revealed that the number of criticisms and odd thoughts correlated significantly (r = 0.59, P = .03) for patients who had poor memory-load CPT performance, but were unrelated (r = −0.07) for patients who did well on the memory-load CPT. The CPT emphasizing early visual processing, either alone or in combination with interpersonal criticism, did not predict the number of patients' unusual thoughts during the interaction.

Conclusion  The results suggest that the combination of patients' working memory deficits and interpersonal criticism jointly predicts psychotic thinking, consistent with a model of schizophrenia that emphasizes the interaction of neurocognitive vulnerability and psychosocial stress factors.

SEVERAL MODELS of schizophrenia hypothesize that the exacerbation of psychotic symptoms involves the interaction of psychobiological vulnerability factors and environmental stressors.1-6 One such viewpoint, a heuristic vulnerability stress model,7,8 further hypothesizes that there are 2 types of enduring vulnerability factors in schizophrenia. Stable vulnerability factors reflect indicators of proneness to develop the disorder. Mediating vulnerability factors also indicate a proneness to develop the disorder, but, in addition, predict and mediate the emergence of symptoms in individuals who already have the disorder.7,8

Initial evidence suggests that early perceptual processing deficits reflect a stable vulnerability factor in schizophrenia, whereas a disturbance in immediate, working memory may be a vulnerability factor that mediates symptom formation. Earlier analyses from our project8,9 examined neurocognitive performance of 17 patients with recent-onset schizophrenia during both psychotic and remitted clinical states. Two versions of the Continuous Performance Test (CPT) were administered, both involving the presentation of a quasi-random series of single digits flashed briefly at a pace of 1 per second. In a degraded-stimulus CPT, early perceptual processing was emphasized by blurring the digits and asking patients to detect each blurred "0." In a memory-load CPT, working memory was stressed by asking patients to detect occasions on which a sequence of digits occurred ("3" followed by "7") in a series of clearly focused single digits.

Patients with schizophrenia performed significantly worse than matched normal controls in both remitted and psychotic states when early perceptual processes were emphasized. More importantly, performance in this situation was not affected by the patient's clinical state, consistent with a stable vulnerability indicator for schizophrenia.8,9 In contrast, when working memory was emphasized, patients' performance was not only abnormal during clinical remission, but also became significantly worse when they were experiencing psychosis.8,9 Thus, disturbances in working memory may be associated with the emergence of psychotic symptoms.10

It is possible, however, that patients' performance on the memory-load CPT became poorer when psychotic because positive symptoms interfered with their ability to perform the task. To show that working memory deficits are a mediating link in the development of psychosis, it would be important to demonstrate that the level of working memory deficit that patients have when they are stable is associated with the emergence of psychotic symptoms. Thus, patients who show larger working memory deficits in a stable state might be expected to have greater vulnerability to develop psychotic symptoms again. The first purpose of this study was to assess whether working memory performance in a laboratory setting when patients were stable would predict the emergence of subclinical psychotic symptoms during stressful family transactions.

The second purpose of the study was to examine whether psychosocial stressors would interact with neurocognitive deficits to predict the emergence of subclinical psychotic symptoms. Research has shown that high levels of criticism, hostility, and/or emotional overinvolvement among relatives of patients with schizophrenia, known as high "expressed emotion" attitudes, are predictive of patient relapse risk.11-13 Yet, while approximately 65% of patients returning to live in environments high in expressed emotion will relapse within 1 year of an index episode, the remainder do not.14 In the current study, we examined whether the relationship between interpersonal criticism and outcome may be partially dependent on the level of neurocognitive deficit that the patient demonstrates. Specifically, we hypothesized that the extent of working memory deficit that patients show in a relatively remitted clinical state interacts with the amount of interpersonal criticism experienced to affect the occurrence of unusual thinking by patients. Furthermore, we hypothesized that perceptual processing deficits, which seem to represent a stable vulnerability factor in schizophrenia, would not be associated with emergence of subclinical psychotic symptoms during a stressful family transaction.

Participants and methods
Participants

Forty-one patients with recent-onset schizophrenia and their family members participated in this study. Patients and key relatives were recruited for a longitudinal study of the early course of schizophrenia.8 Patients and their relatives were provided oral and written information about the project and signed informed consent agreements. Patients were recruited from 4 hospitals in the Los Angeles area. Criteria for patient admission into the longitudinal study included (1) a diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder, mainly schizophrenic, by Research Diagnostic Criteria15; (2) first onset of major psychosis no more than 2 years before first project contact; (3) age between 18 and 45 years; and (4) no known neurological disorder, recent significant and habitual substance abuse, or premorbid mental retardation. Diagnoses were based on an expanded version of the Present State Examination (PSE16) administered to the patient with additional information obtained, as needed, from family members. Minimum interrater agreement between PSE interviewers and a criterion rater before interviewing patients for the study was 85% for the presence of symptoms related to the diagnosis of schizophrenia, and 95% agreement on the absence of items relevant to the diagnosis. Rater drift was also monitored throughout the study to ensure that raters continued to meet these criteria.

Schizophrenia patients in this sample ranged in age from 18 to 32 years (mean [SD] age, 22.5 [3.6] years). Patients were predominantly male (37 men and 4 women), Caucasian (35 Caucasians, 3 Hispanic Americans, 1 Asian American, and 2 of mixed race), and from all social classes (mean [SD] socioeconomic status, 2.9 [1.1] based on the 5-point Hollingshead Index of Social Position17). Their mean (SD) educational level was 12.6 (1.8) years (range, 9-16 years). Patients had been ill, including prodromal symptoms, a mean (SD) of 16.3 (10.9) months (range, 1-53 months) at the time of their initial participation in the study.

Procedure
Neurocognitive Tasks

Patients were presented with 2 versions of the CPT, a measure of visual vigilance or sustained attention.18 Both versions assessed the ability to detect target numerals presented in a rapidly paced sequence. The degraded-stimulus version measures early perceptual processing abilities, whereas the memory-load version assesses patients' ability to hold a prior stimulus in memory and use it as a cue to guide response to the current stimulus. Stimuli were presented at a pace of 1 digit per second with 40-millisecond exposures using an electronic shutter and rear projection system.19 Numbers were 6.3 cm high and 4.8 cm wide and viewed from 1 m. Patients were instructed to press a button held in their dominant hand as quickly as possible whenever the target stimulus appeared. They were told that it was important to respond to targets as well as to avoid pressing the button for nontargets. For each CPT task, the proportion of target detections (hit rate) and the percentage of responses to nontargets (false-alarm rate) was recorded. Order of tasks was counterbalanced across subjects and for each CPT task, the dependent variable was the d′ level, the signal/noise discrimination index that is independent of tendencies to respond liberally or conservatively to stimuli that might be targets.20

Both tasks were administered to patients after they had been clinically stabilized and maintained on a stable dose of fluphenazine decanoate (usually 12.5 mg every 2 weeks) for 1 month. Antiparkinsonian medication was temporarily interrupted 48 hours before testing in 28 of 41 patients. One patient was not administered the memory-load CPT as this test was developed after the patient's testing session.

Degraded-Stimulus CPT

The adult version of the degraded-stimulus CPT19,21 was used. The single digits 0 through 9 were degraded by blurring to the extent that they required a 2.8-diopter lens correction to refocus. Patients were instructed to respond whenever the digit 0 was presented. After a 2.7-minute practice session, 120 target and 366 nontarget stimuli were administered in quasi-random order (identical digits never followed one another and digits preceding 0 were balanced) over an 8.1-minute period.

Memory-Load CPT

In this variant of the A-X CPT,18 patients were told to respond whenever a 3 preceded a 7 in a series of clearly focused single digits.19 Nontargets included other digits as well as 3s and 7s not in the 3 to 7 sequence. After a 2.45-minute practice session, 80 target and 568 nontarget sequences were presented quasi-randomly over a 10.8-minute period. Because a sequence of 2 stimuli is required for each target, this task was longer than the degraded-stimulus CPT to allow sufficient target stimuli for reliable calculation of d′ level.

Direct Family Interaction and Measurement of Interpersonal Criticism

Approximately 5½ weeks after hospital discharge, all patients and family members participating in the longitudinal study were informed of a separate study of family relationships conducted by an affiliated research group. Additional informed consent was obtained for those who chose to participate in this assessment. Fourteen patients participated in the direct family interaction with a single parent (13 with their mothers), 23 with 2 parents, and 4 either with a sibling or with a parent and a sibling.

Family members were left in a room together and asked to discuss, express their feelings, and attempt to resolve, for 10 minutes each, 2 previously identified family problems. One issue was patient-generated while the other was relative-generated. The order of problem presentation was counterbalanced across families. Interactions were videotaped and verbatim transcripts, deleting identifying family information, were developed.

The interpersonal criticism measure was derived from the relatives' affective style coding schema,22 which was developed, in part, as an attempt to capture expressed emotion attitudes manifested during a direct interaction. In this study, the individual affective style code, harsh criticism, was used to index interpersonal stress. Harsh criticisms reflect either personal criticisms of the patient (eg, "You are a lazy person") or guilt-inducing statements (eg, "You've caused our family a lot of trouble"). Previous research found that this variable is predictive of the onset of schizophrenia spectrum disorder in vulnerable adolescents22 as well as the probability of relapse for patients with schizophrenia in individual therapy.23

The unit of analysis was each speech segment. The unit ended when a second speaker either responded to or interrupted the speaker. The total number of harsh criticisms expressed by relatives over the two 10-minute interactions was used as an independent variable. Transcripts were coded by a rater, trained by Jeri A. Doane, PhD, who was found to be reliable in affective style coding in 2 previous studies.22,23 Within this specific sample, the rater and another trained rater coded 6 transcripts. Both raters were blind to other family or patient data. Reliability between the 2 coders for harsh criticism was 0.82 (P = .04, κ coefficient).

Measure of Patients' Unusual Thinking

The dependent variable was a measure of a patient's unusual thinking derived from the Patient Symptom Profile.24 The Patient Symptom Profile was designed to measure both clinical and subclinical levels of psychiatric symptoms or other odd or unusual behavior expressed by patients with schizophrenia within family transactions. The Patient Symptom Profile is based, in part, on dimensions from the expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale25 and the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia.26

The Patient Symptom Profile individual code for unusual thinking was chosen as the dependent measure in this study because this variable directly assesses clinical and subclinical psychotic symptom dimensions. Furthermore, this code was based directly on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale item, unusual thought content, which contributed to the determination of psychotic relapse in the associated longitudinal study.8 This measure assesses any instance of odd, strange, or bizarre thought content (eg, "If that kid bites you, you'll get rabies"). Coding was based solely on speech content, not on whether speech was disorganized.

In previous research, unusual thinking by patients during the interaction was found to correlate significantly with current symptom levels on the thought disturbance factor of the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale.27 Within the current sample, this variable significantly differentiated patients from high vs low expressed-emotion environments.24 Unusual thinking during a baseline family interaction has also been shown to predict relapse in bipolar disorder.28

The unit of analysis was each speech segment. The unit ended when a relative either responded to or interrupted the patient. The dependent variable was the total number of unusual thoughts expressed by the patient over the two 10-minute interactions. Each transcript was coded by the first author (I.S.R.), who was blind to family expressed emotion or affective style data and patient neurocognitive performance. After training, an advanced undergraduate also rated 30 transcripts from this sample. Interrater reliability for the 2 coders for unusual thinking was 0.73 (Cohen κ, P = .01).

Statistical analyses

Data were examined through 2 hierarchical, multiple regression analyses. The first analysis examined whether the hypothesized stable vulnerability factor, degraded-stimulus CPT d′, and number of interpersonal criticisms, would significantly predict frequency of patients' unusual thinking during the family interaction. Degraded-stimulus CPT scores were entered first, interpersonal criticism second, and the interaction term last. In the second equation, the hypothesized mediating vulnerability factor, memory-load CPT d′ was entered first, interpersonal criticism second, and the interaction of memory-load CPT d′ and criticism last. For all analyses, α was set at P<.05.

Results

Table 1 shows means and SDs for all measures. The first analysis examined whether degraded-stimulus CPT d′ and number of interpersonal criticisms would significantly predict patients' unusual thinking. Results indicated that only interpersonal criticism added significantly to the regression model (r2 change = 0.12; P = .02) (Table 2). Consistent with expectations for a stable vulnerability factor, the interaction term failed to add significantly to the equation r2 change = 0.02; P = .38). In the second analysis, memory-load CPT d′ did not significantly predict patients' unusual thinking (F1,38 = 2.66; P = .11). The addition of interpersonal criticism did add significantly to the model (r2 change = 0.15; P = .01), and at step 3, the interaction term also contributed significantly (r2 change = 0.09; P = .03). The complete model accounted for 31% of the variance in patients' unusual thinking (see Table 3 for the complete regression model).

To determine the nature of the interaction, Pearson correlations and scatterplots were used to examine the relationship between interpersonal criticism and unusual thinking among high (top one third) and low (bottom one third) performers on the memory-load CPT. As can be seen in Figure 1, for patients performing in the top third (n = 13) on the memory-load CPT, the correlation between the number of criticisms expressed and patients' unusual thinking was r = −0.07. For patients performing in the bottom third (n = 13) on the memory-load CPT, however, the correlation between the number of criticisms expressed and patients' unusual thinking was r = 0.59 (P = .03). One patient's data were excluded from the bottom third group because he scored more than 2 SDs above the mean on unusual thinking. With this patient's data included in the analysis, the correlation between criticism and odd thinking for patients scoring in the bottom third on the memory-load CPT was r = 0.84 (P<.0001).

Comment

The results indicate, as hypothesized, that the combination of deficits in immediate, working memory in patients with recent-onset schizophrenia, as evidenced by memory-load CPT performance in a stabilized outpatient state, and interpersonal criticism directed toward the patient by relatives is associated with the emergence of unusual thinking by patients during stressful family transactions. The data also indicate that early perceptual processing deficits, when assessed either alone or in combination with interpersonal criticism directed toward the patient, are not associated with the emergence of subclinical psychotic symptoms during stressful family transactions. Thus, these data add further empirical support to prior research,8,9 which suggested that active working memory processing deficits in schizophrenia may serve as a mediating vulnerability factor in the escalation of schizophrenia symptoms.

It is important to point out, however, that reduced working memory capacity, in isolation, was not associated significantly with patients' unusual thinking in this study. Instead, these data suggest that working-memory deficits in schizophrenia might be associated with the tendency toward psychotic thinking when interpersonal stressors occur. These results underscore the importance of examining psychosocial stressors when determining whether patient neurocognitive vulnerability factors are associated with the escalation of subclinical symptoms.

The data also underscore the importance of considering the severity of patients' neurocognitive deficits when determining whether psychosocial stress exacerbates symptoms. Interpersonal criticism had little relationship to patients' unusual thinking unless patients also had reduced active memory capabilities. Thus, patients' neurocognitive deficits may determine whether interpersonal stress potentiates psychotic thinking.

It is noteworthy that patient performance on the degraded-stimulus CPT was not associated with the escalation of subclinical psychotic symptoms in patients with recent-onset schizophrenia. Research has indicated that patients with schizophrenia perform significantly worse than nonpsychiatric subjects on this task8,29,30 as well as on similar neurocognitive tasks that assess early visual information-processing skills.31,32 First-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia also perform worse on these tasks than subjects with no known relatives with schizophrenia.21,33-35 Our results are consistent with prior research that has indicated that these early perceptual processing deficits are indicators of vulnerability to develop schizophrenia but may not be direct factors in the escalation of symptoms within patients who already have the disorder.8,9

This study is limited by its focus on patient subclinical symptoms during stressful family transactions. Although prior research found that these symptoms correlate significantly with patients' psychotic symptoms on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale,27 additional research is needed to determine whether the interaction of working-memory deficits and interpersonal criticism is also associated with increasing the probability of a subsequent psychotic relapse. We also cannot rule out the possibility that criticism by others was partially a consequence of, rather than a predictor of, patients' unusual thinking during the interaction. In prior analyses, however, we have found that while interpersonal criticism in high expressed-emotion families occurs in reaction to the first unusual thought by patients, it is also associated with an increased probability of subsequent unusual thinking by the patient.24

Conclusions

The data provide further evidence of the importance of distinguishing between neurocognitive vulnerability factors in schizophrenia that play a immediate role in symptom formation and those that indicate stable proneness to develop the disorder.7 The results also provide empirical support for a vulnerability stress interaction in schizophrenia—the emergence of odd or unusual thinking is best accounted for by considering the interacting effects of a neurocognitive vulnerability factor and a psychosocial stressor.

Accepted for publication June 20, 2000.

This study was supported in part by grants MH56476 (Dr Rosenfarb), MH08744 (Dr Goldstein), MH14584 (Dr Goldstein), MH37705 (Dr Nuechterlein), and MH30911, from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md.

We thank Sibyl Zaden, Lisa Harmon, Craig Childress, MA, Rhonda Daily, Cheryl Fogel, Karen Hemmerling, BA, Mitchell Stoddard, PhD, Kenneth Zaucha, PhD, and Margaret Rea, PhD, for their help in data collection, and Jim Mintz, PhD, and Sun Hwang, MS, MPH, of the University of California at Los Angeles Center for Research on Treatment and Rehabilitation of Psychosis, for their statistical assistance.

Corresponding author: Irwin S. Rosenfarb, PhD, Department of Psychology and Family Studies, United States International University, 10455 Pomerado Rd, San Diego, CA 92131 (e-mail: irosenfa@usiu.edu).

References
1.
Gottesman  IIShields  J Schizophrenia: The Epigenetic Puzzle.  New York, NY Cambridge University Press1982;
2.
Kendler  KSEaves  LJ Models for the joint effect of genotype and environment on liability to psychiatric illness.  Am J Psychiatry. 1986;143279- 289Google Scholar
3.
Meehl  P Schizotaxia, schizotypy, schizophrenia.  Am Psychol. 1962;17827- 838Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Meehl  P Schizotaxia revisited.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1989;46935- 944Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Rosenthal  D Genetic Theory and Abnormal Behavior.  New York, NY McGraw-Hill Co1970;
6.
Zubin  JSpring  B Vulnerability: a new view of schizophrenia.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1977;86103- 126Google ScholarCrossref
7.
Nuechterlein  KHDawson  ME A heuristic vulnerability/stress model of schizophrenic episodes.  Schizophr Bull. 1984;10300- 312Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Nuechterlein  KHDawson  MEGitlin  MVentura  JGoldstein  MJSnyder  KSYee  CMMintz  J Developmental processes in schizophrenic disorders: longitudinal studies of vulnerability and stress.  Schizophr Bull. 1992;18387- 425Google ScholarCrossref
9.
Nuechterlein  KHDawson  MEVentura  JFogelson  DGitlin  MMintz  J Testing vulnerability models: stability of potential vulnerability indicators across clinical state. Hafner  HGattaz  WFJanzarik  Weds. Search for the Causes of Schizophrenia Vol 2. Heidelberg, Germany Springer-Verlag1991;177- 191Google Scholar
10.
Servan-Schreiber  DCohen  JDSteingard  S Schizophrenic deficit in the processing of context: a test of a theoretical model.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1996;531105- 1112Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Brown  GWBirley  JLWing  JK Influence of family life on the course of schizophrenic disorders: a replication.  Br J Psychiatry. 1972;121241- 258Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Vaughn  CELeff  JP The influence of family and social factors on the course of psychiatric illness: a comparison of schizophrenic and neurotic patients.  Br J Psychiatry. 1976;129125- 137Google ScholarCrossref
13.
Vaughn  CESnyder  KSJones  SFreeman  WBFalloon  IR Family factors in schizophrenic relapse: replication in California of the British research on expressed emotion.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1984;411169- 1177Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Butzlaff  RLHooley  JM Expressed emotion and psychiatric relapse: a meta-analysis.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55547- 552Google ScholarCrossref
15.
Spitzer  RLEndicott  JRobins  E Research Diagnostic Criteria for a Selected Group of Functional Disorders. 3rd ed. New York, NY Biometrics Research Division, New York State Psychiatric Institute1977;
16.
Wing  JKCooper  JESartorius  N The Description and Classification of Psychiatric Symptoms: An Instructional Manual for the Use of the PSE and Catego System.  Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press1974;
17.
Hollingshead  AB Two Factor Index of Social Status.  New Haven, Conn Dept of Sociology, Yale University1957;
18.
Rosvold  HEMirsky  ASarason  IBransome  EDBeck  LH A continuous performance test of brain damage.  J Consult Psychol. 1956;20343- 350Google ScholarCrossref
19.
Nuechterlein  KHEdell  WSNorris  MDawson  ME Attentional vulnerability indicators, thought disorder, and negative symptoms.  Schizophr Bull. 1986;12408- 426Google ScholarCrossref
20.
Green  DMSwets  JA Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics.  New York, NY John Wiley & Sons Inc1966;
21.
Nuechterlein  KH Signal detection in vigilance tasks and behavioral attributes among offspring of schizophrenic mothers and among hyperactive children.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1983;924- 28Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Doane  JAWest  KLGoldstein  MJRodnick  EHJones  JE Parental communication deviance and affective style: predictors of subsequent schizophrenia spectrum disorders in vulnerable adolescents.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1981;38679- 685Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Doane  JAFalloon  IRGoldstein  MJMintz  J Parental affective style and the treatment of schizophrenia: predicting course of illness and social functioning.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;4234- 42Google ScholarCrossref
24.
Rosenfarb  ISGoldstein  MJMintz  JNuechterlein  KH Expressed emotion and subclinical psychopathology observable within the transactions between schizophrenic patients and their family members.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1995;104259- 267Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Lukoff  DNuechterlein  KHVentura  J Manual for expanded Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS).  Schizophr Bull. 1986;12594- 602Google Scholar
26.
Endicott  JSpitzer  RL A diagnostic interview: the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1978;35837- 844Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Rosenfarb  ISGoldstein  MJMiklowitz  DJHarmon  LHNuechterlein  KHRea  MM Patient subclinical psychopathology during family transactions in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.  Presented at: the 13th Annual Meeting of the Society for Psychopathology Research November 12, 1998 Cambridge, Mass.
28.
Rosenfarb  ISMiklowitz  DJGoldstein  MJHarmon  LHNuechterlein  KH Family transactions and relapse in bipolar disorder.  Fam Process. In press.Google Scholar
29.
Addington  JAddington  D Attentional vulnerability indicators in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.  Schizophr Res. 1997;23197- 204Google ScholarCrossref
30.
Bowen  LWallace  CJGlynn  SMNuechterlein  KHLutzker  JRKuehnel  TG Schizophrenic individuals' cognitive functioning and performance in interpersonal interactions and skills training procedures.  J Psychiatr Res. 1994;28289- 30Google ScholarCrossref
31.
Braff  DLSaccuzzo  DPGeyer  MA Information processing dysfunctions in schizophrenia: studies of visual backward masking, sensorimotor gating, and habituation. Steinhauser  SRGruzelier  JHZubin  Jeds. Handbook of Schizophrenia Vol 5. Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1991;303- 334Google Scholar
32.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHMintz  J Backward masking in schizophrenia and mania, I: specifying a mechanism.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1994;51939- 944Google ScholarCrossref
33.
Green  MFNuechterlein  KHBreitmeyer  B Backward masking performance in unaffected siblings of schizophrenic patients: evidence for a vulnerability indicator.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54465- 472Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Grove  WMLebow  BSClementz  BACerri  AMedus  CIacono  WG Familial prevalence and coaggregation of schizotypy indicators: a multitrait family study.  J Abnorm Psychol. 1991;100115- 121Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Nuechterlein  KHAsarnow  RFSubotnik  KLFogelson  DLVentura  JTorquato  RDDawson  ME Neurocognitive vulnerability factors for schizophrenia: convergence across genetic risk studies and longitudinal trait/state studies. Lenzenweger  MFDworkin  RHeds. Origins and Development of Schizophrenia Advances in Experimental Psychopathology Washington, DC American Psychological Association1998;299- 327Google Scholar
×