[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Cortisol levels in the girls with conduct disorder (CD) vs the normal control (NC) group. Each error bar represents 1 SD.

Cortisol levels in the girls with conduct disorder (CD) vs the normal control (NC) group. Each error bar represents 1 SD.

Subject Characteristics*
Subject Characteristics*
1.
Zoccolillo  M Gender and the development of conduct disorder.  Dev Psychopathol. 1993;565- 78Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Rutter  MTizard  JWhilmore  K Education, Health, and Behavior.  London, England Longeman1970;
3.
Kashani  JHOrvaschel  HRosenberg  TKReid  JC Psychopathology in a community sample of children and adolescents: a developmental perspective.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28701- 706Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Fergusson  DMHorwood  JLynskey  MT Prevalence and comorbidity of DSM-III-R diagnoses in a birth cohort of 15 year olds.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;321127- 1134Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Not Available, Total arrest trends, sex, 1993-1997 [Table 35].  Department of Justice. Crime in the United States Washington, DC Department of Justice1997;229Google Scholar
6.
Scahill  MC Female Delinquency Cases, 1997.  Washington, DC US Dept of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention2000;OJJDP Fact Sheet FS 200016.
7.
Gureje  OOmigbodum  OGater  RAcha  R Psychiatric disorders in a paediatric primary care clinic.  Br J Psychiatry. 1994;165527- 530Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Pajer  KA What happens to "bad" girls? a review of the adult outcomes of antisocial adolescent girls.  Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155862- 870Google Scholar
9.
Campbell  MGonzalez  NMSilva  RR The pharmacologic treatment of conduct disorders and rage outbursts.  Pediatr Psychopharmacol. 1992;1569- 85Google Scholar
10.
Christ  MLahey  BFrick  PRusso  MFMcBurnett  KLoeber  RStouthamer-Loeber  MGreen  S Serious conduct problems in the children of adolescent mothers: disentangling confounded correlations.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1990;58840- 844Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Bourduin  CMann  BCone  L Multisystematic treatment of serious juvenile offenders: long-term prevention of criminality and violence.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1995;63569- 578Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Chiland  CYoung  J Children and Violence.  Northvale, NJ Jason Aronson1994;
13.
Haddad  JBarocas  RHollenbeck  A Family organization and parent attitudes of children with conduct disorder.  J Clin Child Psychol. 1991;20152- 161Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Huizinga  DLoeber  RThornberry  T Urban Delinquency and Substance Abuse: Research Summary.  Washington, DC Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention1994;
15.
Mendel  R Prevention or Pork? A Hard-Headed Look at Youth-Oriented Anti-Crime Programs.  Washington, DC American Youth Policy Forum1995;
16.
Woodman  DDHinton  JWO'Neill  MT Cortisol secretion and stress in maximum security hospital patients.  J Psychosom Res. 1978;22133- 136Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Virkkunen  M Urinary free cortisol secretion in habitually violent offenders.  Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1985;7240- 44Google ScholarCrossref
18.
McBurnett  KLahey  BBRathouz  PJLoeber  R Low salivary control and persistent aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;5738- 43Google ScholarCrossref
19.
van Goozen  SMattys  WCohen-Kettenis  PTGispen-de Wied  CWiegant  VMvan England  H Salivary cortisol and cardiovascular activity during stress in oppositional-defiant disorder boys and normal controls.  Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43531- 539Google ScholarCrossref
20.
McBurnett  KLahey  BBCapasso  LLoeber  R Aggressive symptoms and salivary cortisol in clinic-referred boys with conduct disorder.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1996;20169- 178Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Vanyukov  MMoss  HPlail  JBlackson  TMezzich  ATarter  R Antisocial symptoms in preadolescent boys and in their parents: associations with cortisol.  Psychiatry Res. 1993;469- 17Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Moss  HBVanyukov  MMMartin  CS Salivary cortisol responses and the risk for substance abuse in prepubertal boys.  Biol Psychiatry. 1995;38547- 555Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Vinson  GPWhitehouse  BJHinson  JP Structure and function of the adrenal cortex. Grossman  Aed Clinical Endocrinology 2nd ed. London, England Blackwell Science1998;401- 430Google Scholar
24.
Shaffer  DFisher  PDulcan  MKDavies  MPiacentini  JSchwab-Stone  MELahey  BBBourdon  KJensen  PSBird  HRCanino  GRegier  DA The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version 2.3 (DISC-2.3): description, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA Study: Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders Study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35865- 877Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Altemus  MRedwine  LLeong  Y-MYoshikawa  TYehuda  RDetera-Wadleigh  SMurphy  DL Reduced sensitivity to glucocorticoid feedback and reduced glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;17100- 109Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Bloch  MSchmidt  PJSu  T-PTobin  MBRubinow  DR Pituitary-adrenal hormones and testosterone across the menstrual cycle in women with premenstrual syndrome and controls.  Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43897- 903Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Galliven  EASingh  AMichelson  DBina  SGold  PWDeuster  PA Hormonal and metabolic responses to exercise across time of day and menstrual cycle phase.  J Appl Physiol. 1997;831822- 1831Google Scholar
28.
Kirschbaum  CKudielka  BGaab  JSchommer  NHelhammer  D Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis.  Psychosom Med. 1999;61154- 162Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Rubin  RPoland  RLesser  IWinston  RBlodgett  A Neuroendocrine aspects of primary endogenous depression, I: cortisol secretory dynamics in patients and matched control subjects.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44329- 336Google Scholar
30.
Walker  BRBest  RNoon  JPWatt  GCMWebb  DJ Seasonal variation in glucocorticoid activity in healthy men.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;824015- 4019Google Scholar
31.
Kiess  WMeidert  ADressendorfer  RASchriever  KKessler  UKonig  ASchwarz  HPStrasburger  CJ Salivary cortisol levels throughout childhood and adolescence: relation with age, pubertal stage, and weight.  Pediatr Res. 1995;37502- 506Google ScholarCrossref
32.
Van-Cauter  ELeProult  RKupfer  DJ Effects of gender and age on the levels and circadian rhythmicity of plasma cortisol.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;812468- 2473Google Scholar
33.
Born  JDitschuneit  ISchreiber  MDodt  CFehm  HL Effects of age and gender on pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness in humans.  Eur J Endocrinol. 1995;132705- 711Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Yanovski  JYanovski  SCutler  GJChrousos  GFilmer  K Differences in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis of black girls and white girls.  J Pediatr. 1996;129130- 135Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Yanovski  JAYanovski  SZFriedman  TCPeng-Loh  YJayasvasti  VCutler  GB  JrChrousos  GP Etiology of the differences in corticotropin-releasing hormone–induced adrenocorticotropin secretion of black and white women.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;813307- 3311Google Scholar
36.
Flinn  MVEngland  BG Social economics of childhood glucocorticoid stress response and health.  Am J Phys Anthropol. 1997;10233- 53Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Kirschbaum  CPirke  K-MHellhammer  DH Preliminary evidence for reduced cortisol responsivity to psychological stress in women using oral contraceptive medication.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1994;20509- 514Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Lockwood  CRadunovic  NNastic  DPetkovic  SAigner  SBerkowitz  G Corticotrophin-releasing hormone and related pituitary-adrenal axis hormones in fetal and maternal blood during the second half of pregnancy.  J Perinatal Med. 1996;24243- 251Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Smith  R Alterations in the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis during pregnancy and the placental clock that determines the length of parturition.  J Reprod Immunol. 1998;39215- 220Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Dorn  LSusman  E Serum and saliva cortisol relations in adolescents during pregnancy and the early postpartum period.  Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34226- 233Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Yehuda  RSouthwick  SNussbaum  GWahby  VGiller  E  JrMason  J Low urinary cortisol excretion in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 1990;178366- 369Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Rosenbaum  PRubin  DB The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects.  Biometrika. 1983;7041- 55Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Stoff  DMPasatiempo  APYeung  JCooper  TBBridger  WHRabinovich  H Neuroendocrine responses to challenge with dl-fenfluramine and aggression in disruptive behavior disorders of children and adolescents.  Psychiatry Res. 1992;43263- 276Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Kruesi  MJPSchmidt  MEDonnelly  MHibbs  EDHamburger  SD Urinary free cortisol output and disruptive behavior in children.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28441- 443Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Schulz  KPHalperin  JMNewcorn  JHSharma  VGabriel  S Plasma cortisol and aggression in boys with ADHD.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36605- 609Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Chrousos  GPTorpy  DJGold  PW Interactions between the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the female reproductive system: clinical implications.  Ann Intern Med. 1998;129229- 240Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Vamvakopoulos  NChrousos  G Hormonal regulation of human corticotrophin-releasing gene expression: implications for the stress response and immune/inflammatory reaction.  Endocr Rev. 1994;15409- 420Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Gallucci  WTBaum  ALave  IRabin  DSChrousos  GPGold  PWKling  MA Sex differences in sensitivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis.  Health Psychol. 1993;12420- 425Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Riggs  PD Clinical approach to treatment of ADHD in adolescents with substance use disorders and conduct disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37331- 332Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Zoccolillo  M Co-occurrence of conduct disorder and its adult outcomes with depressive and anxiety disorders: a review.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31547- 556Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Rey  JMMorris-Yates  A Are oppositional and conduct disorders of adolescents separate conditions?  Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1993;27281- 287Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Biederman  JNewcorn  JSprich  S Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148564- 577Google Scholar
53.
Chrousos  GPGold  PW The concepts of stress and stress system disorders: overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis.  JAMA. 1992;2671244- 1252Google ScholarCrossref
54.
Chrousos  GElenkov  I Interactions of the endocrine and immune systems. DeGroot  Led Endocrinology 4th ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co2000;571- 586Google Scholar
55.
Crofford  LJPillemer  SRKalogeras  KTCash  JMMichelson  DKling  MSternberg  EMGold  PWChrousos  GPWilder  RL Hypothalamic-pituiatry-adrenal axis perturbations in patients with fibromyalgia.  Arthritis Rheum. 1994;371583- 1592Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Strickland  PMorriss  RWearden  ADeakin  B A comparison of salivary cortisol in chronic fatigue syndrome, community depression and healthy controls.  J Affect Disord. 1998;47191- 194Google ScholarCrossref
57.
Buske-Kirschbaum  AJobst  SWustmans  AKirschbaum  CRauh  WHellhammer  D Attenuated free cortisol response to psychosocial stress in children with atopic dermatitis.  Psychosom Med. 1997;59419- 426Google ScholarCrossref
58.
Rupprecht  MSalzer  BRaum  BHornstein  OPKoch  HURiederer  PSofic  ERupprecht  R Physical stress-induced secretion of adrenal and pituitary hormones in patients with atopic eczema compared with normal controls.  Exp Clin Endocrinol Diabetes. 1997;10539- 45Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Zinner  SH Treatment and prevention of infections in immunocompromised hosts. Gorbach  SLBartlett  JGBlacklow  NReds Infectious Diseases 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co1992;1252- 1260Google Scholar
60.
Zadeh  HHNichols  FCMiyasaki  KT The role of the cell-mediated immune response to Actinobacillus actinomycetemcomitans and Porphyromonas gingivalis in periodontitis.  Periodontology 2000. 1999;20239- 288Google ScholarCrossref
61.
Heim  CEhlert  UHellhammer  DH The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;251- 35Google ScholarCrossref
62.
Forst  MHarry  JGoddard  P A health-profile comparison of delinquent and homeless youths.  J Health Care Poor Underserved. 1993;4386- 400Google ScholarCrossref
63.
Hein  KCohen  MLitt  ISchonberg  SKMeyer  MRMarks  ASheehy  AJ Juvenile detention: another boundary issue for physicians.  Pediatrics. 1980;66239- 245Google Scholar
64.
Feinstein  RLampkin  ALorish  CKlerman  LMaisiak  ROh  M Medical status of adolescents at time of admission to juvenile detention center.  J Adolesc Health. 1998;22190- 196Google ScholarCrossref
65.
Dolan  MHolloway  JBailey  SSmith  C Health status of juvenile offenders: a survey of young offenders appearing before the juvenile courts.  J Adolesc. 1999;22137- 144Google ScholarCrossref
66.
Anderson  BFarrow  JA Incarcerated adolescents in Washington state.  J Adolesc Health. 1998;22363- 367Google ScholarCrossref
67.
Nierenberg  AAFeinstein  AR How to evaluate a diagnostic marker test: lessons from the rise and fall of dexamethasone suppression test.  JAMA. 1988;2591699- 1702Google ScholarCrossref
68.
Samuelson  SWinokur  G Biological markers. Hsu  LHersen  Meds Research in Psychiatry Issues, Strategies, and Methods New York, NY Plenum Medical Book Co1992;235- 263Google Scholar
69.
Cauffman  EFeldman  SWaterman  JSteiner  H Posttraumatic stress disorder among female juvenile offenders.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;371209- 1216Google ScholarCrossref
70.
Friess  EWidemann  KSteiger  AHolsboer  F The hypothalmic-pituitary-adrenocortical system and sleep in man.  Adv Neuroimmunol. 1995;5111- 125Google ScholarCrossref
71.
Delville  YMelloni  RJFerris  C Behavioral and neurobiological consequences of social subjugation during puberty in golden hamsters.  J Neurosci. 1998;182667- 2672Google Scholar
72.
Coccaro  EFKavoussi  RJHauger  RLCooper  TBFerris  CF Cerebrospinal fluid vasopressin levels: correlates with aggression and serotonin function in personality-disordered subjects.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55708- 714Google ScholarCrossref
73.
Susman  EJSchmeddlk  KHWorrall  BKGranger  DAPonirakis  AChrouso  GP Corticotropin-releasing hormone and cortisol: longitudinal associations with depression and antisocial behavior in pregnant adolescents.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38460- 467Google ScholarCrossref
Original Article
March 2001

Decreased Cortisol Levels in Adolescent Girls With Conduct Disorder

Author Affiliations

From the Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Pajer, Gardner, and Perel and Mr Neal) and Medicine (Dr Gardner), University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, Pittsburgh, Pa; and Center for Neurosciences Research, Allegheny General Hospital, Pittsburgh (Dr Rubin).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(3):297-302. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.3.297
Abstract

Background  Female adolescent antisocial behavior is increasing, but little is known about the neuroendocrinologic aspects of this disorder. On the basis of reports of decreased cortisol levels in antisocial males, we investigated morning plasma cortisol levels in adolescent girls with conduct disorder (CD).

Methods  Three plasma samples for cortisol levels were taken every 20 minutes between 8 and 9 AM in 47 adolescent girls with CD (mean ± SD age, 16.5 ± 0.9 years) and 37 normal control girls (mean age, 16.0 ± 0.8 years). All blood was drawn within 72 hours after the onset of menstrual flow.

Results  Girls with CD had significantly lower cortisol levels than girls in the normal control group at all 3 sampling times. This finding was not due to procedural factors, demographic characteristics, or the use of medications. The girls with CD who had no other psychiatric problems had lower cortisol levels than girls with other disorders or those in the normal control group. In the multiple regression analysis, having CD predicted 10% of the variance in cortisol levels.

Conclusions  Morning plasma cortisol levels were significantly diminished in adolescent girls with CD. Decreased cortisol levels appear to be most strongly associated with antisocial girls who do not have other psychiatric disorders.

FEMALE ADOLESCENT antisocial behavior is prevalent, whether defined as conduct disorder (CD) or delinquency. Conduct disorder is the second most common diagnosis given to adolescent girls,1 and one third of adolescent female psychiatric patients receive the diagnosis.2 In the general population, nearly 10% of 15- to 17-year-old girls meet criteria for CD.3,4 The delinquency rate for adolescent girls and the proportion of arrests of females for violent crimes have increased dramatically in the past 2 decades.5,6 Adolescent antisocial behavior is financially and emotionally costly to the teens and their families, and to society.7 Antisocial girls become women with rates of criminal behavior up to 40 times greater than those of other women, a high risk of early death, complex psychiatric problems, a high rate of substance abuse, poor adult physical health, and intergenerational transmission of antisocial behavior.8

Unfortunately, current treatments for female antisocial behavior are ineffective.9-15 Treatments may fail, in part, because little is known about biological correlates of female antisocial behavior. All previous research on neuroendocrinologic function in antisocial disorders has studied males.

One of the most provocative neuroendocrinologic abnormalities reported in antisocial males is decreased cortisol secretion. Low plasma cortisol levels in response to experimental stressors was first described among adult male criminals referred to a maximum-security hospital.16 These men were more violent than a criminal control group, who responded normally to the stressors with an increase in cortisol levels. Ninety-three percent of these hyporesponders had histories of repeated physical violence. Sixty-seven percent of the hyporesponders had committed murder, compared with only 14% of the criminal control group. Similarly, Virkkunen17 found that violent male criminals had decreased cortisol secretion, but that cortisol levels were normal in nonviolent criminals and noncriminal violent men.

Antisocial behavior in boys has also been associated with low resting cortisol levels, especially in boys exhibiting physical aggression.18-20 Similarly, both resting salivary cortisol levels and the cortisol response to psychological stimuli were negatively correlated with symptoms of CD in aggressive and impulsive sons of substance-abusing fathers.21,22

Given that low cortisol levels have been found in antisocial males, we investigated whether they are also low in antisocial females. We chose to determine morning cortisol levels because cortisol secretion follows a circadian rhythm and secretion peaks in most people between 8 and 9 AM.23 We focused on teenage girls who were in the final stage of puberty, and who met criteria for repeated antisocial behavior.

Subjects and methods
Subjects

We recruited 15- to 17-year-old girls to ensure that as many girls as possible would be in Tanner stage V and yet not out of adolescence. Girls were recruited predominantly from the community, through newspaper ads and posters. The ads sought girls with behavior problems such as "truancy, fighting, stealing, and lying." The ads also indicated that we were interested in girls without these problems. Three hundred fifty-four girls were contacted through ads and posters, through friends already in the study (n = 7), or through contacts in clinics (n = 6).

Intake appointments were made after a brief telephone screening and explanation of the project. One hundred fifty-one girls completed intake appointments. Each girl was interviewed concurrently with a parent or guardian who knew her history (the majority of adult informants were mothers or fathers). The intake appointment was used to determine eligibility for the study and to collect baseline nonbiological data.

In addition, a structured psychiatric interview (see below) was used to classify girls into the conduct disorder (CD) or normal control (NC) group. Exclusion criteria for both groups were age outside the 15- to 17-year range, pubertal development less than Tanner stage V, history of head trauma with loss of consciousness for more than 15 minutes, serious medical illness that could affect hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis function (eg, diabetes, thyroid disorders, renal disease), IQ less than 65, or a history of psychosis. Additional exclusion criteria for the NC group were the presence of any lifetime psychiatric disorder or any symptom of CD. Since we were trying to recruit a sample that would be as representative as possible of girls in the general population who matched girls with CD, we did not exclude girls in the NC group who smoked cigarettes or had experimented with alcohol, marijuana, or other drugs. We defined experimentation as not having used these substances more than 5 times throughout a girl's life and having no social or physical sequelae of use; no NC girl exceeded these criteria.

Ninety-three girls (52 in the CD group and 41 in the NC group) were found eligible and agreed to participate. We dropped 5 girls (3 in the CD group and 2 in the NC group) from the neuroendocrine protocol because we could not obtain a blood sample within 4 menstrual cycles of the baseline data collection. Four girls (2 in the CD group and 2 in the NC group) refused to give blood either initially or after an adverse event (vein collapsing, fainting). Thus, the final sample size for the neuroendocrine data presented in this article was 84 (47 in the CD group and 37 in the NC group).

Assessments

After explaining both stages of the study, we obtained written, informed consent from each girl and her parent or guardian. The intake interview was conducted separately with each girl and her parent. Data were collected about the following topics: family demographics, medical and developmental history of the girl, school experiences, psychosocial history, family function, family psychiatric history (parent only), and Tanner pubertal stage self-report (girl only). Psychiatric diagnosis was determined from the computerized version of the NIMH (National Institute of Mental Health) Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Version 2.3 (DISC), Parent and Youth Versions.24 All interviewers held bachelor's or postgraduate degrees. They were trained by the principal investigator (K.P.), who in turn had been trained by the developers of the DISC.

As indicated above, the DISC was used to classify girls into 2 groups. On the basis of concerns regarding the validity of using the aggression and age at onset DSM-IV criteria to diagnose female CD,1 we modified the DISC slightly. First, we rephrased the question on fighting to read "Do you often get into fights?" rather than "Do you often start fights?" Second, our diagnostic algorithm required that antisocial behaviors be demonstrated for at least 1 year before the interview, but onset before age 13 years was dropped from the items to which it is attached in the DSM-IV criteria.

Subjects were given a diagnosis based on meeting criteria from either the youth or the parent report. It is difficult to know who is the more accurate informant at this age because some adolescents may share their feelings and activities with their parents, and others may not. Moreover, some adolescents exaggerate reports of what they do, while others minimize them. Therefore, we considered data from all sources to be equally valid.

Procedures

If a girl met criteria for either group, she was asked to participate in neuroendocrinologic testing. To control for any menstrual cycle effects on HPA axis activity, we drew all blood within the first 72 hours after the onset of menstrual flow.25-28 Each girl participating in stage 2 was asked to call the study office as soon as she started her next menstrual period. When contacted, we arranged for an early-morning phlebotomy appointment within 72 hours of when she had begun her period. Fifty-two percent of the subjects had their blood drawn in their homes or at another designated meeting place (eg, school). The others were done at the study office. We inserted an indwelling catheter into the antecubital fossa of one arm. Samples were taken immediately on insertion (time 0), 20 minutes later (time 1), and 40 minutes later (time 2). Time 2 samples were used in the analyses. They were likely to be the most reliable estimates of morning basal secretion, since 40 minutes should have allowed the HPA axis to recover from the stress of catheter insertion. The catheter was kept patent with a saline flush between samples. Sixty-seven percent of the blood draws occurred before 9 AM (this time, however, was not significantly associated with CD vs NC status). However, because of the unpredictable schedules and behavior of these subjects, some girls had their blood drawn later than 9 AM, with starting times ranging to 11:15 AM. Most girls were studied within 1 menstrual cycle of baseline data collection, but erratic menses and scheduling difficulties resulted in some subjects having their blood drawn 2 to 4 cycles later.

All cortisol assays were performed without regard to diagnostic status. Plasma cortisol levels were determined with radioimmunoassay by means of a commercially available kit (Nichols Institute, San Juan Capistrano, Calif). Interassay variability, intra-assay variability, and assay sensitivity were 6.1%, 4.7%, and 1.4 nmol/L, respectively.29

Ten girls were taking medroxyprogesterone acetate and 5 were using oral contraceptives. One girl in the NC group signed up for the study and was pregnant by the time she was ready for neuroendocrinologic testing. Since the menstrual cycle in all 16 of these girls was suppressed, we drew blood when it was convenient for them, and as close as possible to the intake interview. We then tested for effects of these variables in the data analysis.

Because hypocortisolemia in antisocial men may be particularly related to violence, we categorized the girls with CD as aggressive or nonaggressive on the basis of the presence of 1 or more aggressive behaviors in either the parent or youth DISC report (fighting, cruelty to animals or people, carrying or using a weapon, fire-setting, stealing with confrontation, police arrests for assault). Each behavior described by the girl or her parent was assigned 1 point, and the final score for aggressive CD was the sum of these points. Girls with CD were also divided into 3 groups defined by the presence of psychiatric comorbidity: CD and no comorbidity, CD and oppositional defiant disorder only, and CD and multiple diagnoses.

The protocol was approved by the institutional review boards at Allegheny General Hospital and the University of Pittsburgh Medical School, Pittsburgh, Pa. The girls and their parents were paid $20 for baseline testing, and each girl received $50 on completion of the neuroendocrinologic testing.

Data analysis

Any variable not displaying a normal distribution was log-transformed, including plasma cortisol level. Comparisons between the 2 groups were tested for statistical significance by using unpaired t tests, univariate and repeated-measures analyses of variance, correlation coefficients, or χ2, depending on whether the variables were interval or nominal. All tests were 2-tailed, and statistical significance was set at P≤.05.

Cortisol levels are associated with many factors, including the time of day, time of year, and place when blood was drawn,30 demographic factors,31-36 taking oral contraceptives,28,37 being pregnant,38-40 and other psychiatric conditions, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).41 Statistically controlling for all of these factors would absorb more degrees of freedom than we could afford with our sample. Therefore, we calculated propensity scores42 (ie, the probability of CD vs NC membership) by logistically regressing group status on the above covariates. We then controlled for CD vs NC differences on the covariates by calculating a linear regression of cortisol on group status and the propensity score. By including the propensity score in the regression, we controlled for that information in the covariates that increased the likelihood of group membership. SPSS (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill) was used to conduct the analyses.

Results
Subject characteristics

The girls in the CD group were, on average, 6 months older than the NC girls (Table 1). Because all girls were in Tanner stage V, we doubt that this finding is clinically significant. The CD group included slightly higher proportions of African American girls and girls of lower socioeconomic status. Rates of smoking, alcohol, and drug experimentation were also higher among girls with CD, particularly experimentation with drugs other than marijuana (primarily hallucinogens). Girls with CD had high rates of comorbid substance abuse, internalizing, and oppositional defiant disorders, although few of the girls with CD had comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Cd vs nc differences in plasma cortisol

Mean plasma cortisol levels were significantly lower in the CD group at all 3 time samples (Figure 1). A repeated-measures analysis of variance was performed, and the effect of group status was significant: F1,82 = 8.40, P =.005. Results of t tests on log-transformed cortisol variables, which included corrections for unequal within-group error variances, showed t71.3 = 2.13, P =.04 at time 0; t75.8 = 2.74, P =.008 at time 1; and t78.4 = 3.08, P =.003 at time 2. The fractional degrees of freedom reflect corrections for unequal variance. Results were similar when the analysis was repeated without the girl who was pregnant. They were also similar when analyses were repeated without a girl who took lithium carbonate, a girl who took sertraline hydrochloride, girls who took oral contraceptives, girls who took medroxyprogesterone, and girls with PTSD. The effect sizes were substantial: time 0, d = 0.48; time 1, d = 0.61; and time 2, d = 0.67; where d = (xCDxNC)/SDpooled. Cortisol level was not significantly associated with age, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, or use of oral contraceptives.

To control for NC vs CD differences on other variables that might affect cortisol level, we estimated propensity scores to summarize these differences (see above). We then computed a linear regression of time 2 cortisol level on CD vs NC group status and the propensity score. In this regression, belonging to the CD group lowered cortisol level by 185.1 nmol/L (t81 = 2.53; P =.01), despite our having controlled for the group differences represented by the propensity score.

Plasma cortisol: aggression and psychiatric comorbidity

Thirty-seven girls met the criteria for aggressive CD, with the number of aggressive behaviors ranging from 1 to 10 (mean and median, 3 behaviors). The mean of the plasma levels of cortisol in the aggressive CD group (n = 37) was 336.9 ± 173.5 nmol/L compared with a mean of 386.5 ± 102.6 nmol/L in the nonaggressive CD group (n = 10). However, this difference was not statistically significant (t45 = 1.92; P =.07).

To determine whether decreased cortisol level was simply a correlate of severe psychopathology, as manifested by multiple diagnoses, we examined the mean cortisol levels in the following groups: NC (n = 37) (467.4 ± 212.2 nmol/L), CD with no comorbidity (n = 12) (291.9 ± 131.6 nmol/L), CD with ODD only (n = 6) (338.8 ± 103.2 nmol/L), and CD with multiple diagnoses (n = 29) (372.2 ± 179.1 nmol/L). It is clear that the girls with CD who had no comorbidity, not the girls with multiple comorbid diagnoses, had the lowest mean cortisol level. Only 2 girls had comorbid PTSD, but their cortisol levels were the lowest 2 in the sample. However, these 2 subjects also met criteria for major depression, separation anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder.

Comment

This is the first study, to our knowledge, of cortisol levels in antisocial girls. It confirmed our hypothesis that adolescent girls with CD would have lower morning cortisol levels than girls without any psychiatric disorder. Thus, it appears that adolescent antisocial girls may have HPA axis dysregulation similar to that found in antisocial boys and men.

The difference in mean cortisol levels between the CD and NC groups is much larger than those reported in previous studies on males.16,17 Not all researchers have found a negative correlation between cortisol levels and antisocial behavior in boys. Mean plasma cortisol levels drawn for morning baseline levels before a fenfluramine challenge were not different in a mixed group of prepubertal and adolescent boys who had "disruptive behavior disorders" compared with normal controls.43 In 2 samples of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, the majority of whom had comorbid CD or oppositional defiant disorder, urinary free cortisol levels44 and plasma cortisol levels45 were no different than levels in NC subjects. However, all of these samples were composed of boys spanning the age range from latency to adolescence and who had been selected on the basis of disruptive behavior or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, rather than repeated acts of antisocial behavior. Our more robust effect may be due to our sampling strategy, which selected a homogeneous group of late-adolescent girls who were in Tanner stage V and who had exhibited a pattern of repeated antisocial acts for at least 1 year. Our design further limited intersubject variance secondary to sampling time and menstrual cycle effects by standardizing these factors across subjects. However, it is also possible that the difference in cortisol levels between antisocial and normal subjects may be more pronounced in females because normal women have higher basal and reactive levels of cortisol than do normal men.46-48

The girls with CD displayed substantial psychiatric comorbidity, a finding consistent with other reports in the literature.49-52 Nonetheless, we were able to demonstrate that the relationship between cortisol level and CD was not simply a reflection of severity of psychopathology, because subjects without other diagnoses actually had the lowest mean cortisol levels.

The finding that antisocial females have low cortisol levels raises important clinical questions. First, low cortisol levels may put these girls at risk for later autoimmune diseases, some types of atopic illnesses, increased inflammation, and infections with extracellular pathogens.53-61 This may partially explain the poor adult health of women who had been antisocial adolescents.8,62-66

Second, low cortisol level may be a diagnostic marker for subtypes of girls with CD. To determine whether a biological correlate of a behavioral syndrome is a diagnostic clinical marker, one must first show that the relationship between the two is not due to a confounding factor. We have extended the previous work in males by demonstrating that the association between cortisol and CD is not due to factors such as season or race. The next step is to demonstrate that the biological finding discriminates between subgroups within the spectrum of a diagnostic category.67,68 We did find that girls with aggressive CD had lower mean cortisol levels than girls with nonaggressive CD did, but this difference was not statistically significant. However, only 10 girls with CD were not aggressive. Thus, the question of whether low cortisol level is a marker for aggressive CD in girls remains unanswered.

On the basis of a study reporting a high rate of PTSD in a sample of incarcerated adolescent females69 and data suggesting low cortisol levels in subjects with PTSD,41 we also investigated whether low cortisol level was a marker for antisocial girls with PTSD. The girls with CD with comorbid PTSD had the lowest cortisol levels in our sample, but there were only 2 of them and they also met criteria for several other diagnoses. Furthermore, the NC vs CD differences remained when these 2 cases were deleted from analyses. This important issue should be addressed in a study with larger numbers of antisocial girls with PTSD.

Our design permitted us to examine many alternative explanations for the group difference in cortisol levels. One limitation of our study, however, is that we could not exclude sleep disturbance as an explanation for the difference between the CD and NC groups. If the girls with CD are less well supervised and their lives more chaotic, it is possible that, as a group, they may routinely go to sleep later than midnight. This could shift their circadian rhythm such that the 8 to 9 AM blood-drawing time may still be in the prepeak phase of their HPA axis cycle.70 This question merits further research.

To date, there have been no studies of the mechanisms underlying decreased cortisol levels in antisocial subjects. However, animal and human data suggest that the ratio of corticotropin-releasing hormone to arginine vasopressin may be lower than normal in aggressive subjects, particularly in animals who have been bullied and become bullies themselves.71,72 This explanation may account for both the aggression and PTSD aspects of our findings. Although this research has focused on males, one clinical study of pregnant teens did report that the girls with CD had lower plasma levels of corticotropin-releasing hormone than the pregnant controls without CD.73

Future studies on HPA axis function in these girls should focus on describing the basal state of cortisol secretion throughout the rest of the 24-hour cycle, determining if their HPA axis responds normally to experimentally induced stressors, and methodically investigating the function of each component of the HPA axis (eg, response to corticotropin-releasing hormone, assessment of glucocorticoid receptor function). Research on potential mechanisms may result in more effective pharmacologic interventions or facilitate the identification of subgroups of girls with CD who respond to different types of treatments.

Accepted for publication September 25, 2000.

This study was supported by grant MH01285-04 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md (Dr Pajer).

We are grateful for the efforts of Judith Navratil, Barbara LeBeau, Deborah McCaskill, and Phyllis Wanetick, MSN, in collecting these data. We also thank the subjects for their participation.

Corresponding author and reprints: Kathleen Pajer, MD, MPH, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, 3811 O'Hara St, Suite 430, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 (pajerka@msx.upmc.edu).

References
1.
Zoccolillo  M Gender and the development of conduct disorder.  Dev Psychopathol. 1993;565- 78Google ScholarCrossref
2.
Rutter  MTizard  JWhilmore  K Education, Health, and Behavior.  London, England Longeman1970;
3.
Kashani  JHOrvaschel  HRosenberg  TKReid  JC Psychopathology in a community sample of children and adolescents: a developmental perspective.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28701- 706Google ScholarCrossref
4.
Fergusson  DMHorwood  JLynskey  MT Prevalence and comorbidity of DSM-III-R diagnoses in a birth cohort of 15 year olds.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;321127- 1134Google ScholarCrossref
5.
Not Available, Total arrest trends, sex, 1993-1997 [Table 35].  Department of Justice. Crime in the United States Washington, DC Department of Justice1997;229Google Scholar
6.
Scahill  MC Female Delinquency Cases, 1997.  Washington, DC US Dept of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention2000;OJJDP Fact Sheet FS 200016.
7.
Gureje  OOmigbodum  OGater  RAcha  R Psychiatric disorders in a paediatric primary care clinic.  Br J Psychiatry. 1994;165527- 530Google ScholarCrossref
8.
Pajer  KA What happens to "bad" girls? a review of the adult outcomes of antisocial adolescent girls.  Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155862- 870Google Scholar
9.
Campbell  MGonzalez  NMSilva  RR The pharmacologic treatment of conduct disorders and rage outbursts.  Pediatr Psychopharmacol. 1992;1569- 85Google Scholar
10.
Christ  MLahey  BFrick  PRusso  MFMcBurnett  KLoeber  RStouthamer-Loeber  MGreen  S Serious conduct problems in the children of adolescent mothers: disentangling confounded correlations.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1990;58840- 844Google ScholarCrossref
11.
Bourduin  CMann  BCone  L Multisystematic treatment of serious juvenile offenders: long-term prevention of criminality and violence.  J Consult Clin Psychol. 1995;63569- 578Google ScholarCrossref
12.
Chiland  CYoung  J Children and Violence.  Northvale, NJ Jason Aronson1994;
13.
Haddad  JBarocas  RHollenbeck  A Family organization and parent attitudes of children with conduct disorder.  J Clin Child Psychol. 1991;20152- 161Google ScholarCrossref
14.
Huizinga  DLoeber  RThornberry  T Urban Delinquency and Substance Abuse: Research Summary.  Washington, DC Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention1994;
15.
Mendel  R Prevention or Pork? A Hard-Headed Look at Youth-Oriented Anti-Crime Programs.  Washington, DC American Youth Policy Forum1995;
16.
Woodman  DDHinton  JWO'Neill  MT Cortisol secretion and stress in maximum security hospital patients.  J Psychosom Res. 1978;22133- 136Google ScholarCrossref
17.
Virkkunen  M Urinary free cortisol secretion in habitually violent offenders.  Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1985;7240- 44Google ScholarCrossref
18.
McBurnett  KLahey  BBRathouz  PJLoeber  R Low salivary control and persistent aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;5738- 43Google ScholarCrossref
19.
van Goozen  SMattys  WCohen-Kettenis  PTGispen-de Wied  CWiegant  VMvan England  H Salivary cortisol and cardiovascular activity during stress in oppositional-defiant disorder boys and normal controls.  Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43531- 539Google ScholarCrossref
20.
McBurnett  KLahey  BBCapasso  LLoeber  R Aggressive symptoms and salivary cortisol in clinic-referred boys with conduct disorder.  Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1996;20169- 178Google ScholarCrossref
21.
Vanyukov  MMoss  HPlail  JBlackson  TMezzich  ATarter  R Antisocial symptoms in preadolescent boys and in their parents: associations with cortisol.  Psychiatry Res. 1993;469- 17Google ScholarCrossref
22.
Moss  HBVanyukov  MMMartin  CS Salivary cortisol responses and the risk for substance abuse in prepubertal boys.  Biol Psychiatry. 1995;38547- 555Google ScholarCrossref
23.
Vinson  GPWhitehouse  BJHinson  JP Structure and function of the adrenal cortex. Grossman  Aed Clinical Endocrinology 2nd ed. London, England Blackwell Science1998;401- 430Google Scholar
24.
Shaffer  DFisher  PDulcan  MKDavies  MPiacentini  JSchwab-Stone  MELahey  BBBourdon  KJensen  PSBird  HRCanino  GRegier  DA The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version 2.3 (DISC-2.3): description, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA Study: Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders Study.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35865- 877Google ScholarCrossref
25.
Altemus  MRedwine  LLeong  Y-MYoshikawa  TYehuda  RDetera-Wadleigh  SMurphy  DL Reduced sensitivity to glucocorticoid feedback and reduced glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle.  Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;17100- 109Google ScholarCrossref
26.
Bloch  MSchmidt  PJSu  T-PTobin  MBRubinow  DR Pituitary-adrenal hormones and testosterone across the menstrual cycle in women with premenstrual syndrome and controls.  Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43897- 903Google ScholarCrossref
27.
Galliven  EASingh  AMichelson  DBina  SGold  PWDeuster  PA Hormonal and metabolic responses to exercise across time of day and menstrual cycle phase.  J Appl Physiol. 1997;831822- 1831Google Scholar
28.
Kirschbaum  CKudielka  BGaab  JSchommer  NHelhammer  D Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis.  Psychosom Med. 1999;61154- 162Google ScholarCrossref
29.
Rubin  RPoland  RLesser  IWinston  RBlodgett  A Neuroendocrine aspects of primary endogenous depression, I: cortisol secretory dynamics in patients and matched control subjects.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44329- 336Google Scholar
30.
Walker  BRBest  RNoon  JPWatt  GCMWebb  DJ Seasonal variation in glucocorticoid activity in healthy men.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;824015- 4019Google Scholar
31.
Kiess  WMeidert  ADressendorfer  RASchriever  KKessler  UKonig  ASchwarz  HPStrasburger  CJ Salivary cortisol levels throughout childhood and adolescence: relation with age, pubertal stage, and weight.  Pediatr Res. 1995;37502- 506Google ScholarCrossref
32.
Van-Cauter  ELeProult  RKupfer  DJ Effects of gender and age on the levels and circadian rhythmicity of plasma cortisol.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;812468- 2473Google Scholar
33.
Born  JDitschuneit  ISchreiber  MDodt  CFehm  HL Effects of age and gender on pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness in humans.  Eur J Endocrinol. 1995;132705- 711Google ScholarCrossref
34.
Yanovski  JYanovski  SCutler  GJChrousos  GFilmer  K Differences in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis of black girls and white girls.  J Pediatr. 1996;129130- 135Google ScholarCrossref
35.
Yanovski  JAYanovski  SZFriedman  TCPeng-Loh  YJayasvasti  VCutler  GB  JrChrousos  GP Etiology of the differences in corticotropin-releasing hormone–induced adrenocorticotropin secretion of black and white women.  J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;813307- 3311Google Scholar
36.
Flinn  MVEngland  BG Social economics of childhood glucocorticoid stress response and health.  Am J Phys Anthropol. 1997;10233- 53Google ScholarCrossref
37.
Kirschbaum  CPirke  K-MHellhammer  DH Preliminary evidence for reduced cortisol responsivity to psychological stress in women using oral contraceptive medication.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1994;20509- 514Google ScholarCrossref
38.
Lockwood  CRadunovic  NNastic  DPetkovic  SAigner  SBerkowitz  G Corticotrophin-releasing hormone and related pituitary-adrenal axis hormones in fetal and maternal blood during the second half of pregnancy.  J Perinatal Med. 1996;24243- 251Google ScholarCrossref
39.
Smith  R Alterations in the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis during pregnancy and the placental clock that determines the length of parturition.  J Reprod Immunol. 1998;39215- 220Google ScholarCrossref
40.
Dorn  LSusman  E Serum and saliva cortisol relations in adolescents during pregnancy and the early postpartum period.  Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34226- 233Google ScholarCrossref
41.
Yehuda  RSouthwick  SNussbaum  GWahby  VGiller  E  JrMason  J Low urinary cortisol excretion in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder.  J Nerv Ment Dis. 1990;178366- 369Google ScholarCrossref
42.
Rosenbaum  PRubin  DB The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects.  Biometrika. 1983;7041- 55Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Stoff  DMPasatiempo  APYeung  JCooper  TBBridger  WHRabinovich  H Neuroendocrine responses to challenge with dl-fenfluramine and aggression in disruptive behavior disorders of children and adolescents.  Psychiatry Res. 1992;43263- 276Google ScholarCrossref
44.
Kruesi  MJPSchmidt  MEDonnelly  MHibbs  EDHamburger  SD Urinary free cortisol output and disruptive behavior in children.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28441- 443Google ScholarCrossref
45.
Schulz  KPHalperin  JMNewcorn  JHSharma  VGabriel  S Plasma cortisol and aggression in boys with ADHD.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36605- 609Google ScholarCrossref
46.
Chrousos  GPTorpy  DJGold  PW Interactions between the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the female reproductive system: clinical implications.  Ann Intern Med. 1998;129229- 240Google ScholarCrossref
47.
Vamvakopoulos  NChrousos  G Hormonal regulation of human corticotrophin-releasing gene expression: implications for the stress response and immune/inflammatory reaction.  Endocr Rev. 1994;15409- 420Google ScholarCrossref
48.
Gallucci  WTBaum  ALave  IRabin  DSChrousos  GPGold  PWKling  MA Sex differences in sensitivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis.  Health Psychol. 1993;12420- 425Google ScholarCrossref
49.
Riggs  PD Clinical approach to treatment of ADHD in adolescents with substance use disorders and conduct disorder.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37331- 332Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Zoccolillo  M Co-occurrence of conduct disorder and its adult outcomes with depressive and anxiety disorders: a review.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31547- 556Google ScholarCrossref
51.
Rey  JMMorris-Yates  A Are oppositional and conduct disorders of adolescents separate conditions?  Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1993;27281- 287Google ScholarCrossref
52.
Biederman  JNewcorn  JSprich  S Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorder.  Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148564- 577Google Scholar
53.
Chrousos  GPGold  PW The concepts of stress and stress system disorders: overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis.  JAMA. 1992;2671244- 1252Google ScholarCrossref
54.
Chrousos  GElenkov  I Interactions of the endocrine and immune systems. DeGroot  Led Endocrinology 4th ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co2000;571- 586Google Scholar
55.
Crofford  LJPillemer  SRKalogeras  KTCash  JMMichelson  DKling  MSternberg  EMGold  PWChrousos  GPWilder  RL Hypothalamic-pituiatry-adrenal axis perturbations in patients with fibromyalgia.  Arthritis Rheum. 1994;371583- 1592Google ScholarCrossref
56.
Strickland  PMorriss  RWearden  ADeakin  B A comparison of salivary cortisol in chronic fatigue syndrome, community depression and healthy controls.  J Affect Disord. 1998;47191- 194Google ScholarCrossref
57.
Buske-Kirschbaum  AJobst  SWustmans  AKirschbaum  CRauh  WHellhammer  D Attenuated free cortisol response to psychosocial stress in children with atopic dermatitis.  Psychosom Med. 1997;59419- 426Google ScholarCrossref
58.
Rupprecht  MSalzer  BRaum  BHornstein  OPKoch  HURiederer  PSofic  ERupprecht  R Physical stress-induced secretion of adrenal and pituitary hormones in patients with atopic eczema compared with normal controls.  Exp Clin Endocrinol Diabetes. 1997;10539- 45Google ScholarCrossref
59.
Zinner  SH Treatment and prevention of infections in immunocompromised hosts. Gorbach  SLBartlett  JGBlacklow  NReds Infectious Diseases 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co1992;1252- 1260Google Scholar
60.
Zadeh  HHNichols  FCMiyasaki  KT The role of the cell-mediated immune response to Actinobacillus actinomycetemcomitans and Porphyromonas gingivalis in periodontitis.  Periodontology 2000. 1999;20239- 288Google ScholarCrossref
61.
Heim  CEhlert  UHellhammer  DH The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders.  Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;251- 35Google ScholarCrossref
62.
Forst  MHarry  JGoddard  P A health-profile comparison of delinquent and homeless youths.  J Health Care Poor Underserved. 1993;4386- 400Google ScholarCrossref
63.
Hein  KCohen  MLitt  ISchonberg  SKMeyer  MRMarks  ASheehy  AJ Juvenile detention: another boundary issue for physicians.  Pediatrics. 1980;66239- 245Google Scholar
64.
Feinstein  RLampkin  ALorish  CKlerman  LMaisiak  ROh  M Medical status of adolescents at time of admission to juvenile detention center.  J Adolesc Health. 1998;22190- 196Google ScholarCrossref
65.
Dolan  MHolloway  JBailey  SSmith  C Health status of juvenile offenders: a survey of young offenders appearing before the juvenile courts.  J Adolesc. 1999;22137- 144Google ScholarCrossref
66.
Anderson  BFarrow  JA Incarcerated adolescents in Washington state.  J Adolesc Health. 1998;22363- 367Google ScholarCrossref
67.
Nierenberg  AAFeinstein  AR How to evaluate a diagnostic marker test: lessons from the rise and fall of dexamethasone suppression test.  JAMA. 1988;2591699- 1702Google ScholarCrossref
68.
Samuelson  SWinokur  G Biological markers. Hsu  LHersen  Meds Research in Psychiatry Issues, Strategies, and Methods New York, NY Plenum Medical Book Co1992;235- 263Google Scholar
69.
Cauffman  EFeldman  SWaterman  JSteiner  H Posttraumatic stress disorder among female juvenile offenders.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;371209- 1216Google ScholarCrossref
70.
Friess  EWidemann  KSteiger  AHolsboer  F The hypothalmic-pituitary-adrenocortical system and sleep in man.  Adv Neuroimmunol. 1995;5111- 125Google ScholarCrossref
71.
Delville  YMelloni  RJFerris  C Behavioral and neurobiological consequences of social subjugation during puberty in golden hamsters.  J Neurosci. 1998;182667- 2672Google Scholar
72.
Coccaro  EFKavoussi  RJHauger  RLCooper  TBFerris  CF Cerebrospinal fluid vasopressin levels: correlates with aggression and serotonin function in personality-disordered subjects.  Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55708- 714Google ScholarCrossref
73.
Susman  EJSchmeddlk  KHWorrall  BKGranger  DAPonirakis  AChrouso  GP Corticotropin-releasing hormone and cortisol: longitudinal associations with depression and antisocial behavior in pregnant adolescents.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38460- 467Google ScholarCrossref
×