Context
Although depression, anxiety, and hostility/anger have each been associated with an increased risk of coronary artery disease, these overlapping negative emotions have not been simultaneously examined as predictors of the progression of subclinical atherosclerosis.
Objective
To evaluate the relative importance of depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and hostility/anger in predicting subclinical atherosclerotic progression over a 3-year period.
Design/Setting
The Pittsburgh Healthy Heart Project, an ongoing prospective cohort study of healthy, older men and women from the general community. At baseline, questionnaires were administered to assess depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, hostility, anger experience, and anger expression. Mean carotid intima-media thickness was assessed by B-mode ultrasonography during the baseline and 3-year follow-up visits.
Participants
Of the 464 adults enrolled in the project, 324 (69.8%) were included in this report because they had complete baseline and follow-up data.
Main Outcome Measure
Three-year change in mean carotid intima-media thickness.
Results
Regression analyses indicated that higher depressive symptoms at baseline were associated with greater 3-year change in carotid intima-media thickness (ΔR2 = 0.026, P = .002), even after taking into account demographic factors, cardiovascular risk factors, medication use, medical conditions, and other correlated negative emotions. Measures of anxiety symptoms, hostility, anger experience, and anger expression were each unrelated to intima-media thickness change. Post hoc analyses examining depressive symptom clusters showed that the somatic-vegetative symptoms of depression (ΔR2 = 0.027, P = .002), but not the cognitive-affective symptoms, were positively associated with intima-media thickness change.
Conclusion
Our findings suggest that the somatic-vegetative features of depression, but perhaps not anxiety and hostility/anger, may play an important role in the earlier stages of the development of coronary artery disease.
Considerable evidence from prospective studies suggests that individual differences in the experience and expression of various negative emotions—ie, depression, anxiety, and hostility/anger—are associated with an increased risk of coronary artery disease (CAD) in initially healthy populations.1 A limitation of this literature, however, is that most investigations have examined the influence of a single negative emotion (eg, depression only) on CAD outcomes. As Suls and Bunde1(p270) recently discussed, this approach is not ideal because of the “appreciable construct and measurement overlap” that exists between these psychological factors. For instance, self-report measures of depression are highly correlated with anxiety measures (coefficients typically range from 0.45-0.75),2 and self-report measures of hostility/anger are moderately correlated with both depression and anxiety measures (coefficients usually range from 0.25-0.50).3-6 Because of this overlap, it is possible that all 3 of these negative emotions are independent risk factors, with each factor uniquely contributing to CAD development. It is also possible that 1 or more of these negative emotions are merely markers for another negative emotion that itself is a CAD risk factor (eg, hostility may predict future CAD simply by virtue of its relationship with depression). A third possibility is that these negative emotions reflect the same underlying factor (eg, negative affectivity7), and it is this common factor that is strongly associated with CAD risk and that accounts for the individual negative emotion–CAD relationships.
To identify the aspects of these overlapping negative emotions that may be involved in the pathogenesis of CAD, longitudinal studies are needed in which the effects of depression, anxiety, and hostility/anger on CAD outcomes are simultaneously examined. Unfortunately, to our knowledge, only 2 such investigations involving healthy individuals have been conducted. Chang and colleagues8 found that men who reported the highest level of anger in response to stress were at increased risk of premature myocardial infarction 32 to 48 years later, even after adjustment for depression and anxiety measures. However, because parallel analyses were not reported for depression and anxiety, it is unclear whether these negative emotions were also independent predictors of premature myocardial infarction. In another sample of men, Kubzansky et al9 observed that the shared aspects of depression, anxiety, and anger and the unique aspects of anxiety were both predictors of incident CAD, independent of one another and independent of the unique aspects of depression and anger. Those findings are difficult to interpret, however, given that the scales created for that study were not standard measures of negative emotions (eg, the depression measure assessed only hopelessness and suicidal ideation).
In addition to concurrently evaluating these overlapping negative emotions, another important direction for future research is to examine the stage(s) of CAD development during which these psychological factors may exert an influence. In most previous investigations of initially healthy persons, the outcome measure has been the incidence of clinical CAD, typically defined as the occurrence of a coronary event (eg, myocardial infarction or sudden cardiac death).1 Because a coronary event is the final step of the pathogenesis of CAD,10 it is presently not known whether depression, anxiety, and hostility/anger may play a role in the earlier stages of CAD development (eg, the initiation and progression of atherosclerosis) and/or the later stages (eg, the triggering of coronary events).11 Noninvasive methods for measuring subclinical atherosclerosis, such as the assessment of carotid intima-media thickness (IMT) by ultrasonography,12 can be useful for determining whether these negative emotions contribute to the earlier stages. To date, however, few studies have investigated the potential links between depression, anxiety, and hostility/anger and subclinical atherosclerotic progression,13-17 and none has examined more than 1 of these negative emotions at the same time. Accordingly, we simultaneously evaluated the relative importance of depressive symptoms, anxiety symptoms, and hostility/anger in predicting 3-year change in carotid IMT among healthy, older men and women.
Participants were healthy, older adults who volunteered to participate in the Pittsburgh Healthy Heart Project, an ongoing prospective cohort study examining biobehavioral factors as predictors of subclinical atherosclerosis. This study received the approval of the institutional review board at the University of Pittsburgh. Participants provided written informed consent and were paid $450. Recruitment strategies included mass mailings, media advertisements, and posters placed throughout the community. Major inclusion criteria were age (50-70 years) and menopausal status (women were required to be perimenopausal or postmenopausal, defined as the absence of menses during the past 6 months). Individuals were not enrolled if they reported a history of chronic medical disorders (including symptomatic cardiovascular disease), although persons with diabetes not taking insulin were included. Individuals were also not enrolled if they reported (1) lipid-lowering or antihypertensive medication use during the past year, (2) regular use of medications with autonomic effects (eg, antihistamines/decongestants or dietary aids), or (3) excessive alcohol consumption (≥5 drinks, >3 times per week). In addition, persons with systolic blood pressure greater than 180 mm Hg or diastolic blood pressure greater than 110 mm Hg were not enrolled. Of the 464 enrolled adults, 360 completed baseline and follow-up assessments of carotid IMT. We excluded those who underwent carotid endarterectomy during the follow-up interval (n = 1) and those who had missing data for the variables listed in Table 1 (n = 35), leaving a final sample of 324 adults.
During the baseline phase of the Pittsburgh Healthy Heart Project, participants attended 11 visits during a 5-month period in the following order: a medical screen, 3 visits for ambulatory monitoring training and questionnaire assessments, 1 visit for cardiovascular reactivity testing, 2 visits for ultrasound assessments, and 4 visits for additional ambulatory monitoring training and questionnaire assessments (Figure 1). At 6, 18, and 30 months after baseline, participants were interviewed by telephone to assess changes in their medical history and medication use. Approximately 3 years after baseline, participants attended 2 follow-up visits, during which the ultrasound assessments were repeated and a medical update was completed.
Data regarding age, sex (0, male; 1, female), race/ethnicity (0, white; 1, nonwhite), and education level (1, high school or less; 2, technical school or some college; 3, bachelor's degree; 4, master's degree or higher) were obtained from a questionnaire administered at the medical screen (Table 1). Participants were asked to choose from the following race/ethnicity categories: white, black, Asian, Hispanic, and other. Because only 6 individuals selected the Asian, Hispanic, or other categories, race/ethnicity was coded as a binary variable.
Cardiovascular Risk Factors
Participants underwent a blood pressure assessment, anthropometric measurements, and a blood draw at the medical screen. They were instructed to abstain from food and caffeine for 12 hours before this visit. Following American Heart Association guidelines,18 3 blood pressure readings were taken at 2-minute intervals with a standard mercury sphygmomanometer. Systolic and diastolic blood pressure were computed as the mean of the last 2 readings. Body mass index was calculated as weight in kilograms divided by the square of height in meters. Standard assays were performed to determine levels of serum total cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein cholesterol, and triglycerides. Low-density lipoprotein cholesterol level was calculated by the Friedewald equation.19 Fasting serum glucose level was measured by standard colorimetry, and fasting serum insulin level was measured by radioimmunoassay. These cardiovascular risk factors were reassessed by the same methods at the follow-up visits. Three-year change in systolic and diastolic blood pressure, body mass index, low-density lipoprotein cholesterol, high-density lipoprotein cholesterol, triglycerides, fasting glucose, and fasting insulin was computed by subtracting the baseline level from the follow-up level.
During the medical screen, participants also completed the Paffenbarger Physical Activity Questionnaire20 and questionnaires assessing tobacco and alcohol use. To obtain an estimate of physical activity level, the number of blocks walked and stairs climbed per day reported on the Paffenbarger Physical Activity Questionnaire were first converted to kilocalories per week and then summed (sports, leisure, and recreational activities were not included). Current tobacco use was coded as a binary variable (0, no; 1, yes). Daily alcohol intake in grams per day was computed by the quantity-frequency method.21
Table 1 shows descriptive statistics for the cardiovascular risk factors. Because the distributions for baseline triglycerides, fasting glucose, fasting insulin, and daily alcohol intake were positively skewed (skewness, 1.86, 4.12, 1.87, and 2.67, respectively), these variables were log transformed (skewness, 0.14, 2.48, −0.14, and 0.48, respectively).
Medical Conditions and Medication Use
Participants were asked about their medical history and medication use at the medical screen; at 6-, 18-, and 30-month telephone interviews; and at the follow-up visits. Using these data, we identified (1) participants who reported a history of diabetes or were uncertain of their diabetes status at baseline (n = 7) and (2) participants who received a cardiovascular diagnosis (eg, myocardial infarction) or underwent a cardiovascular procedure (eg, bypass surgery) during the follow-up interval (n = 14). These data were also used to create 7 medication use variables (0, no; 1, yes; see Table 1).
Participants completed self-report questionnaires on a computer to assess their experience and expression of various negative emotions. At the third baseline visit, participants were administered the Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II),22 a widely used instrument that measures the severity of depressive symptoms, and the Beck Anxiety Inventory,23 a questionnaire that assesses the severity of anxiety symptoms. On the BDI-II, participants were asked to rate the severity of their depressive symptoms during the past week instead of the past 2 weeks. Participants completed the Cook-Medley Hostility Scale24 at the fourth baseline visit. The Cook-Medley Hostility Scale measures several components of hostility, including cynicism, hostile attributions, hostile affect, aggressive responding, and social avoidance. Because 1 item of the Cook-Medley Hostility Scale was accidentally omitted, the value for this item was imputed by taking the mean of the other 49 items. During the ninth baseline visit, participants were administered the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory,6 which consists of 5 subscales assessing the current experience of anger (state anger), the general tendency to experience anger (trait anger), the frequency with which anger is suppressed (anger-in) and expressed outwardly (anger-out), and the degree to which attempts are made to control the expression of anger (anger control). Because most participants (87.3%) reported no state anger and because the state anger subscale was not designed to be stable over time, it was not examined as a predictor of IMT change. The negative emotion measures that we used all have good psychometric properties, including moderate to high internal consistency, test-retest reliability, and construct validity.22,25-27
Table 1 displays descriptive statistics for the negative emotion measures. Because the distributions for scores on the BDI-II, Beck Anxiety Inventory, and trait anger subscale of the State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory were positively skewed (skewness, 1.30, 1.96, and 1.21, respectively), these variables were log transformed (skewness, −0.12, −0.26, and 0.39, respectively). Correlations between the negative emotion measures were generally in the small to moderate range (Table 2), indicating that these measures shared a modest yet significant amount of variance. The observed correlations were slightly smaller than those previously reported,2-6 perhaps because the variability of some measures was less in our sample than in the comparison samples.
Carotid IMT is considered to be a noninvasive measure of subclinical atherosclerosis, as it is positively associated with the extent of coronary atherosclerosis and is an independent predictor of future coronary events.12 Approximately 2 months after the medical screen, participants underwent baseline assessments of carotid IMT. B-mode ultrasound scanners (Toshiba SSA-270A and SSA-140A; Toshiba American Medical Systems, Tustin, Calif) were used to obtain digitized images of 6 segments of the carotid arteries: the distal 1 cm of the common carotid artery, the carotid bulb, and the proximal 1 cm of the internal carotid artery from both the left and right sides. An average of 3 years after the baseline assessment, carotid IMT was reassessed by the same methods. To avoid problems with reader drift over time, baseline and follow-up images were scored concurrently.
An automated edge detection system (AMS; Goteborg University, Gothenburg, Sweden)28 was used to measure IMT, defined as the mean distance between the intima-lumen interface and the media-adventitial interface of the far wall. Baseline IMT (mean [SD], 0.761 [0.149] mm) and follow-up IMT (mean [SD], 0.851 [0.217] mm) were computed by averaging the IMT measures for the 6 carotid artery segments obtained during the baseline and follow-up assessments, respectively. Three-year change in IMT (mean [SD], 0.091 [0.141] mm) was calculated as mean follow-up IMT minus mean baseline IMT. Annualized carotid IMT progression rates observed in this study (all segments, 0.030 mm; common carotid, 0.012 mm; carotid bulb, 0.039 mm; internal carotid, 0.040 mm) were comparable, although slightly higher, than those found in past investigations,29,30 possibly because our sample was older than previous samples.
To evaluate the relative importance of various negative emotions in predicting 3-year change in carotid IMT, multiple regression analyses were performed. Mean baseline IMT was entered into each model in step 1. In step 2, control variables that were independent predictors of IMT change were entered into each model. Potential control variables were the variables listed in Table 1, as well as the first-order interactions for the demographic factors. To identify factors that were independent predictors of IMT change, each potential control variable was first entered into a separate model containing mean baseline IMT only (see Table 1 for partial correlations). Potential control variables that were significant predictors (P<.05) were then simultaneously entered into the same model. Variables that remained significant or marginally significant (P<.10) were retained in the model; variables that became nonsignificant (P≥.10) were removed. In step 3, scores on the BDI-II, Beck Anxiety Inventory, Cook-Medley Hostility Scale, and State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory subscales of trait anger, anger-in, anger-out, and anger control were first entered into separate models containing mean baseline IMT and the selected control variables. Because each negative emotion measure was examined in a separate model, a Bonferroni correction was applied to the tests of their regression coefficients to maintain a familywise type I error rate of 5% or less. All of the negative emotion measures were then simultaneously entered into the same model.
To ensure that diabetes did not account for any observed negative emotion–IMT change relationships, analyses were repeated after excluding participants who reported a history of diabetes or were uncertain of their diabetes status at baseline (n = 7). To rule out the possibility that any observed relationships were driven primarily by individuals who developed a cardiovascular disease, analyses were also repeated after excluding participants who received a cardiovascular diagnosis or underwent a cardiovascular procedure during follow-up (n = 14). In addition, interactions (cross-product terms) between each negative emotion measure and sex were tested to determine whether associations between these measures and IMT change varied between men and women.
Standard diagnostic procedures were performed for all analyses to assess for the presence of outliers, influential cases, nonnormally distributed errors, nonconstant error variance, nonlinearity, and collinearity.31 No violations of assumptions were detected, though a small number of outliers (≤6 cases in each model) were identified. Because none of the outliers was also an influential case, they were not excluded from the analyses.
As shown in Table 3, regression analyses indicated that mean baseline IMT was positively related to 3-year change in carotid IMT and accounted for 1.5% of the variance (step 1). Of the potential control variables, only the age × sex interaction, baseline daily alcohol intake, and selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (SSRI) use during follow-up were independent predictors of IMT change. Thus, only these variables (and the main effects of age and sex) were included in the models (step 2). Follow-up analysis of the age × sex interaction showed that age was positively related to IMT change among men (β = .164, P = .04) but was negatively related among women (β = −.187, P = .02). In addition, higher daily alcohol intake at baseline was associated with greater change in IMT. Conversely, SSRI use during follow-up was associated with reduced IMT change, as mean 3-year change in carotid IMT was −0.009 mm for those who used an SSRI vs 0.095 mm for those who did not. Together, the selected control variables explained an additional 8.8% of the variance.
Separate regression analyses for each negative emotion measure indicated that BDI-II score was positively associated with 3-year change in carotid IMT, explaining 2.6% of the variance beyond that accounted for by baseline IMT and the selected control variables (step 3). In contrast, Beck Anxiety Inventory score, Cook-Medley Hostility Scale score, and State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory subscale scores of trait anger, anger-in, anger-out, and anger control were each unrelated to IMT change. When all 7 negative emotion measures were simultaneously entered into the same model, BDI-II score remained a significant predictor (β = .206, P = .001). The BDI-II score also remained a significant predictor after excluding participants who reported a history of diabetes or were uncertain of their diabetes status at baseline (β = .154, P = .005) and after excluding those who received a cardiovascular diagnosis or underwent a cardiovascular procedure during follow-up (β = .166, P = .002). No significant interactions between the negative emotion measures and sex were detected (all P>.15). To further illustrate the observed relationship, participants were classified into tertiles on the basis of their BDI-II score. As can be seen in Figure 2, mean 3-year change in carotid IMT (adjusted for baseline IMT and the selected control variables) was 0.061 mm for participants in the lower tertile, 0.094 mm for those in the middle tertile, and 0.116 mm for those in the upper tertile (P for linear trend =.003).
To explore whether particular clusters of depressive symptoms were more strongly associated with 3-year change in carotid IMT than were others, post hoc analyses were performed. Two BDI-II subscale scores were computed on the basis of the results of Dozois et al32: a cognitive-affective score (sum of items 1-3, 5-9, 13, and 14) and a somatic-vegetative score (sum of items 4, 10-12, and 15-21). The cognitive-affective subscale (mean score [SD], 1.1 [1.7]) consisted of items assessing the cognitive and emotional symptoms of depression (eg, sadness, pessimism, and indecisiveness), whereas the somatic-vegetative subscale (mean score [SD], 2.9 [2.8]) primarily included items assessing the physical symptoms (eg, anhedonia, fatigue, and sleep/appetite disturbance). Both subscale scores were log transformed to reduced positive skewness. Separate regression analyses for each subscale score indicated that the somatic-vegetative score (β = .165, P = .002), but not the cognitive-affective score (β = .061, P = .26), was positively associated with IMT change in the presence of baseline IMT and the selected control variables. The somatic-vegetative score explained approximately the same amount of variance as did the BDI-II total score (ΔR2 = 0.027). In addition, the somatic-vegetative score remained a significant predictor when both BDI-II subscales and the other 6 negative emotion measures were simultaneously entered into the same model (β = .184, P = .003), after excluding participants who reported a history of diabetes or were uncertain of their diabetes status (β = .153, P = .005), and after excluding those who received a cardiovascular diagnosis or underwent a cardiovascular procedure during follow-up (β = .165, P = .003). The interactions between the BDI-II subscales and sex were nonsignificant (both P>.50).
The present study simultaneously examined overlapping negative emotions as predictors of subclinical atherosclerotic progression among healthy, older adults. We found that mild to moderate depressive symptoms, as assessed by the BDI-II, were associated with greater 3-year change in carotid IMT. In contrast, measures of anxiety symptoms, hostility, anger experience, and anger expression were each unrelated to IMT change. Post hoc analyses showed that the BDI-II somatic-vegetative subscale score, but not the cognitive-affective subscale score, was predictive of IMT change, which demonstrates that this symptom cluster was responsible for the association with the BDI-II total score. The observed relationships were not accounted for by potentially confounding factors (baseline IMT, demographic factors, cardiovascular risk factors, medication use, and medical conditions) and remained significant after adjustment for other correlated negative emotions. Taken together, our results indicate that depression, but perhaps not anxiety and hostility/anger, may be involved in the initiation and/or progression of atherosclerosis. More specifically, our findings suggest that the somatic-vegetative features of depression that are not shared with other negative emotions may play an important role in the earlier stages of CAD development.
Although this study is the first, to our knowledge, to report an association between depressive symptoms and IMT change, our results are consistent with the findings of past investigations examining the effect of depression-related factors on subclinical atherosclerotic progression.13,14,16 Interestingly, each of those studies found that a cognitive-affective aspect of depression (ie, discontent, hopelessness, and pessimism) predicted IMT change. Thus, it could be argued that those previous findings are in agreement with ours on a broad construct level but contradict ours on a specific component level. However, because the somatic-vegetative aspects of depression were not examined in those studies, it is not possible to determine whether the observed relationships were specific to the cognitive-affective aspects. Our results also are in line with those of studies reporting positive associations between depressive disorders or symptoms and measures of subclinical atherosclerosis obtained at one point in time.33-37 Finally, the present findings contribute to the growing body of evidence supporting a link between depression and clinical CAD1 and suggest that depression may predict future coronary events, in part because of its involvement in early atherosclerotic processes. It should be noted that, although the results of Kubzansky and colleagues9 appear to be at odds with ours, the absence of a significant link between the unique aspects of depression and incident CAD in their study may have resulted from the limited scope and restricted variability of their depression measure.
Several plausible mechanisms could underlie the observed relationship between depressive symptoms and IMT change. For instance, depression has been associated with pathophysiologic changes thought to be involved in atherosclerosis, including autonomic nervous system dysfunction,38 hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis dysregulation,39 enhanced inflammatory processes,40 and altered platelet function.41 It is unlikely, however, that traditional cardiovascular risk factors mediate the observed relationship, as it persisted after these factors were taken into account. The observed relationship might also be explained by a third factor that could cause both increased depressive symptoms at baseline and greater IMT progression. One such factor is inflammation stemming from subclinical atherosclerosis, given that proinflammatory cytokines can produce behavioral changes similar to the symptoms of depression.42 Another possible third factor is subclinical cerebrovascular disease, which has been hypothesized to be involved in the etiology of late-onset depression.43 Neither of these potential third factors, however, appears to be a likely explanation for our findings. Because our analyses were adjusted for baseline IMT (which can be considered a general marker of systemic atherosclerosis given that it predicts coronary events, stroke, and peripheral vascular disease44-50), it is likely that we adequately controlled for the potential influence of subclinical cardiovascular disease on baseline depressive symptoms.
It is unclear why the somatic-vegetative symptoms of depression predicted subclinical atherosclerotic progression, whereas the cognitive-affective symptoms did not. One possibility is that there may be stronger links between the somatic-vegetative symptoms and the mechanisms underlying the depression-CAD relationship. Another possibility is that older individuals, such as those in our sample, may be more likely to report the somatic-vegetative symptoms. Recent evidence suggests that this may indeed be the case. Kim and colleagues51 found that depressed patients 60 years or older endorsed more somatic symptoms and fewer cognitive symptoms on the BDI than did depressed patients younger than 60 years. Our data also support this idea, as the cognitive-affective subscale had a smaller mean and standard deviation than did the somatic-vegetative subscale. Thus, the cognitive-affective score may not have predicted IMT change because of the restricted variability of this subscale.
Our findings pertaining to the influence of anxiety and hostility/anger on subclinical atherosclerotic progression contrast with existing evidence. Paterniti and colleagues17 found that sustained anxiety predicted 4-year IMT progression, and Julkunen et al15 reported that cynical distrust (a component of hostility) and anger suppression were both positively associated with 2-year IMT progression. In addition, positive associations have been reported between hostility/anger and measures of subclinical atherosclerosis obtained at one point in time.52-56 Potential reasons for these discrepant findings include differences in the negative emotion measures used (eg, see Paterniti et al17) and the characteristics of the participants studied (eg, see Julkunen et al15). Furthermore, because neither depressive disorders nor symptoms were assessed in these previous studies, it is possible that measures of anxiety and hostility/anger may have been acting as markers for depression in these instances. Even though our findings suggest that anxiety and hostility/anger may not be involved in early atherosclerotic processes, they are not necessarily inconsistent with the evidence linking these factors with future CAD,1,8,9 as these negative emotions could play a role in later stages of CAD development.57
Although this study makes important contributions to the literature, a few limitations should be noted. First, because of an oversight while constructing the computerized version of the BDI-II, participants were asked to rate the severity of their depressive symptoms during the past week (the time frame for the original BDI) instead of during the past 2 weeks (the time frame for the BDI-II). This irregularity creates some minor interpretation problems, but its most important influence is probably the underestimation of the true BDI-II score, as individuals tend to score lower on the original BDI than on the BDI-II.32 This, in turn, could have weakened the relationship between depressive symptoms and IMT change by reducing variability in the BDI-II scores. Second, a number of traditional cardiovascular risk factors were not associated with IMT change in the present sample. Significant relationships, however, were detected between traditional risk factors (ie, systolic and diastolic blood pressure) and carotid IMT in cross-sectional analyses.58 In addition, we observed an inverse relationship between SSRI use during follow-up and IMT change, which is consistent with recent evidence that treatment with SSRIs may have beneficial cardiovascular effects.59,60 Several factors may have limited our ability to detect relationships between traditional risk factors and IMT change; these include the overall health of the sample, the relatively brief follow-up period, and the general stability of these risk factors over time. Moreover, it has been found that longitudinal associations between traditional risk factors and carotid IMT are generally not as strong as cross-sectional associations.29 Third, because this study included only healthy, older individuals, our findings may not be generalizable to younger populations or to older populations in which chronic medical disorders are prevalent. Therefore, it is necessary that our results be replicated in population-based studies with less stringent inclusion criteria.
In sum, our findings indicate that the somatic-vegetative features of depression may play an important role in the initiation and/or progression of atherosclerosis, although anxiety and hostility/anger may not. Our results also highlight a key advantage of simultaneously examining overlapping negative emotions: this approach will help us to identify the deleterious aspects of these multidimensional factors. Identifying these components, in turn, may provide insights into the mechanisms underlying the negative emotion−CAD relationships and may facilitate the development of focused interventions designed to reduce the CAD risk of individuals prone to experience negative emotions.
Correspondence: Jesse C. Stewart, PhD, Department of Psychology, Indiana University–Purdue University Indianapolis, 402 N. Blackford St, Room LD 100E, Indianapolis, IN 46202 (jstew@iupui.edu).
Submitted for Publication: February 14, 2006; final revision received July 14, 2006; accepted July 14, 2006.
Author Contributions: Drs Stewart and Kamarck had full access to all of the data in this study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This research was supported by grant HL56346 from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute, training grant HL07560 from the National Institutes of Health, and grants HL076852 and HL076858 from the Pittsburgh Mind-Body Center.
Previous Presentation: This study was presented in part at the 63rd annual meeting of the American Psychosomatic Society; March 4, 2005; Vancouver, British Columbia.
Acknowledgment: We thank the entire project staff of the Pittsburgh Healthy Heart Project for their assistance with data collection.
1.Suls
JBunde
J Anger, anxiety, and depression as risk factors for cardiovascular disease: the problems and implications of overlapping affective dispositions.
Psychol Bull 2005;131260- 300
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Clark
LAWatson
D Tripartite model of anxiety and depression: psychometric evidence and taxonomic implications.
J Abnorm Psychol 1991;100316- 336
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Friedman
HSBooth-Kewley
S Personality, type A behavior, and coronary heart disease: the role of emotional expression.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1987;53783- 792
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Raynor
DAPogue-Geile
MFKamarck
TWMcCaffery
JMManuck
SB Covariation of psychosocial characteristics associated with cardiovascular disease: genetic and environmental influences.
Psychosom Med 2002;64191- 203
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Smith
TWFrohm
KD What's so unhealthy about hostility? construct validity and psychosocial correlates of the Cook and Medley Ho scale.
Health Psychol 1985;4503- 520
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Spielberger
CD State-Trait Anger Expression Inventory Professional Manual. Odessa, Fla Psychological Assessment Resources1988;
7.Watson
DClark
LA Negative affectivity: the disposition to experience aversive emotional states.
Psychol Bull 1984;96465- 490
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Chang
PPFord
DEMeoni
LAWang
NYKlag
MJ Anger in young men and subsequent premature cardiovascular disease: the precursors study.
Arch Intern Med 2002;162901- 906
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Kubzansky
LDCole
SRKawachi
IVokonas
PSparrow
D Shared and unique contributions of anger, anxiety, and depression to coronary heart disease: a prospective study in the normative aging study.
Ann Behav Med 2006;3121- 29
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Falk
EFuster
V Atherogenesis and its determinants. Fuster
VAlexander
RWO’Rourke
RAeds Hurst's The Heart. 10th ed New York, NY McGraw-Hill2001;1065- 1093
11.Kop
WJ Chronic and acute psychological risk factors for clinical manifestations of coronary artery disease.
Psychosom Med 1999;61476- 487
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Mancini
GBDahlof
BDiez
J Surrogate markers for cardiovascular disease: structural markers.
Circulation 2004;109
((suppl 1))
IV22- IV30
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Agewall
SWikstrand
JDahlof
CFagerberg
B Negative feelings (discontent) predict progress of intima-media thickness of the common carotid artery in treated hypertensive men at high cardiovascular risk.
Am J Hypertens 1996;9545- 550
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Everson
SAKaplan
GAGoldberg
DESalonen
RSalonen
JT Hopelessness and 4-year progression of carotid atherosclerosis: the Kuopio Ischemic Heart Disease Risk Factor Study.
Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 1997;171490- 1495
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Julkunen
JSalonen
RKaplan
GAChesney
MASalonen
JT Hostility and the progression of carotid atherosclerosis.
Psychosom Med 1994;56519- 525
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Matthews
KARaikkonen
KSutton-Tyrrell
KKuller
LH Optimistic attitudes protect against progression of carotid atherosclerosis in healthy middle-aged women.
Psychosom Med 2004;66640- 644
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Paterniti
SZureik
MDucimetiere
PTouboul
PJFeve
JMAlperovitch
A Sustained anxiety and 4-year progression of carotid atherosclerosis.
Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 2001;21136- 141
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Perloff
DGrim
CFlack
JFrohlich
EDHill
MMcDonald
MMorgenstern
BZ Human blood pressure determination by sphygmomanometry.
Circulation 1993;882460- 2470
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Friedewald
WTLevy
RIFredrickson
DS Estimation of the concentration of low-density lipoprotein cholesterol in plasma, without use of the preparative ultracentrifuge.
Clin Chem 1972;18499- 502
PubMedGoogle Scholar 20.Paffenbarger
RSWing
ALHyde
RT Physical activity as an index of heart attack risk in college alumni.
Am J Epidemiol 1978;108161- 175
PubMedGoogle Scholar 21.Garg
RWagener
DKMadans
JH Alcohol consumption and risk of ischemic heart disease in women.
Arch Intern Med 1993;1531211- 1216
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Beck
ATSteer
RABrown
GK Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory. 2nd ed San Antonio, Tex Psychological Corp1996;
23.Beck
ATSteer
RA Manual for the Beck Anxiety Inventory. San Antonio, Tex Psychological Corp1990;
24.Cook
WWMedley
DM Proposed hostility and pharisaic-virtue scales for the MMPI.
J Appl Psychol 1954;38414- 418
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Bishop
GDQuah
S-H Reliability and validity of measures of anger/hostility in Singapore: Cook & Medley Ho Scale, STAXI and Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory.
Pers Individ Dif 1998;24867- 878
Google ScholarCrossref 26.Eckhardt
CNorlander
BDeffenbacher
J The assessment of anger and hostility: a critical review.
Aggress Violent Behav 2004;917- 43
Google ScholarCrossref 27.Steer
RABeck
AT Beck Anxiety Inventory. Zalaquett
CPWood
RJeds
Evaluating Stress: A Book of Resources. Lanham, Md Scarecrow Press Inc1997;23- 40
Google Scholar 28.Wendelhag
ILiang
QGustavsson
TWikstrand
J A new automated computerized analyzing system simplifies readings and reduces the variability in ultrasound measurement of intima-media thickness.
Stroke 1997;282195- 2200
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Chambless
LEFolsom
ARDavis
VSharrett
RHeiss
GSorlie
PSzklo
MHoward
GEvans
GW Risk factors for progression of common carotid atherosclerosis: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Study, 1987-1998.
Am J Epidemiol 2002;15538- 47
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Mackinnon
ADJerrard-Dunne
PSitzer
MBuehler
Avon Kegler
SMarkus
HS Rates and determinants of site-specific progression of carotid artery intima-media thickness: the carotid atherosclerosis progression study.
Stroke 2004;352150- 2154
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Fox
J Regression Diagnostics. Newbury Park Calif Sage1991;
32.Dozois
DJADobson
KSAhnberg
JL A psychometric evaluation of the Beck Depression Inventory-II.
Psychol Assess 1998;1083- 89
Google ScholarCrossref 33.Agatisa
PKMatthews
KABromberger
JTEdmundowicz
DChang
YFSutton-Tyrrell
K Coronary and aortic calcification in women with a history of major depression.
Arch Intern Med 2005;1651229- 1236
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Elovainio
MKeltikangas-Jarvinen
LKivimaki
MPulkki
LPuttonen
SHeponiemi
TJuonala
MViikari
JSRaitakari
OT Depressive symptoms and carotid artery intima-media thickness in young adults: the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study.
Psychosom Med 2005;67561- 567
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Haas
DCDavidson
KWSchwartz
DJRieckmann
NRoman
MJPickering
TGGerin
WSchwartz
JE Depressive symptoms are independently predictive of carotid atherosclerosis.
Am J Cardiol 2005;95547- 550
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Jones
DJBromberger
JTSutton-Tyrrell
KMatthews
KA Lifetime history of depression and carotid atherosclerosis in middle-aged women.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;60153- 160
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Tiemeier
Hvan Dijck
WHofman
AWitteman
JCStijnen
TBreteler
MM Relationship between atherosclerosis and late-life depression: the Rotterdam Study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2004;61369- 376
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Carney
RMFreedland
KEVeith
RC Depression, the autonomic nervous system, and coronary heart disease.
Psychosom Med 2005;67
((suppl 1))
S29- S33
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Grippo
AJJohnson
AK Biological mechanisms in the relationship between depression and heart disease.
Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2002;26941- 962
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Kop
WJGottdiener
JS The role of immune system parameters in the relationship between depression and coronary artery disease.
Psychosom Med 2005;67
((suppl 1))
S37- S41
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Bruce
ECMusselman
D Depression, alterations in platelet function, and ischemic heart disease.
Psychosom Med 2005;67
((suppl 1))
S34- S36
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Alexopoulos
GSMeyers
BSYoung
RCCampbell
SSilbersweig
DCharlson
M The “vascular depression” hypothesis.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54915- 922
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Bots
MLHoes
AWKoudstaal
PJHofman
AGrobbee
DE Common carotid intima-media thickness and risk of stroke and myocardial infarction: the Rotterdam Study.
Circulation 1997;961432- 1437
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Bots
MLHofman
AGrobbee
DE Common carotid intima-media thickness and lower extremity arterial atherosclerosis: the Rotterdam Study.
Arterioscler Thromb 1994;141885- 1891
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Burke
GLEvans
GWRiley
WASharrett
ARHoward
GBarnes
RWRosamond
WCrow
RSRautaharju
PMHeiss
G Arterial wall thickness is associated with prevalent cardiovascular disease in middle-aged adults: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study.
Stroke 1995;26386- 391
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Chambless
LEHeiss
GFolsom
ARRosamond
WSzklo
MSharrett
ARClegg
LX Association of coronary heart disease incidence with carotid arterial wall thickness and major risk factors: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) Study, 1987-1993.
Am J Epidemiol 1997;146483- 494
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Chambless
LEFolsom
ARClegg
LXSharrett
ARShahar
ENieto
FJRosamond
WDEvans
G Carotid wall thickness is predictive of incident clinical stroke: the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities (ARIC) study.
Am J Epidemiol 2000;151478- 487
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Hodis
HNMack
WJLaBree
LSelzer
RHLiu
CRLiu
CHAzen
SP The role of carotid arterial intima-media thickness in predicting clinical coronary events.
Ann Intern Med 1998;128262- 269
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.O’Leary
DHPolak
JFKronmal
RAManolio
TABurke
GLWolfson
SK
JrCardiovascular Health Study Collaborative Research Group, Carotid-artery intima and media thickness as a risk factor for myocardial infarction and stroke in older adults.
N Engl J Med 1999;34014- 22
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Kim
YPilkonis
PAFrank
EThase
MEReynolds
CF Differential functioning of the Beck Depression Inventory in late-life patients: use of item response theory.
Psychol Aging 2002;17379- 391
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Bleil
MEMcCaffery
JMMuldoon
MFSutton-Tyrrell
KManuck
SB Anger-related personality traits and carotid artery atherosclerosis in untreated hypertensive men.
Psychosom Med 2004;66633- 639
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Iribarren
CSidney
SBild
DELiu
KMarkovitz
JHRoseman
JMMatthews
K Association of hostility with coronary artery calcification in young adults: the CARDIA study.
JAMA 2000;2832546- 2551
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Knox
SSAdelman
AEllison
RCArnett
DKSiegmund
KWeidner
GProvince
MA Hostility, social support, and carotid artery atherosclerosis in the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute Family Heart Study.
Am J Cardiol 2000;861086- 1089
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Koh
KBChoe
KOAn
SK Anger and coronary calcification in individuals with and without risk factors of coronary artery disease.
Yonsei Med J 2003;44793- 799
PubMedGoogle Scholar 56.Matthews
KAOwens
JFKuller
LHSutton-Tyrrell
KJansen-McWilliams
L Are hostility and anxiety associated with carotid atherosclerosis in healthy postmenopausal women?
Psychosom Med 1998;60633- 638
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Mittleman
MAMaclure
MSherwood
JBMulry
RPTofler
GHJacobs
SCFriedman
RBenson
HMuller
JEDeterminants of Myocardial Infarction Onset Study Investigators, Triggering of acute myocardial infarction onset by episodes of anger.
Circulation 1995;921720- 1725
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Kamarck
TWMuldoon
MFShiffman
SSutton-Tyrrell
KGwaltney
CJanicki
DL Experiences of demand and control in daily life as correlates of subclinical carotid atherosclerosis in a healthy older sample.
Health Psychol 2004;2324- 32
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Glassman
AHO’Connor
CMCaliff
RMSwedberg
KSchwartz
PBigger
JT
JrKrishnan
KRvan Zyl
LTSwenson
JRFinkel
MSLandau
CShapiro
PAPepine
CJMardekian
JHarrison
WMBarton
DMclvor
MSertraline Antidepressant Heart Attack Randomized Trial (SADHEART) Group, Sertraline treatment of major depression in patients with acute MI or unstable angina.
JAMA 2002;288701- 709
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Taylor
CBYoungblood
MECatellier
DVeith
RCCarney
RMBurg
MMKaufmann
PGShuster
JMellman
TBlumenthal
JAKrishnan
RJaffe
ASENRICHD Investigators, Effects of antidepressant medication on morbidity and mortality in depressed patients after myocardial infarction.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62792- 798
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref