Background
Female adolescent antisocial behavior is increasing, but little is known about the neuroendocrinologic aspects of this disorder. On the basis of reports of decreased cortisol levels in antisocial males, we investigated morning plasma cortisol levels in adolescent girls with conduct disorder (CD).
Methods
Three plasma samples for cortisol levels were taken every 20 minutes between 8 and 9 AM in 47 adolescent girls with CD (mean ± SD age, 16.5 ± 0.9 years) and 37 normal control girls (mean age, 16.0 ± 0.8 years). All blood was drawn within 72 hours after the onset of menstrual flow.
Results
Girls with CD had significantly lower cortisol levels than girls in the normal control group at all 3 sampling times. This finding was not due to procedural factors, demographic characteristics, or the use of medications. The girls with CD who had no other psychiatric problems had lower cortisol levels than girls with other disorders or those in the normal control group. In the multiple regression analysis, having CD predicted 10% of the variance in cortisol levels.
Conclusions
Morning plasma cortisol levels were significantly diminished in adolescent girls with CD. Decreased cortisol levels appear to be most strongly associated with antisocial girls who do not have other psychiatric disorders.
FEMALE ADOLESCENT antisocial behavior is prevalent, whether defined as conduct disorder (CD) or delinquency. Conduct disorder is the second most common diagnosis given to adolescent girls,1 and one third of adolescent female psychiatric patients receive the diagnosis.2 In the general population, nearly 10% of 15- to 17-year-old girls meet criteria for CD.3,4 The delinquency rate for adolescent girls and the proportion of arrests of females for violent crimes have increased dramatically in the past 2 decades.5,6 Adolescent antisocial behavior is financially and emotionally costly to the teens and their families, and to society.7 Antisocial girls become women with rates of criminal behavior up to 40 times greater than those of other women, a high risk of early death, complex psychiatric problems, a high rate of substance abuse, poor adult physical health, and intergenerational transmission of antisocial behavior.8
Unfortunately, current treatments for female antisocial behavior are ineffective.9-15 Treatments may fail, in part, because little is known about biological correlates of female antisocial behavior. All previous research on neuroendocrinologic function in antisocial disorders has studied males.
One of the most provocative neuroendocrinologic abnormalities reported in antisocial males is decreased cortisol secretion. Low plasma cortisol levels in response to experimental stressors was first described among adult male criminals referred to a maximum-security hospital.16 These men were more violent than a criminal control group, who responded normally to the stressors with an increase in cortisol levels. Ninety-three percent of these hyporesponders had histories of repeated physical violence. Sixty-seven percent of the hyporesponders had committed murder, compared with only 14% of the criminal control group. Similarly, Virkkunen17 found that violent male criminals had decreased cortisol secretion, but that cortisol levels were normal in nonviolent criminals and noncriminal violent men.
Antisocial behavior in boys has also been associated with low resting cortisol levels, especially in boys exhibiting physical aggression.18-20 Similarly, both resting salivary cortisol levels and the cortisol response to psychological stimuli were negatively correlated with symptoms of CD in aggressive and impulsive sons of substance-abusing fathers.21,22
Given that low cortisol levels have been found in antisocial males, we investigated whether they are also low in antisocial females. We chose to determine morning cortisol levels because cortisol secretion follows a circadian rhythm and secretion peaks in most people between 8 and 9 AM.23 We focused on teenage girls who were in the final stage of puberty, and who met criteria for repeated antisocial behavior.
We recruited 15- to 17-year-old girls to ensure that as many girls as possible would be in Tanner stage V and yet not out of adolescence. Girls were recruited predominantly from the community, through newspaper ads and posters. The ads sought girls with behavior problems such as "truancy, fighting, stealing, and lying." The ads also indicated that we were interested in girls without these problems. Three hundred fifty-four girls were contacted through ads and posters, through friends already in the study (n = 7), or through contacts in clinics (n = 6).
Intake appointments were made after a brief telephone screening and explanation of the project. One hundred fifty-one girls completed intake appointments. Each girl was interviewed concurrently with a parent or guardian who knew her history (the majority of adult informants were mothers or fathers). The intake appointment was used to determine eligibility for the study and to collect baseline nonbiological data.
In addition, a structured psychiatric interview (see below) was used to classify girls into the conduct disorder (CD) or normal control (NC) group. Exclusion criteria for both groups were age outside the 15- to 17-year range, pubertal development less than Tanner stage V, history of head trauma with loss of consciousness for more than 15 minutes, serious medical illness that could affect hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis function (eg, diabetes, thyroid disorders, renal disease), IQ less than 65, or a history of psychosis. Additional exclusion criteria for the NC group were the presence of any lifetime psychiatric disorder or any symptom of CD. Since we were trying to recruit a sample that would be as representative as possible of girls in the general population who matched girls with CD, we did not exclude girls in the NC group who smoked cigarettes or had experimented with alcohol, marijuana, or other drugs. We defined experimentation as not having used these substances more than 5 times throughout a girl's life and having no social or physical sequelae of use; no NC girl exceeded these criteria.
Ninety-three girls (52 in the CD group and 41 in the NC group) were found eligible and agreed to participate. We dropped 5 girls (3 in the CD group and 2 in the NC group) from the neuroendocrine protocol because we could not obtain a blood sample within 4 menstrual cycles of the baseline data collection. Four girls (2 in the CD group and 2 in the NC group) refused to give blood either initially or after an adverse event (vein collapsing, fainting). Thus, the final sample size for the neuroendocrine data presented in this article was 84 (47 in the CD group and 37 in the NC group).
After explaining both stages of the study, we obtained written, informed consent from each girl and her parent or guardian. The intake interview was conducted separately with each girl and her parent. Data were collected about the following topics: family demographics, medical and developmental history of the girl, school experiences, psychosocial history, family function, family psychiatric history (parent only), and Tanner pubertal stage self-report (girl only). Psychiatric diagnosis was determined from the computerized version of the NIMH (National Institute of Mental Health) Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children, Version 2.3 (DISC), Parent and Youth Versions.24 All interviewers held bachelor's or postgraduate degrees. They were trained by the principal investigator (K.P.), who in turn had been trained by the developers of the DISC.
As indicated above, the DISC was used to classify girls into 2 groups. On the basis of concerns regarding the validity of using the aggression and age at onset DSM-IV criteria to diagnose female CD,1 we modified the DISC slightly. First, we rephrased the question on fighting to read "Do you often get into fights?" rather than "Do you often start fights?" Second, our diagnostic algorithm required that antisocial behaviors be demonstrated for at least 1 year before the interview, but onset before age 13 years was dropped from the items to which it is attached in the DSM-IV criteria.
Subjects were given a diagnosis based on meeting criteria from either the youth or the parent report. It is difficult to know who is the more accurate informant at this age because some adolescents may share their feelings and activities with their parents, and others may not. Moreover, some adolescents exaggerate reports of what they do, while others minimize them. Therefore, we considered data from all sources to be equally valid.
If a girl met criteria for either group, she was asked to participate in neuroendocrinologic testing. To control for any menstrual cycle effects on HPA axis activity, we drew all blood within the first 72 hours after the onset of menstrual flow.25-28 Each girl participating in stage 2 was asked to call the study office as soon as she started her next menstrual period. When contacted, we arranged for an early-morning phlebotomy appointment within 72 hours of when she had begun her period. Fifty-two percent of the subjects had their blood drawn in their homes or at another designated meeting place (eg, school). The others were done at the study office. We inserted an indwelling catheter into the antecubital fossa of one arm. Samples were taken immediately on insertion (time 0), 20 minutes later (time 1), and 40 minutes later (time 2). Time 2 samples were used in the analyses. They were likely to be the most reliable estimates of morning basal secretion, since 40 minutes should have allowed the HPA axis to recover from the stress of catheter insertion. The catheter was kept patent with a saline flush between samples. Sixty-seven percent of the blood draws occurred before 9 AM (this time, however, was not significantly associated with CD vs NC status). However, because of the unpredictable schedules and behavior of these subjects, some girls had their blood drawn later than 9 AM, with starting times ranging to 11:15 AM. Most girls were studied within 1 menstrual cycle of baseline data collection, but erratic menses and scheduling difficulties resulted in some subjects having their blood drawn 2 to 4 cycles later.
All cortisol assays were performed without regard to diagnostic status. Plasma cortisol levels were determined with radioimmunoassay by means of a commercially available kit (Nichols Institute, San Juan Capistrano, Calif). Interassay variability, intra-assay variability, and assay sensitivity were 6.1%, 4.7%, and 1.4 nmol/L, respectively.29
Ten girls were taking medroxyprogesterone acetate and 5 were using oral contraceptives. One girl in the NC group signed up for the study and was pregnant by the time she was ready for neuroendocrinologic testing. Since the menstrual cycle in all 16 of these girls was suppressed, we drew blood when it was convenient for them, and as close as possible to the intake interview. We then tested for effects of these variables in the data analysis.
Because hypocortisolemia in antisocial men may be particularly related to violence, we categorized the girls with CD as aggressive or nonaggressive on the basis of the presence of 1 or more aggressive behaviors in either the parent or youth DISC report (fighting, cruelty to animals or people, carrying or using a weapon, fire-setting, stealing with confrontation, police arrests for assault). Each behavior described by the girl or her parent was assigned 1 point, and the final score for aggressive CD was the sum of these points. Girls with CD were also divided into 3 groups defined by the presence of psychiatric comorbidity: CD and no comorbidity, CD and oppositional defiant disorder only, and CD and multiple diagnoses.
The protocol was approved by the institutional review boards at Allegheny General Hospital and the University of Pittsburgh Medical School, Pittsburgh, Pa. The girls and their parents were paid $20 for baseline testing, and each girl received $50 on completion of the neuroendocrinologic testing.
Any variable not displaying a normal distribution was log-transformed, including plasma cortisol level. Comparisons between the 2 groups were tested for statistical significance by using unpaired t tests, univariate and repeated-measures analyses of variance, correlation coefficients, or χ2, depending on whether the variables were interval or nominal. All tests were 2-tailed, and statistical significance was set at P≤.05.
Cortisol levels are associated with many factors, including the time of day, time of year, and place when blood was drawn,30 demographic factors,31-36 taking oral contraceptives,28,37 being pregnant,38-40 and other psychiatric conditions, including posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD).41 Statistically controlling for all of these factors would absorb more degrees of freedom than we could afford with our sample. Therefore, we calculated propensity scores42 (ie, the probability of CD vs NC membership) by logistically regressing group status on the above covariates. We then controlled for CD vs NC differences on the covariates by calculating a linear regression of cortisol on group status and the propensity score. By including the propensity score in the regression, we controlled for that information in the covariates that increased the likelihood of group membership. SPSS (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Ill) was used to conduct the analyses.
The girls in the CD group were, on average, 6 months older than the NC girls (Table 1). Because all girls were in Tanner stage V, we doubt that this finding is clinically significant. The CD group included slightly higher proportions of African American girls and girls of lower socioeconomic status. Rates of smoking, alcohol, and drug experimentation were also higher among girls with CD, particularly experimentation with drugs other than marijuana (primarily hallucinogens). Girls with CD had high rates of comorbid substance abuse, internalizing, and oppositional defiant disorders, although few of the girls with CD had comorbid attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.
Cd vs nc differences in plasma cortisol
Mean plasma cortisol levels were significantly lower in the CD group at all 3 time samples (Figure 1). A repeated-measures analysis of variance was performed, and the effect of group status was significant: F1,82 = 8.40, P =.005. Results of t tests on log-transformed cortisol variables, which included corrections for unequal within-group error variances, showed t71.3 = 2.13, P =.04 at time 0; t75.8 = 2.74, P =.008 at time 1; and t78.4 = 3.08, P =.003 at time 2. The fractional degrees of freedom reflect corrections for unequal variance. Results were similar when the analysis was repeated without the girl who was pregnant. They were also similar when analyses were repeated without a girl who took lithium carbonate, a girl who took sertraline hydrochloride, girls who took oral contraceptives, girls who took medroxyprogesterone, and girls with PTSD. The effect sizes were substantial: time 0, d = 0.48; time 1, d = 0.61; and time 2, d = 0.67; where d = (x‒CD − x‒NC)/SDpooled. Cortisol level was not significantly associated with age, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, or use of oral contraceptives.
To control for NC vs CD differences on other variables that might affect cortisol level, we estimated propensity scores to summarize these differences (see above). We then computed a linear regression of time 2 cortisol level on CD vs NC group status and the propensity score. In this regression, belonging to the CD group lowered cortisol level by 185.1 nmol/L (t81 = 2.53; P =.01), despite our having controlled for the group differences represented by the propensity score.
Plasma cortisol: aggression and psychiatric comorbidity
Thirty-seven girls met the criteria for aggressive CD, with the number of aggressive behaviors ranging from 1 to 10 (mean and median, 3 behaviors). The mean of the plasma levels of cortisol in the aggressive CD group (n = 37) was 336.9 ± 173.5 nmol/L compared with a mean of 386.5 ± 102.6 nmol/L in the nonaggressive CD group (n = 10). However, this difference was not statistically significant (t45 = 1.92; P =.07).
To determine whether decreased cortisol level was simply a correlate of severe psychopathology, as manifested by multiple diagnoses, we examined the mean cortisol levels in the following groups: NC (n = 37) (467.4 ± 212.2 nmol/L), CD with no comorbidity (n = 12) (291.9 ± 131.6 nmol/L), CD with ODD only (n = 6) (338.8 ± 103.2 nmol/L), and CD with multiple diagnoses (n = 29) (372.2 ± 179.1 nmol/L). It is clear that the girls with CD who had no comorbidity, not the girls with multiple comorbid diagnoses, had the lowest mean cortisol level. Only 2 girls had comorbid PTSD, but their cortisol levels were the lowest 2 in the sample. However, these 2 subjects also met criteria for major depression, separation anxiety disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder.
This is the first study, to our knowledge, of cortisol levels in antisocial girls. It confirmed our hypothesis that adolescent girls with CD would have lower morning cortisol levels than girls without any psychiatric disorder. Thus, it appears that adolescent antisocial girls may have HPA axis dysregulation similar to that found in antisocial boys and men.
The difference in mean cortisol levels between the CD and NC groups is much larger than those reported in previous studies on males.16,17 Not all researchers have found a negative correlation between cortisol levels and antisocial behavior in boys. Mean plasma cortisol levels drawn for morning baseline levels before a fenfluramine challenge were not different in a mixed group of prepubertal and adolescent boys who had "disruptive behavior disorders" compared with normal controls.43 In 2 samples of boys with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, the majority of whom had comorbid CD or oppositional defiant disorder, urinary free cortisol levels44 and plasma cortisol levels45 were no different than levels in NC subjects. However, all of these samples were composed of boys spanning the age range from latency to adolescence and who had been selected on the basis of disruptive behavior or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, rather than repeated acts of antisocial behavior. Our more robust effect may be due to our sampling strategy, which selected a homogeneous group of late-adolescent girls who were in Tanner stage V and who had exhibited a pattern of repeated antisocial acts for at least 1 year. Our design further limited intersubject variance secondary to sampling time and menstrual cycle effects by standardizing these factors across subjects. However, it is also possible that the difference in cortisol levels between antisocial and normal subjects may be more pronounced in females because normal women have higher basal and reactive levels of cortisol than do normal men.46-48
The girls with CD displayed substantial psychiatric comorbidity, a finding consistent with other reports in the literature.49-52 Nonetheless, we were able to demonstrate that the relationship between cortisol level and CD was not simply a reflection of severity of psychopathology, because subjects without other diagnoses actually had the lowest mean cortisol levels.
The finding that antisocial females have low cortisol levels raises important clinical questions. First, low cortisol levels may put these girls at risk for later autoimmune diseases, some types of atopic illnesses, increased inflammation, and infections with extracellular pathogens.53-61 This may partially explain the poor adult health of women who had been antisocial adolescents.8,62-66
Second, low cortisol level may be a diagnostic marker for subtypes of girls with CD. To determine whether a biological correlate of a behavioral syndrome is a diagnostic clinical marker, one must first show that the relationship between the two is not due to a confounding factor. We have extended the previous work in males by demonstrating that the association between cortisol and CD is not due to factors such as season or race. The next step is to demonstrate that the biological finding discriminates between subgroups within the spectrum of a diagnostic category.67,68 We did find that girls with aggressive CD had lower mean cortisol levels than girls with nonaggressive CD did, but this difference was not statistically significant. However, only 10 girls with CD were not aggressive. Thus, the question of whether low cortisol level is a marker for aggressive CD in girls remains unanswered.
On the basis of a study reporting a high rate of PTSD in a sample of incarcerated adolescent females69 and data suggesting low cortisol levels in subjects with PTSD,41 we also investigated whether low cortisol level was a marker for antisocial girls with PTSD. The girls with CD with comorbid PTSD had the lowest cortisol levels in our sample, but there were only 2 of them and they also met criteria for several other diagnoses. Furthermore, the NC vs CD differences remained when these 2 cases were deleted from analyses. This important issue should be addressed in a study with larger numbers of antisocial girls with PTSD.
Our design permitted us to examine many alternative explanations for the group difference in cortisol levels. One limitation of our study, however, is that we could not exclude sleep disturbance as an explanation for the difference between the CD and NC groups. If the girls with CD are less well supervised and their lives more chaotic, it is possible that, as a group, they may routinely go to sleep later than midnight. This could shift their circadian rhythm such that the 8 to 9 AM blood-drawing time may still be in the prepeak phase of their HPA axis cycle.70 This question merits further research.
To date, there have been no studies of the mechanisms underlying decreased cortisol levels in antisocial subjects. However, animal and human data suggest that the ratio of corticotropin-releasing hormone to arginine vasopressin may be lower than normal in aggressive subjects, particularly in animals who have been bullied and become bullies themselves.71,72 This explanation may account for both the aggression and PTSD aspects of our findings. Although this research has focused on males, one clinical study of pregnant teens did report that the girls with CD had lower plasma levels of corticotropin-releasing hormone than the pregnant controls without CD.73
Future studies on HPA axis function in these girls should focus on describing the basal state of cortisol secretion throughout the rest of the 24-hour cycle, determining if their HPA axis responds normally to experimentally induced stressors, and methodically investigating the function of each component of the HPA axis (eg, response to corticotropin-releasing hormone, assessment of glucocorticoid receptor function). Research on potential mechanisms may result in more effective pharmacologic interventions or facilitate the identification of subgroups of girls with CD who respond to different types of treatments.
Accepted for publication September 25, 2000.
This study was supported by grant MH01285-04 from the National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Md (Dr Pajer).
We are grateful for the efforts of Judith Navratil, Barbara LeBeau, Deborah McCaskill, and Phyllis Wanetick, MSN, in collecting these data. We also thank the subjects for their participation.
Corresponding author and reprints: Kathleen Pajer, MD, MPH, Department of Psychiatry, University of Pittsburgh School of Medicine, 3811 O'Hara St, Suite 430, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 (pajerka@msx.upmc.edu).
2.Rutter
MTizard
JWhilmore
K Education, Health, and Behavior. London, England Longeman1970;
3.Kashani
JHOrvaschel
HRosenberg
TKReid
JC Psychopathology in a community sample of children and adolescents: a developmental perspective.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28701- 706
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Fergusson
DMHorwood
JLynskey
MT Prevalence and comorbidity of
DSM-III-R diagnoses in a birth cohort of 15 year olds.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1993;321127- 1134
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Not Available, Total arrest trends, sex, 1993-1997 [Table 35].
Department of Justice. Crime in the United States Washington, DC Department of Justice1997;229
Google Scholar 6.Scahill
MC Female Delinquency Cases, 1997. Washington, DC US Dept of Justice, Office of Justice Programs, Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention2000;OJJDP Fact Sheet FS 200016.
7.Gureje
OOmigbodum
OGater
RAcha
R Psychiatric disorders in a paediatric primary care clinic.
Br J Psychiatry. 1994;165527- 530
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Pajer
KA What happens to "bad" girls? a review of the adult outcomes of antisocial adolescent girls.
Am J Psychiatry. 1998;155862- 870
Google Scholar 9.Campbell
MGonzalez
NMSilva
RR The pharmacologic treatment of conduct disorders and rage outbursts.
Pediatr Psychopharmacol. 1992;1569- 85
Google Scholar 10.Christ
MLahey
BFrick
PRusso
MFMcBurnett
KLoeber
RStouthamer-Loeber
MGreen
S Serious conduct problems in the children of adolescent mothers: disentangling confounded correlations.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 1990;58840- 844
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Bourduin
CMann
BCone
L Multisystematic treatment of serious juvenile offenders: long-term prevention of criminality and violence.
J Consult Clin Psychol. 1995;63569- 578
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Chiland
CYoung
J Children and Violence. Northvale, NJ Jason Aronson1994;
13.Haddad
JBarocas
RHollenbeck
A Family organization and parent attitudes of children with conduct disorder.
J Clin Child Psychol. 1991;20152- 161
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Huizinga
DLoeber
RThornberry
T Urban Delinquency and Substance Abuse: Research Summary. Washington, DC Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention1994;
15.Mendel
R Prevention or Pork? A Hard-Headed Look at Youth-Oriented Anti-Crime Programs. Washington, DC American Youth Policy Forum1995;
16.Woodman
DDHinton
JWO'Neill
MT Cortisol secretion and stress in maximum security hospital patients.
J Psychosom Res. 1978;22133- 136
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Virkkunen
M Urinary free cortisol secretion in habitually violent offenders.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1985;7240- 44
Google ScholarCrossref 18.McBurnett
KLahey
BBRathouz
PJLoeber
R Low salivary control and persistent aggression in boys referred for disruptive behavior.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;5738- 43
Google ScholarCrossref 19.van Goozen
SMattys
WCohen-Kettenis
PTGispen-de Wied
CWiegant
VMvan England
H Salivary cortisol and cardiovascular activity during stress in oppositional-defiant disorder boys and normal controls.
Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43531- 539
Google ScholarCrossref 20.McBurnett
KLahey
BBCapasso
LLoeber
R Aggressive symptoms and salivary cortisol in clinic-referred boys with conduct disorder.
Ann N Y Acad Sci. 1996;20169- 178
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Vanyukov
MMoss
HPlail
JBlackson
TMezzich
ATarter
R Antisocial symptoms in preadolescent boys and in their parents: associations with cortisol.
Psychiatry Res. 1993;469- 17
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Moss
HBVanyukov
MMMartin
CS Salivary cortisol responses and the risk for substance abuse in prepubertal boys.
Biol Psychiatry. 1995;38547- 555
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Vinson
GPWhitehouse
BJHinson
JP Structure and function of the adrenal cortex. Grossman
Aed
Clinical Endocrinology 2nd ed. London, England Blackwell Science1998;401- 430
Google Scholar 24.Shaffer
DFisher
PDulcan
MKDavies
MPiacentini
JSchwab-Stone
MELahey
BBBourdon
KJensen
PSBird
HRCanino
GRegier
DA The NIMH Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Version 2.3 (DISC-2.3): description, acceptability, prevalence rates, and performance in the MECA Study: Methods for the Epidemiology of Child and Adolescent Mental Disorders Study.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1996;35865- 877
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Altemus
MRedwine
LLeong
Y-MYoshikawa
TYehuda
RDetera-Wadleigh
SMurphy
DL Reduced sensitivity to glucocorticoid feedback and reduced glucocorticoid receptor mRNA expression in the luteal phase of the menstrual cycle.
Neuropsychopharmacology. 1997;17100- 109
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Bloch
MSchmidt
PJSu
T-PTobin
MBRubinow
DR Pituitary-adrenal hormones and testosterone across the menstrual cycle in women with premenstrual syndrome and controls.
Biol Psychiatry. 1998;43897- 903
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Galliven
EASingh
AMichelson
DBina
SGold
PWDeuster
PA Hormonal and metabolic responses to exercise across time of day and menstrual cycle phase.
J Appl Physiol. 1997;831822- 1831
Google Scholar 28.Kirschbaum
CKudielka
BGaab
JSchommer
NHelhammer
D Impact of gender, menstrual cycle phase, and oral contraceptives on the activity of the hypothalamus-pituitary-adrenal axis.
Psychosom Med. 1999;61154- 162
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Rubin
RPoland
RLesser
IWinston
RBlodgett
A Neuroendocrine aspects of primary endogenous depression, I: cortisol secretory dynamics in patients and matched control subjects.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1987;44329- 336
Google Scholar 30.Walker
BRBest
RNoon
JPWatt
GCMWebb
DJ Seasonal variation in glucocorticoid activity in healthy men.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1997;824015- 4019
Google Scholar 31.Kiess
WMeidert
ADressendorfer
RASchriever
KKessler
UKonig
ASchwarz
HPStrasburger
CJ Salivary cortisol levels throughout childhood and adolescence: relation with age, pubertal stage, and weight.
Pediatr Res. 1995;37502- 506
Google ScholarCrossref 32.Van-Cauter
ELeProult
RKupfer
DJ Effects of gender and age on the levels and circadian rhythmicity of plasma cortisol.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;812468- 2473
Google Scholar 33.Born
JDitschuneit
ISchreiber
MDodt
CFehm
HL Effects of age and gender on pituitary-adrenocortical responsiveness in humans.
Eur J Endocrinol. 1995;132705- 711
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Yanovski
JYanovski
SCutler
GJChrousos
GFilmer
K Differences in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis of black girls and white girls.
J Pediatr. 1996;129130- 135
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Yanovski
JAYanovski
SZFriedman
TCPeng-Loh
YJayasvasti
VCutler
GB
JrChrousos
GP Etiology of the differences in corticotropin-releasing hormone–induced adrenocorticotropin secretion of black and white women.
J Clin Endocrinol Metab. 1996;813307- 3311
Google Scholar 36.Flinn
MVEngland
BG Social economics of childhood glucocorticoid stress response and health.
Am J Phys Anthropol. 1997;10233- 53
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Kirschbaum
CPirke
K-MHellhammer
DH Preliminary evidence for reduced cortisol responsivity to psychological stress in women using oral contraceptive medication.
Psychoneuroendocrinology. 1994;20509- 514
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Lockwood
CRadunovic
NNastic
DPetkovic
SAigner
SBerkowitz
G Corticotrophin-releasing hormone and related pituitary-adrenal axis hormones in fetal and maternal blood during the second half of pregnancy.
J Perinatal Med. 1996;24243- 251
Google ScholarCrossref 39.Smith
R Alterations in the hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis during pregnancy and the placental clock that determines the length of parturition.
J Reprod Immunol. 1998;39215- 220
Google ScholarCrossref 40.Dorn
LSusman
E Serum and saliva cortisol relations in adolescents during pregnancy and the early postpartum period.
Biol Psychiatry. 1993;34226- 233
Google ScholarCrossref 41.Yehuda
RSouthwick
SNussbaum
GWahby
VGiller
E
JrMason
J Low urinary cortisol excretion in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 1990;178366- 369
Google ScholarCrossref 42.Rosenbaum
PRubin
DB The central role of the propensity score in observational studies for causal effects.
Biometrika. 1983;7041- 55
Google ScholarCrossref 43.Stoff
DMPasatiempo
APYeung
JCooper
TBBridger
WHRabinovich
H Neuroendocrine responses to challenge with
dl-fenfluramine and aggression in disruptive behavior disorders of children and adolescents.
Psychiatry Res. 1992;43263- 276
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Kruesi
MJPSchmidt
MEDonnelly
MHibbs
EDHamburger
SD Urinary free cortisol output and disruptive behavior in children.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1989;28441- 443
Google ScholarCrossref 45.Schulz
KPHalperin
JMNewcorn
JHSharma
VGabriel
S Plasma cortisol and aggression in boys with ADHD.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1997;36605- 609
Google ScholarCrossref 46.Chrousos
GPTorpy
DJGold
PW Interactions between the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis and the female reproductive system: clinical implications.
Ann Intern Med. 1998;129229- 240
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Vamvakopoulos
NChrousos
G Hormonal regulation of human corticotrophin-releasing gene expression: implications for the stress response and immune/inflammatory reaction.
Endocr Rev. 1994;15409- 420
Google ScholarCrossref 48.Gallucci
WTBaum
ALave
IRabin
DSChrousos
GPGold
PWKling
MA Sex differences in sensitivity of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis.
Health Psychol. 1993;12420- 425
Google ScholarCrossref 49.Riggs
PD Clinical approach to treatment of ADHD in adolescents with substance use disorders and conduct disorder.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;37331- 332
Google ScholarCrossref 50.Zoccolillo
M Co-occurrence of conduct disorder and its adult outcomes with depressive and anxiety disorders: a review.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1992;31547- 556
Google ScholarCrossref 51.Rey
JMMorris-Yates
A Are oppositional and conduct disorders of adolescents separate conditions?
Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 1993;27281- 287
Google ScholarCrossref 52.Biederman
JNewcorn
JSprich
S Comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with conduct, depressive, anxiety, and other disorder.
Am J Psychiatry. 1991;148564- 577
Google Scholar 53.Chrousos
GPGold
PW The concepts of stress and stress system disorders: overview of physical and behavioral homeostasis.
JAMA. 1992;2671244- 1252
Google ScholarCrossref 54.Chrousos
GElenkov
I Interactions of the endocrine and immune systems. DeGroot
Led
Endocrinology 4th ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co2000;571- 586
Google Scholar 55.Crofford
LJPillemer
SRKalogeras
KTCash
JMMichelson
DKling
MSternberg
EMGold
PWChrousos
GPWilder
RL Hypothalamic-pituiatry-adrenal axis perturbations in patients with fibromyalgia.
Arthritis Rheum. 1994;371583- 1592
Google ScholarCrossref 56.Strickland
PMorriss
RWearden
ADeakin
B A comparison of salivary cortisol in chronic fatigue syndrome, community depression and healthy controls.
J Affect Disord. 1998;47191- 194
Google ScholarCrossref 57.Buske-Kirschbaum
AJobst
SWustmans
AKirschbaum
CRauh
WHellhammer
D Attenuated free cortisol response to psychosocial stress in children with atopic dermatitis.
Psychosom Med. 1997;59419- 426
Google ScholarCrossref 58.Rupprecht
MSalzer
BRaum
BHornstein
OPKoch
HURiederer
PSofic
ERupprecht
R Physical stress-induced secretion of adrenal and pituitary hormones in patients with atopic eczema compared with normal controls.
Exp Clin Endocrinol Diabetes. 1997;10539- 45
Google ScholarCrossref 59.Zinner
SH Treatment and prevention of infections in immunocompromised hosts. Gorbach
SLBartlett
JGBlacklow
NReds
Infectious Diseases 2nd ed. Philadelphia, Pa WB Saunders Co1992;1252- 1260
Google Scholar 60.Zadeh
HHNichols
FCMiyasaki
KT The role of the cell-mediated immune response to
Actinobacillus actinomycetemcomitans and
Porphyromonas gingivalis in periodontitis.
Periodontology 2000. 1999;20239- 288
Google ScholarCrossref 61.Heim
CEhlert
UHellhammer
DH The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders.
Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2000;251- 35
Google ScholarCrossref 62.Forst
MHarry
JGoddard
P A health-profile comparison of delinquent and homeless youths.
J Health Care Poor Underserved. 1993;4386- 400
Google ScholarCrossref 63.Hein
KCohen
MLitt
ISchonberg
SKMeyer
MRMarks
ASheehy
AJ Juvenile detention: another boundary issue for physicians.
Pediatrics. 1980;66239- 245
Google Scholar 64.Feinstein
RLampkin
ALorish
CKlerman
LMaisiak
ROh
M Medical status of adolescents at time of admission to juvenile detention center.
J Adolesc Health. 1998;22190- 196
Google ScholarCrossref 65.Dolan
MHolloway
JBailey
SSmith
C Health status of juvenile offenders: a survey of young offenders appearing before the juvenile courts.
J Adolesc. 1999;22137- 144
Google ScholarCrossref 67.Nierenberg
AAFeinstein
AR How to evaluate a diagnostic marker test: lessons from the rise and fall of dexamethasone suppression test.
JAMA. 1988;2591699- 1702
Google ScholarCrossref 68.Samuelson
SWinokur
G Biological markers. Hsu
LHersen
Meds
Research in Psychiatry Issues, Strategies, and Methods New York, NY Plenum Medical Book Co1992;235- 263
Google Scholar 69.Cauffman
EFeldman
SWaterman
JSteiner
H Posttraumatic stress disorder among female juvenile offenders.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1998;371209- 1216
Google ScholarCrossref 70.Friess
EWidemann
KSteiger
AHolsboer
F The hypothalmic-pituitary-adrenocortical system and sleep in man.
Adv Neuroimmunol. 1995;5111- 125
Google ScholarCrossref 71.Delville
YMelloni
RJFerris
C Behavioral and neurobiological consequences of social subjugation during puberty in golden hamsters.
J Neurosci. 1998;182667- 2672
Google Scholar 72.Coccaro
EFKavoussi
RJHauger
RLCooper
TBFerris
CF Cerebrospinal fluid vasopressin levels: correlates with aggression and serotonin function in personality-disordered subjects.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1998;55708- 714
Google ScholarCrossref 73.Susman
EJSchmeddlk
KHWorrall
BKGranger
DAPonirakis
AChrouso
GP Corticotropin-releasing hormone and cortisol: longitudinal associations with depression and antisocial behavior in pregnant adolescents.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1999;38460- 467
Google ScholarCrossref