Background
Urban and rural populations have different rates of psychotic illness. If psychosis exists as a continuous phenotype in nature, urban-rural population differences in the rate of psychotic disorder should be accompanied by similar differences in the rate of abnormal mental states characterized by psychotic or psychosislike symptoms.
Methods
A random sample of 7076 individuals aged 18 to 64 years were interviewed by trained lay interviewers with the Composite International Diagnostic Interview. Approximately half of those with evidence of psychosis according to the Composite International Diagnostic Interview were additionally interviewed by clinicians. We investigated associations between a 5-level urbanicity rating and (1) any DSM-III-R diagnosis of psychotic disorder (sample prevalence, 1.5%), (2) any rating of hallucinations and/or delusions (sample prevalence, 4.2%), and (3) any rating of psychotic or psychosislike symptoms (sample prevalence, 17.5%).
Results
Level of urbanicity was associated not only with DSM-III-R psychotic disorder (adjusted odds ratio [OR] over 5 levels, 1.47; 95% confidence interval [CI], 1.25-1.72), but also, independently, with any rating of delusion and/or hallucination (adjusted OR, 1.28; 95% CI, 1.17-1.40; clinician-assessed psychotic symptoms only: OR, 1.30; 95% CI, 1.03-1.64) and any rating of psychosislike symptom (adjusted OR, 1.18; 95% CI, 1.13-1.24). Psychotic symptoms were strongly and independently associated with psychotic disorder, regardless of the level of urbanization.
Conclusions
Community level of psychotic and psychosislike symptoms may be inextricably linked to the prevalence of psychotic disorder. The prevalence of abnormal mental states that facilitate development to overt psychotic illness increases progressively with level of urbanization.
SOME RISK FACTORS for psychotic illness can be used to define populations with different levels of risk. For example, exposure to urban birth, upbringing, or residence increases the risk for later psychotic illness, suggesting that urban and rural populations have different lifetime risks.1-4 A plausible explanation for the urban-rural differences is that environmental factors associated with urban life make individuals more vulnerable to the development of psychotic states.
The factors that increase the risk for psychotic disorder in urban areas may contribute to a much larger pool of preclinical psychotic experiences, of which only a small proportion may continue to result in overt disorder. This hypothesis would be compatible with the suspected multifactorial origin of psychotic disorders, according to which it is unlikely that any multifactorial disease exists as a purely dichotomous entity in nature without less severe, nonpathologic manifestations of the phenotype.5 It is also compatible with accumulating evidence that schizotypal signs and psychosislike symptoms such as delusional ideation and isolated hallucinations are prevalent in the general population6-9 and show longitudinal,10-12 neuropsychological,13 psychopathologic,14 familial,15 neuroradiologic,16,17 epidemiologic,6,18 and risk factor19 continuities with clinical psychotic syndromes such as schizophrenia. All of these data suggest that, at the level of the general population, lesser psychotic states exist that are associated with the more severe clinical disorders that necessitate hospital admission.
Given these continuities, it is attractive to hypothesize that the higher level of psychotic disorders in urban areas is accompanied by a similar increase in the level of psychosislike symptoms. Population studies of minor psychiatric disorders have shown that the prevalence of disorder is linearly related to the mean number of psychiatric symptoms.20,21 A similar relationship between symptoms and disorder may exist in psychosis.
In the current study, we investigated to what degree the increase in risk for psychotic disorder associated with urban life is reflected in similar increases in the mean number of psychotic and psychosislike symptoms. We hypothesized that the mean level of symptoms would increase with the rate of disorder across increasingly urbanized areas. In addition, we hypothesized that the association between symptoms and disorder would be constant across the populations in the different strata, suggesting variation of susceptibility between populations rather than within populations.20 For example, if the rate of some rare psychotic disorder is higher in population A than in population B, whereas their levels of more prevalent psychotic symptoms are the same, a likely explanation is that (1) some rare cause of a rare disorder is more prevalent in population A and (2) psychosislike experiences in the population are qualitatively distinct from the disorder. Thus, populations A and B are essentially similar, except for the distribution of some rare cause affecting a few individuals. There is variation within populations. If, however, not only the rate of disorder, but also the rate of symptoms, is higher in population A than in population B, a likely explanation would be that (1) the population level of vulnerability differs between population A and population B and (2) the psychosislike experiences are, at least in part, on a quantitative continuum with disorder. A graphic representation of this argument, using hypothetical data, is depicted in Figure 1.
The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study is a prospective study with 3 measurement points during a period of 3 years.22-24 The current report is based on the baseline data. A multistage, stratified, random sampling procedure was used to select first 90 municipalities, then a sample of private households, and finally a Dutch-speaking individual aged 18 to 64 years within each household. Selected households were sent an introductory letter by the Minister of Health, inviting them to participate. A total of 7076 individuals provided informed consent and were interviewed at baseline, representing a response rate of 69.7%. Nearly 44% of nonresponders agreed to complete a mailed questionnaire, including a General Health Questionnaire,25 and were found to have the same mean score on the questionnaire (responders, 1.19; nonresponders, 1.16). Nonresponse was not associated with level of urbanicity.22,23 The sample was representative of the Dutch population in terms of sex, marital status, and level of urbanization,23 with the exception of a slight underrepresentation of individuals in the age group 18 to 24 years. As this was a study of relative rather than absolute risk, no poststratification weightings were applied to the data.
Subjects were interviewed at home. The Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) version 1.126-28 was used, yielding DSM-III-R diagnoses. The CIDI was designed for trained interviewers who are not clinicians and has high interrater reliability29,30 and high test-retest reliability.31-33 Ninety interviewers experienced in systematic data collection gathered the data, having received a 3-day training course in recruiting and interviewing, followed by a 4-day course at the World Health Organization–CIDI training center in Amsterdam, the Netherlands. Extensive monitoring and quality checks took place throughout the entire data collection period.23
Lifetime ratings from the 17 CIDI core psychosis sections on delusions (13 items) and hallucinations (4 items) were used (items G1-G13, G15, G16, G20, and G21). These concern classic psychotic symptoms involving, for example, persecution, thought interference, auditory hallucinations, and passivity phenomena. All of these items can be rated in 6 ways: 1, no symptom; 2, symptom present but not clinically relevant (not bothered by it and not seeking help for it); 3, symptom the result of ingestion of drugs; 4, symptom the result of somatic disease; 5, true psychiatric symptom; and 6, may not really be a symptom because there appears to be some plausible explanation for it. Because psychotic symptoms are difficult to diagnose in a structured interview,34-36 clinical reinterviews were conducted over the telephone by an experienced trainee psychiatrist for all individuals who had at least 1 rating of 5 or 6, using questions from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R, an instrument with proved reliability and validity in diagnosing schizophrenia.37 The CIDI ratings were corrected on the basis of these clinical interviews, and the Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study DSM-III-R diagnoses of psychotic disorder are based on the data from these clinical reinterviews.
To examine the validity of the contrasts implied in the different CIDI ratings, associations were compared between the ratings and lifetime mental health service use (see below) and quality of life measured with the 36-item Short Form Health Survey quality-of-life schedule.38,39
Psychotic disorder was defined as any DSM-III-R affective or nonaffective psychotic diagnosis. Psychotic symptom was defined as any CIDI rating of 2, 3, 4, 5, or 6 on any of the 17 CIDI core psychosis items. A previous study showed that all 5 of these ratings on the CIDI psychosis items were strongly associated with each other, including the clinical reinterview ratings of psychotic symptom (ie, a rating of 5 on any of the CIDI psychosis items). In addition, the 5 ratings independently showed a similar pattern of associations with known risk factors for psychosis.19 As they therefore appear to reflect the same underlying latent dimension of "psychosis," they were joined into a single broad rating of psychotic symptom for the purpose of the current study. To check on the validity of this procedure, associations were also examined with "psychotic symptom" narrowly defined as a clinical reinterview rating of psychosis (ie, a rating of 5 on any of the CIDI psychosis items).
Five levels of urbanization were defined, following the standard classification of urbanization of place of residence according to the Dutch Central Bureau of Statistics. These are based on the density of addresses per square kilometer in an area and are classified as less than 500, 500 to 999, 1000 to 1499, 1500 to 2499, and 2500 or more. This density is calculated by assessing the density of addresses in a circle of 1 km around each address. The density of addresses in an area is then calculated as the mean address density of all the addresses in that area.40
The lifetime prevalences of at least 1 psychotic symptom, broadly and narrowly defined, and of any psychotic disorder were examined in relation to level of urbanicity of place of residence, adjusted for the a priori selected possible confounding effects of age in years, sex, level of education (4 levels), and country of birth of subject, subject's mother, and subject's father (coded Dutch-born, foreign-born, and information missing).
To assess the independence of any associations with urbanicity in the different symptom groups, associations were also examined after exclusion of individuals with psychotic disorder from the group with narrowly defined psychotic symptoms, and after exclusion of individuals with psychotic disorder and individuals with narrowly defined psychotic symptoms from the group with broadly defined psychotic symptoms.
To examine the possible effects of selective "drift" of mental health patients toward urban areas, we also looked for interactions with lifetime history of mental health care (any contact with community mental health center, psychiatric outpatient clinic, private psychiatrist, psychologist, or psychotherapist, or any psychiatric admission or day treatment; n = 1352). Logistic regression yielding odds ratios (ORs) and 95% confidence intervals (CIs)41 was used to examine associations between psychotic disorder on the one hand, and psychotic symptoms broadly and narrowly defined (coded 0, 1, 2, 3, 4, and 5 or more symptoms) on the other, at different levels of urbanicity. Interactions were assessed by likelihood ratio (LR) tests.42
Of the 479 individuals who were eligible for a clinical reinterview over the telephone, 226 (47.2%) were actually interviewed. Successful reinterview was not associated with level of urbanicity (OR, 0.91; 95% CI, 0.79-1.04). Of a possible 226 × 17 = 3842 CIDI ratings of psychotic symptoms in the 226 individuals who were reinterviewed, changes after clinical reinterview were introduced in 266 ratings (6.9%). Change of CIDI rating was not associated with level of urbanicity (OR, 0.95; 95% CI, 0.86-1.04). Sensitivity analyses representing the possible extremes of clinical reinterview were performed to examine whether differential clinical reinterview rates could have biased the results.
The sample consisted of 7076 individuals (46.6% male) with a mean age of 41.2 years (SD, 12.2). There were 936 individuals (13.2%) who were foreign-born or whose father or mother was foreign-born. The prevalences of the different CIDI ratings on the 17 psychosis items were as follows: any CIDI rating of 2, n = 915 (12.9%); any CIDI rating of 3 or 4, n = 39 (0.6%); any CIDI rating of 5, n = 295 (4.2%); and any CIDI rating of 6, n = 285 (4.0%). After adjustment for age, sex, ethnic group, educational level, level of urbanicity, and presence of any psychotic or nonpsychotic DSM-III-R diagnosis, all but one of the different CIDI ratings were independently (assessed by entering them together in the model) associated with a lifetime history of mental health service (rating 2: OR, 0.99; 95% CI, 0.82-1.19; rating 3 or 4: OR, 2.65; 95% CI, 1.30-5.44; rating 5: OR, 4.19; 95% CI, 3.23-5.44; rating 6: OR, 3.12; 95% CI, 2.39-4.06), and all were associated with a lower quality-of-life total score measured with the quality-of-life schedule of the 36-item Short Form Health Survey (rating 2: multiple regression coefficient B = −1.99; 95% CI, −3.24 to −0.75; rating 3 or 4: B = −6.78; 95% CI, −12.25 to −1.13; rating 5: B = −6.79; 95% CI, −8.94 to −4.63; rating 6: B = −3.09; 95% CI, −5.25 to −0.94).
The lifetime prevalence of DSM-III-R schizophrenia, schizoaffective psychosis, and schizophreniform disorder was 0.37% (26 cases), and the lifetime prevalence of affective psychosis (major depression or bipolar disorder with psychotic features) was 1.14% (81 cases), making a total of 107 cases (1.51%). The prevalence of psychotic symptoms broadly defined was 17.5% (n = 1237), and the prevalence of psychotic symptoms narrowly defined was 4.2% (n = 295).
Psychosis in relation to urbanicity
The lifetime prevalences of DSM-III-R psychotic disorder, psychotic symptoms narrowly defined, and psychotic symptoms broadly defined increased in a monotonic fashion with level of urbanicity. Adjustment for age, sex, level of education, and country of birth of subject and parents changed the parameters only by a small amount (Table 1). Associations also remained after exclusion of individuals with psychotic disorder from the group with narrowly defined psychotic symptoms, and after exclusion of individuals with psychotic disorder and individuals with narrowly defined psychotic symptoms from the group with broadly defined psychotic symptoms (Table 1). There was no interaction with lifetime mental health treatment for any of the 3 groups (LR test psychotic disorder: χ21 = 0.05, P = .83; LR test narrowly defined: χ21 = 0.21, P = .64; LR test broadly defined: χ21 = 0.00, P = .98).
The following analyses were conducted. First, we repeated the analyses of association between urbanicity and the 3 groups of symptom ratings, adjusted for age, sex, level of education, and country of birth of subject and parents, excluding the 253 individuals who were eligible for clinical reinterview but who were not interviewed. Second, we repeated the same analyses restricted to the group who were interviewed by the psychiatric trainees (n = 226). Third, we repeated the analyses assuming that all individuals who were eligible for reinterview, but were not reinterviewed, would have received a rating of 5 (true symptom) on all CIDI psychosis items if they had been reinterviewed. Finally, we repeated the same analyses assuming that these individuals would have received a rating of 1 (no symptom) on all ratings. The pattern of results for all these analyses was the same and was similar to the results in Table 1 (Table 2).
Associations between symptoms and disorder at different levels of urbanicity
Psychotic symptoms, broadly and narrowly defined, were strongly associated with psychotic disorder (broadly defined: summary OR over 6 levels, 3.59; 95% CI, 3.17-4.06; narrowly defined: OR, 6.96; 95% CI, 5.57-8.68). The association between psychotic disorder and broadly defined psychotic symptoms remained after adjustment for presence of narrowly defined psychotic symptoms (adjusted OR, 2.29; 95% CI, 1.93-2.73), indicating that broadly defined symptoms were associated with psychotic disorder independent of their association with narrow symptoms. There was no evidence that this association differed as a function of urbanicity (LR test broadly defined: χ24 = 4.20, P = .38; LR test narrowly defined: χ24 = 4.51, P = .34), or if the sample was restricted to those who had had clinical reinterviews (n = 226; P = .54 and P = .95, respectively).
In a representative sample of 7076 subjects sampled from the general population, lifetime level of psychotic and psychosislike symptoms independently increased with level of urbanicity in the same manner as did DSM-III-R psychotic disorder. Initial response rate, clinical reinterview response rate, and change rate of CIDI psychosis items after clinical reinterview were not associated with level of urbanicity. At all levels of urbanicity, psychosislike symptoms were strongly associated with psychotic disorder. These findings therefore suggest that the increased prevalence of psychotic disorder in urban environments should be interpreted in light of increased levels of "psychosis proneness" in urban populations.
More generally, the results suggest that there is a link between community level of psychotic symptoms and rates of clinical disorder. As the association between symptoms and disorder did not differ as a function of urbanicity, the implication is that susceptibility to psychotic disorder varies between populations and can be demonstrated by comparing rates of psychosislike phenomena. Given that the rates of these phenomena are much higher than those for rare psychotic disorders, etiologic research may be served by focusing on these related phenotypes. Similarly, preventive action may be served by population interventions rather than, or in addition to, the high-risk strategies that are currently being explored.
The high rates of psychotic illness in urban environments may be the result of the influence of environmental factors. As the urban effect appears to have its impact during urban upbringing rather than during adult residence per se,1,3 developmental mechanisms ought to be considered. A possible developmental mechanism whereby social factors may create enduring liabilities for adult psychosis are the effects of the wider social environment, such as the neighborhood environment, on child and adolescent development.43 Mental states are reactive to experience, and differences in the level of deprivation and social isolation of the neighborhood environment in urban areas have been shown to be associated with variation in a range of mental health outcomes from problem behavior in children44 to incidence of neurosis and schizophrenia.45,46 High levels of deprivation and low levels of social capital47 in the wider social environment may enhance the development of "at-risk" mental states that in turn may facilitate the onset of clinical psychosis in adult life.
These results should be viewed in light of several possible limitations. First, we examined lifetime rates of disorder and symptoms in relation to current urban residence. Thus, one explanation for the findings is that symptomatic individuals could have "drifted" to urban areas. Although we cannot exclude this mechanism, a previous report by our group found that there was a high degree of lifetime stability of urban exposure status (around 75% of individuals living in urbanized areas had also been born there), indicating that current exposure is likely to reflect stable lifetime exposure in most cases.3 In addition, the association between psychosis and urbanicity did not differ as a function of lifetime mental health patient status. This suggests that it is unlikely that the findings can be explained solely by a process of urban drift of the most symptomatic individuals, in which case one would have expected associations to be stronger in the patient group. Second, the validity of the CIDI ratings of psychosis, such as a rating of 2 (symptom present but not clinically relevant) is not well researched. However, the associations with lifetime mental health service contact (no association with rating of 2 and significant associations with other ratings, especially rating of 5 for true psychotic symptom) and lower quality of life (weakest for rating of 2 and strongest for rating of 5), independent of each other and of any lifetime DSM-III-R diagnosis, provided some degree of validity for the conceptual contrast of the ratings as well as for their existence independent of psychiatric disorder. Third, psychotic symptom ratings were assessed by lay interviewers (CIDI ratings of 1, no symptom; 2, symptom present but not clinically relevant; 3, symptom the result of ingestion of drugs; and 4, symptom the result of somatic disease), whereas the other CIDI ratings (5, true psychiatric symptom; and 6, may not really be a symptom because there appears to be some plausible explanation for it) were assessed by clinicians through telephone interviews in approximately 50% of eligible cases. Although the sensitivity analyses showed that incomplete rates of reinterview by clinicians are unlikely to have biased the results, it is likely that, especially with lay interviewer ratings,1-4 a degree of misclassification did occur. However, for misclassification to explain the results, one would have to hypothesize that with greater degrees of urbanization, interviewers became progressively more likely to misclassify in one specific direction, and, furthermore, this influence of misclassification would have had to be the same for lay interviewers and clinicians. Although this cannot be excluded, it is unlikely, also because the rate of change in CIDI ratings after clinician reinterview was not associated with urbanicity and because the findings were similar when the sample was restricted to those who had been interviewed by a clinician.
Accepted for publication January 23, 2001.
Corresponding author and reprints: Jim van Os, MD, PhD, MRCPsych, Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, Maastricht University, European Graduate School of Neuroscience, PO Box 616 (PAR 45), 6200 MD Maastricht, the Netherlands.
2.Marcelis
MNavarro-Mateu
FMurray
RSelten
JPvan Os
J Urbanization and psychosis: a study of 1942-1978 birth cohorts in the Netherlands.
Psychol Med. 1998;28871- 879
Google ScholarCrossref 3.Marcelis
MTakei
Nvan Os
J Urbanization and risk for schizophrenia: does the effect operate before or around the time of illness onset?
Psychol Med. 1999;291197- 1203
Google ScholarCrossref 4.Mortensen
PBPedersen
CBWestergaard
TWohlfahrt
JEwald
HMors
OAndersen
PKMelbye
M Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia.
N Engl J Med. 1999;340603- 608
Google ScholarCrossref 5.Sham
P Statistics in Human Genetics. London, England Edward Arnold Publisher1998;
6.Tien
AY Distributions of hallucinations in the population.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1991;26287- 292
Google ScholarCrossref 7.Verdoux
HMaurice-Tison
SGay
Bvan Os
JSalamon
RBourgeois
ML A survey of delusional ideation in primary-care patients.
Psychol Med. 1998;28127- 134
Google ScholarCrossref 8.Eaton
WWRomanoski
AAnthony
JCNestadt
G Screening for psychosis in the general population with a self-report interview.
J Nerv Ment Dis. 1991;179689- 693
Google ScholarCrossref 9.McKellar
P Experience and Behaviour. Harmondsworth, England Penguin Press1968;
10.Chapman
LJChapman
JPKwapil
TREckblad
MZinser
MC Putatively psychosis-prone subjects 10 years later.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1994;103171- 183
Google ScholarCrossref 11.Kwapil
TRMiller
MBZinser
MCChapman
JChapman
LJ Magical ideation and social anhedonia as predictors of psychosis proneness: a partial replication.
J Abnorm Psychol. 1997;106491- 495
Google ScholarCrossref 12.Poulton
RCaspi
AMoffitt
TECannon
MMurray
RHarrington
H Children's self-reported psychotic symptoms and adult schizophreniform disorder: a 15-year longitudinal study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;571053- 1058
Google ScholarCrossref 13.Chen
WJLiu
SKChang
CJLien
YJChang
YHHwu
HG Sustained attention deficit and schizotypal personality features in nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients.
Am J Psychiatry. 1998;1551214- 1220
Google Scholar 14.Gruzelier
JH The factorial structure of schizotypy, part I: affinities with syndromes of schizophrenia.
Schizophr Bull. 1996;22611- 620
Google ScholarCrossref 15.Kendler
KSMcGuire
MGruenberg
AMWalsh
D Schizotypal symptoms and signs in the Roscommon Family Study: their factor structure and familial relationship with psychotic and affective disorders.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1995;52296- 303
Google ScholarCrossref 16.Buchsbaum
MSYang
SHazlett
ESiegel
BV
JrGermans
MHaznedar
MO'Flaithbheartaigh
SWei
TSilverman
JSiever
LJ Ventricular volume and asymmetry in schizotypal personality disorder and schizophrenia assessed with magnetic resonance imaging.
Schizophr Res. 1997;2745- 53
Google ScholarCrossref 17.Dickey
CCMcCarley
RWVoglmaier
MMNiznikiewicz
MASeidman
LJHirayasu
YFischer
ITeh
EKVan Rhoads
RJakab
MKikinis
RJolesz
FAShenton
ME Schizotypal personality disorder and MRI abnormalities of temporal lobe gray matter.
Biol Psychiatry. 1999;451393- 1402
Google ScholarCrossref 18.Peters
ERJoseph
SAGarety
PA Measurement of delusional ideation in the normal population: introducing the PDI (Peters et al. Delusions Inventory).
Schizophr Bull. 1999;25553- 576
Google ScholarCrossref 19.van Os
JHanssen
MBijl
RVRavelli
A Strauss (1969) revisited: a psychosis continuum in the general population?
Schizophr Res. 2000;4511- 20
Google ScholarCrossref 20.Anderson
JHuppert
FRose
G Normality, deviance and minor psychiatric morbidity in the community.
Psychol Med. 1993;23475- 485
Google ScholarCrossref 21.Whittington
JEHuppert
FA Changes in the prevalence of psychiatric disorder in a community are related to changes in the mean level of psychiatric symptoms.
Psychol Med. 1996;261253- 1260
Google ScholarCrossref 22.Bijl
RVRavelli
AVan Zessen
G Prevalence of psychiatric disorder in the general population: results from the Netherlands mental health survey and incidence study.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1998;33587- 596
Google ScholarCrossref 23.Bijl
RVVan Zessen
GRavelli
ADe Rijk
CLangendoen
Y The Netherlands Mental Health Survey and Incidence Study (NEMESIS): objectives and design.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1998;33581- 587
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Alegria
MBijl
RVLin
EWalters
EEKessler
RC Income differences in persons seeking outpatient treatment for mental disorders: a comparison of the United States with Ontario and the Netherlands.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57383- 391
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Goldberg
DWilliams
P User's Guide to the GHQ. Windsor, England NFER-Nelson1988;
26.Robins
LNWing
JWittchen
HUHelzer
JEBabor
TFBurke
JFarmer
AJablenski
APickens
RRegier
DASartorius
NTowle
LH The Composite International Diagnostic Interview: an epidemiologic instrument suitable for use in conjunction with different diagnostic systems and in different cultures.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1988;451069- 1077
Google ScholarCrossref 27.World Health Organization, Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) Version 1.0. Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1990;
28.Smeets
RMWDingemans
PMAJ Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI) Version 1.1. Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1993;
29.Wittchen
HURobins
LNCottler
LBSartorius
NBurke
JDRegier
D Cross-cultural feasibility, reliability and sources of variance of the Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI): the Multicentre WHO/ADAMHA Field Trials.
Br J Psychiatry. 1991;159645- 653[published correction appears in
Br J Psychiatry. 1992;160:136].
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Cottler
LBRobins
LNGrant
BFBlaine
JTowle
LHWittchen
H-USartorius
Nparticipants in the WHO/ADAMHA field trials, The CIDI-core substance abuse and dependence questions: cross-cultural and nosological issues.
Br J Psychiatry. 1991;159653- 658
Google ScholarCrossref 31.Semler
GVon Cranach
MWittchen
H-U Comparison Between the Composite International Diagnostic Interview and the Present State Examination: Report to the WHO/ADAMHA Task Force on Instrument Development. Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1987;
32.Wacker
HRBattegay
RMullejans
RSchlosser
C Using the CIDI in the general population. Stefanis
CNRabavilas
ADSoldatos
CReds.
Psychiatry A World Perspective Amsterdam, the Netherlands Elsevier Science Publishers1990;138- 143
Google Scholar 33.Wittchen
HU Reliability and validity studies of the WHO–Composite International Diagnostic Interview (CIDI): a critical review.
J Psychiatr Res. 1994;2857- 84
Google ScholarCrossref 34.Helzer
JERobins
LNMcEvoy
LTSpitznagel
ELStoltzman
RKFarmer
ABrockington
IF A comparison of clinical and diagnostic interview schedule diagnoses: physician reexamination of lay-interviewed cases in the general population.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42657- 666
Google ScholarCrossref 35.Anthony
JCFolstein
MRomanoski
AJVon Korff
MRNestadt
GRChahal
RMerchant
ABrown
CHShapiro
SKramer
MGruenberg
EM Comparison of the lay diagnostic interview schedule and a standardized psychiatric diagnosis: experience in Eastern Baltimore.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1985;42667- 675
Google ScholarCrossref 36.Cooper
SACollacott
RA Relapse of depression in people with Down's syndrome.
Br J Dev Disabil. 1994;4032- 37
Google ScholarCrossref 37.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JBGibbon
MFirst
MB The Structured Clinical Interview for
DSM-III-R (SCID), I: history, rationale, and description.
Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1992;49624- 629
Google ScholarCrossref 38.Ware
JEGandek
BGroup
IP The SF-36 Health Survey: development and use in mental health research and the IQOLA project.
Int J Ment Health. 1994;2349- 73
Google Scholar 39.Van der Zee
KSanderman
RHeyink
J De Psychometrische kwaliteiten van de MOS 36-item Short Form Health Survey (SF-36) in een Nederlandse populatie.
Tijdschr Soc Gezondheidzorg. 1993;71183- 191
Google Scholar 40.Central Bureau of Statistics, Bevolking der Gemeenten van Nederland. The Hague, the Netherlands CBS Publications1993;
42.Radack
KLRouan
GHedges
J The likelihood ratio: an improved measure for reporting and evaluating diagnostic test results.
Arch Pathol Lab Med. 1986;110689- 693
Google Scholar 43.Leventhal
TBrooks-Gunn
J The neighborhoods they live in: the effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes.
Psychol Bull. 2000;126309- 337
Google ScholarCrossref 44.Kalff
ACKroes
MVles
JSHHendriksen
JGMFeron
FJMSteyaert
JVan Zeben
TMCBJolles
Jvan Os
J Neighbourhood-level and individual-level effects on child problem behaviour: a multilevel analysis.
J Epidemiol Community Health. 2001;55246- 250
Google ScholarCrossref 45.Driessen
GGunther
Nvan Os
J Shared social environment and psychiatric disorder: a multilevel analysis of individual and ecological effects.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 1998;33596- 605
Google ScholarCrossref 46.van Os
JDriessen
GGunther
NDelespaul
P Neighbourhood variation in schizophrenia incidence: evidence for person-environment interaction.
Br J Psychiatry. 2000;176243- 249
Google ScholarCrossref 47.Kawachi
IKennedy
BPLochner
KProthrow-Stith
D Social capital, income inequality, and mortality.
Am J Public Health. 1997;871491- 1498
Google ScholarCrossref