Context
Reduction in adult neurogenesis has been proposed as a mechanism for onset of depression. Semaphorins and their coreceptors, plexins, have been implicated in nervous system development and in adult neurogenesis. A recent genomewide association study of schizophrenia identified a variant of the gene encoding plexin A2 (PLXNA2) to be most consistently associated across study samples. Common genetic liabilities have been reported between psychiatric and psychological measures, but few examples exist of common genetic variants.
Objective
To perform a genetic association study between 6 single nucleotide polymorphisms from the PLXNA2 gene (rs3736963, rs2767565, rs752016, rs1327175, rs2478813, and rs716461) and anxiety, depression, neuroticism, and psychological distress.
Design
Extreme discordant and concordant siblings.
Setting
Australia.
Participants
Study participants were selected with respect to extreme neuroticism scores from a population cohort of 18 742 twin individuals and their siblings. The participants and their parents (if blood or buccal samples were available) were genotyped, for a total of 2854 genotyped individuals from 990 families. Of these, 624 individuals with a diagnosis of anxiety or depression from 443 families were used in the association analysis.
Main Outcome Measures
All the participants completed the Composite International Diagnostic Interview, the 23-item Neuroticism scale of the revised Eysenck Personality Questionnaire, and the 10-item Kessler Psychological Distress Scale. Diagnoses of DSM-IV depression and anxiety were determined from the Composite International Diagnostic Interview.
Results
There was evidence of an allelic association between rs2478813 (and other single nucleotide polymorphisms correlated with it) and anxiety, depression, neuroticism, and psychological distress; the association with anxiety is significant after Bonferroni correction for multiple testing (empirical P<.001). The mouse ortholog of PLXNA2 is located in a highly significant linkage region previously reported for anxiety in mice.
Conclusion
PLXNA2 is a candidate for causal variation in anxiety and in other psychiatric disorders through its comorbidity with anxiety.
For the past 4 decades,1,2 most theories of the etiology of major depression have centered on brain alterations in neurochemistry. Recently, a novel theory proposed that the waning and waxing of neurogenesis in the adult hippocampus is causal in the onset of and recovery from episodes of clinical depression.3,4 Pivotal to this theory was the discovery that neurogenesis is not restricted to the developing brain but also occurs in the mature adult brain.5 More recently, it has been shown that lithium (a commonly prescribed mood-stabilizing drug with antidepressant properties) enhances hippocampal neurogenesis6 and, specifically, that neurogenesis is a requirement for the behavioral responses of antidepressants to be affected.7 Therefore, genes that mediate hippocampal neurogenesis are plausible candidates for having risk variants for depression.
Semaphorins are members of a large, highly conserved family of molecular cues that have been implicated in the development of the nervous system, the guidance of axonal projections, axonal fasciculation, dendritic guidance, and neuronal migration.8,9 In addition to their widespread expression during neuronal development, some semaphorins are persistently expressed in the adult nervous system,8 are involved in neuronal apoptosis,10 and have been shown to induce pruning of hippocampal axon branches in the mature brain.11 Plexins are members of the semaphorin receptor family that act with neuropilins in mediating the effects of semaphorins.12 Neuropilins have been found at synapses, suggesting that semaphorins also have a role as synaptic modulators.13 Plexin A2 (PLXNA2), together with neuropilin 1 or 2, forms a functional receptor for class 3 semaphorins,14 one of which, semaphorin 3A, has been found at elevated levels in patients with schizophrenia.15 The functional role of semaphorins and their receptors makes them plausible candidates for variants to have a causal role in a broad range of psychiatric and psychological disorders.
Recently, a genomewide association study of schizophrenia that analyzed more than 25 000 single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) from approximately 14 000 genes reported that, of 62 SNPs found to be associated in the “discovery” case-control set, 1 (accession ID rs752016 at dbSNP [see www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/projects/SNP]) showed consistency of association across replication samples (odds ratio [OR], 1.49; P = .006 in individuals of European descent). Fine mapping identified a region spanning exons 5 to 26 (approximately 60 kilobase [kb]) of the PLXNA2 gene. As yet, it is unknown whether the PLXNA2 variant associated with schizophrenia16 might act during neuronal development, adult neurogenesis, or both. Currently, schizophrenia is viewed as a disorder of neurodevelopment and of synaptic activity regulated by neurotransmitters such as dopamine and serotonin.17 However, the first study to implicate adult neurogenesis in schizophrenia has just been published,18 inviting the hypothesis that common variants may affect adult neurogenesis in a spectrum of psychiatric disorders (although the same study found no evidence of neural stem cell proliferation, which is considered to be the first stage of adult neurogenesis in patients with major depression).
Under the Kraepelin19 categorical disease model (and in DSM-IV and International Statistical Classification of Diseases, 10th Revision), schizophrenia and major depression are considered separate conditions. However, mounting evidence suggests that this clinically based distinction may not provide adequate guidance in attempting to elucidate the etiologic basis of these disorders.20 There are few specific genetic risk factors that have been associated conclusively with both schizophrenia and depression, the most widely studied being the neuropathogenic role of brain-derived neurotrophic factor (reviewed by Angelucci et al21). Neuroticism, a dimension of personality that includes a tendency toward anxiety, depression, and low self-esteem, was found, when rated at age 16 years, to be associated with increased risk of later diagnosis of schizophrenia (OR, 1.9322). Neuroticism can be measured using self-report questionnaires such as the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire (EPQ),23,24 and high EPQ neuroticism scores are associated with greater risk of early-onset depressive and anxiety disorders.25 Neuroticism, depression, and anxiety are considered to have a common genetic basis.26 The selection of participants with high EPQ neuroticism scores provided considerably enhanced odds of a range of related DSM-IV diagnoses, such as depression, social phobia, and generalized anxiety disorder27 (in the study sample used in this article). In their review, Fanous and Kendler28 concluded that, although neuroticism, depression, and schizophrenia may have shared specific genetic risk factors, more conclusive empirical evidence is needed.
Given (1) the comprehensive strategy used by Mah et al,16 which resulted in their proposal of PLXNA2 as a candidate gene for schizophrenia, (2) the possible shared etiology of schizophrenia, depression, and anxiety disorders, and (3) the function of plexins, which fits with the hypothesis that reduction in neurogenesis in the adult brain precipitates the onset of depression, we looked for associations between a set of 6 SNPs in PLXNA2 and measures of neuroticism, psychological distress, depression, and anxiety. In this study, we show evidence of associations between PLXNA2 SNPs and a spectrum of psychiatric and psychological traits, particularly driven by an association with anxiety, which shows a high degree of comorbidity with the other phenotypic measures. This study sample is unique because the same cohort of people, selected as sibling pairs concordant or discordant for extreme neuroticism scores, completed detailed questionnaires generating multiple phenotypic measures for each person, allowing us to follow up the initial association results to identify the phenotypic subtypes that drive the association in this sample.
This study was approved by the Queensland Institute of Medical Research human research ethics committee. Australian twin families recruited from the Australian Twin Registry completed self-report questionnaires between 1980 and 1995, including either the full 90-item EPQ revised (EPQ-R)24 with a 23-item neuroticism scale or a shortened questionnaire (EPQ-Rs) with a 12-item neuroticism scale. The EPQ-R or EPQ-Rs neuroticism scores were available for 18 742 Australian twin individuals and their siblings. Sibling pairs that were either concordant or discordant for extreme EPQ scores (1 sibling in the top or bottom decile, the other sibling in the top or bottom quintile, and excluding monozygotic twin pairs) were recruited to complete a more detailed personality questionnaire. Using these criteria, multiple siblings were selected from some families. Blood (or buccal) samples were obtained where possible from the selected siblings and their parents. Full details of the recruitment procedure for this extreme discordant and concordant study, including response rates and incidence of DSM-IV diagnoses of anxiety- and depression-related disorders and longitudinal stability of neuroticism scores, are given by Kirk et al27 and Birley et al.29
The study participants completed a questionnaire that included a shortened Composite International Diagnostic Interview,30 which provided DSM-IV31 lifetime diagnoses of depression (code 296.2: major depressive disorder, single episode; code 296.3: major depressive disorder, recurrent episode; or code 300.4: dysthymic disorder) and anxiety (code 300.23: social phobia; code 300.02: generalized anxiety disorder; code 300.01: panic with agoraphobia; code 300.21: panic without agoraphobia; code 300.22: agoraphobia without panic; and code 300.03: obsessive-compulsive disorder). Standard clinical significance exclusion criteria32 were applied, which helps to ensure accurate prevalence rates of DSM-IV diagnoses.32 Prevalence rates in the sample compared with the Australian population are discussed by Kirk et al.27 Diagnoses were coded as follows: 2 = affected, 1 = unaffected for all DSM-IV diagnoses, and 0 = not scored or affected for a different DSM-IV diagnosis. The phenotypes used for initial association analysis were DEP (any DSM-IV depression diagnosis), ANX (any DSM-IV anxiety diagnosis), and DEPorANX (any DSM-IV depression or anxiety diagnosis). Individual DSM-IV diagnoses that had more than approximately 100 affected participants were used to follow up on associations detected in the broad diagnosis groups.
The questionnaire completed by study participants also included the full 23-item EPQ-R neuroticism scale and the 10-item Kessler Psychological Distress Scale (K10),33 a measure of current nonspecific psychological distress in the anxiety-depression spectrum. The EPQ-R and K10 scores were transformed to be residuals from regression of the arcsine transformed score34 on age, sex, and age × sex. The distribution of the residuals was nonnormal, reflecting the population distribution of scores and the ascertainment criteria. For this reason and to facilitate comparison with the DSM-IV diagnostic criteria, individuals were considered to be “affected” for the EPQ-R and the K10 if their residual scores were in the top xth percentile of available residuals. Percentiles x = 5, 10, 15, and 20 were considered, and the resulting traits are labeled EPQ-x% and K10-x%.
In total, there were 2063 genotyped study participants from 990 families, with genotypes available on an additional 791 family members. A description of the families with at least 1 affected genotyped individual and with at least 1 parent or 1 sibling genotyped is given in Table 1 for DEPorANX, DEP, ANX, EPQ-20%, and K10-20%. For example, there were 624 genotyped individuals with a diagnosis of anxiety and/or depression from 443 families. Numbers of affected individuals and pedigrees in common between phenotypes are given in Table 2. Given the overlap in definition of affected status between the phenotypic data sets, mutually exclusive phenotypic data subsets were created to follow up on initial association results.
Mah et al16 genotyped 67 SNPs in a 100-kb window around their most associated SNP (rs752016) in their discovery case-control sample and then selected 14 SNPs for genotyping on their case-control replication samples. Of these SNPs, 4 were selected for genotyping in their family-based replication samples (which included an Australian sample genotyped at the laboratory of the Queensland Institute of Medical Research). Two of these 4 SNPs were genotyped in the present sample (rs752016 and rs1327175). For better representation of the 50-kb region (Figure), rs841865 was replaced by 2 SNPs that bounded it (rs3736963 and rs2767565), and, similarly, rs2478813 and rs716461 were genotyped instead of rs249808. Genotyping was performed using primer extension on the MassARRAY system (Sequenom Inc) as described by James et al.35 More than 99% of genotyped samples had genotypes for all 6 SNPs, with error rates less than 0.1%.35
The Genetic Power Calculator36 was used to investigate the power of the sample for a range of genetic model scenarios and assuming a transmission disequilibrium test design. A type I error of 0.05/30 = 0.0017 was chosen, conservatively assuming that we would perform independent tests with 6 SNPs × 5 phenotypes (considering the x% groupings of K10 and EPQ as single phenotypes because their results are interpreted as a set). For example, 300 nuclear families with 1 affected offspring have greater than 80% power to detect a genotyped causal locus of frequency 0.3, assuming a type I error of 0.0017 and a heterozygous genotype relative risk of 1.65 under a genetic model of multiplicative allelic action; 150 nuclear families could detect a causal genotyped locus with a heterozygous genotype relative risk of 2.00 using the same criteria. These calculations provide a baseline indication of power as the present study design includes many families with multiple affected siblings or unaffected siblings, which should result in increased power.37 The software program SIB-PAIR38 was used to check for departures from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium and to estimate the |D′| and r2 measures of linkage disequilibrium from parental or inferred parental genotypes.
Association analysis was undertaken using 2 different methods: the pedigree disequilibrium test (PDT)37 and the transmission disequilibrium score test (TDST).39 The PDT calculates a measure of association within each family and then combines the measures for all families. It is a test for linkage disequilibrium (ie, linkage and association), and it uses information from extended families and from affected and unaffected individuals. In a linear logistic regression framework, the TDST uses information from 1 or more affected offspring from nuclear families only and constructs case and control haplotypes from all parental chromosomes. The test is equivalent to the haplotype-based haplotype relative risk test40 when there is only 1 affected offspring per family and when all genotypes are known. As a within-family association test, the PDT is valid in the presence of population stratification and gains power from using both concordant and discordant siblings and extended families. In contrast, the TDST uses only affected offspring (Clayton39 argued that the amount of information from unaffected offspring in this test would be negligible) and considers only nuclear families but gains power from being an association test across families and should also be robust to population stratification.39 Because only affected offspring were identified for EPQ-x% and K10-x%, only TDST analysis was undertaken for these traits. Single- and 2-marker haplotype tests PDT and TDST were conducted as implemented in the software program UNPHASED41 (pdtphase and tdtphase, respectively) with the options missing, to allow imputation of missing parental genotypes; EM, to allow estimation of uncertain haplotype frequencies; and rare 0.01, to require haplotypes to have a frequency greater than 1%. The option condition was used to investigate the independence of associated markers, and the option sibsex was used to investigate sex-specific associations. Permutation tests were performed when the asymptotic P value was less than .05, using the options permutation 10 000 and, for TDST, robustperm to account for the family structure. The resulting empirical P values account for a nonasymptotic distribution of the test statistic resulting from the structure of the data. The permutation 10 000 option used with the window 2 and individual (TDST only) options provided an empirical P value for the most significant 2-marker haplotype correcting for the multiple testing of all 2-marker haplotypes. All pedigrees and genotypes were included for each analysis even when no individuals were measured or had affected family members so that all available genotypes were available for estimation of haplotype frequencies and determination of missing parental genotypes. The ORs representing the increased frequency of a haplotype in cases vs controls were calculated relative to the haplotype with the highest frequency from the frequencies estimated in TDST.
No SNPs showed evidence of departure from Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium. Minor allele frequencies, |D′|, and r2 measures of linkage disequilibrium show high linkage disequilibrium on the |D′| scale, but, as expected from the differences in allele frequencies, linkage disequilibrium is low on the r2 scale (Table 3).
Association results for individual SNPs and DEPorANX, DEP, ANX, EPQ-x%, and K10-x% are listed in Table 4. Empirical P values (based on permutations for individuals tests) are reported when P<.05. In general, P values from permutation testing were in agreement with the asymptotic P values for PDT. Asymptotic P values for TDST were smaller than the corresponding empirical P values for TDST, presumably because the different family structures in this design cause the distribution of the test statistic to deviate from theoretical expectations. Empirical P values for PDT and TDST were mostly in good agreement to the same order of magnitude, suggesting that most information for association comes from affected offspring and that there is no evidence of population stratification. The following discussion and follow-up analyses consider TDST results only.
All 5 phenotypes (considering the x% groupings of K10 and EPQ as single phenotypes because the results are interpreted as a set) show evidence (P<.05) of association with the SNP rs2478813, and the association with ANX (P<.001) retains significance after Bonferroni correction (P<.002) for the 30 tests undertaken (it is also significant if we consider the x% groupings as individual tests, a total of 66 tests P<.001). The minor allele A of rs2478813 is associated with the affected status of the phenotypes. There is also evidence of associations between the SNPs rs3736963, rs2767565, and rs752016 and DEPorANX, ANX, and DEP, but there is little evidence of association between the genotyped SNPs immediately flanking rs2478813 (rs1327175 and rs716461). Although there may be more than 1 underlying associated variant, or an underlying associated variant that is present on the background of a haplotype of multiple genotyped SNPs, these results can all be explained by a single underlying associated variant that is most highly associated with the SNP rs2478813. First, rs2478813 shows a high |D′| relationship with the other 5 SNPs but a moderate to low r2 relationship (Table 2), which reflects the difference in allele frequencies between the SNPs and the absence (for |D′| = 1 or near absence for high |D′|) of 1 of the 4 possible haplotypes, when considering the haplotypes of pairs of SNPs each including rs2478813. The minor allele A of the SNP rs2478813 is coupled predominantly with the common alleles C and G (Table 2) of the genotyped SNPs flanking rs1327175 and rs716461, respectively, so that the strong association between rs2478813 and the 5 traits does not result in any correlated associations with rs1327175 and rs716461. In contrast, the high |D′| relationship between rs2478813 and the first 3 genotyped SNPs (rs3736963, rs2767565, and rs752016) reflects the coupling of the minor A allele of rs2478813 with the minor alleles of the other SNPs so that the strong association between the phenotypes and rs2478813 is reflected as weaker associations with rs3736963, rs2767565, and rs752016. Second, haplotype analysis of pairs of SNPs showed no association to be more significant than that with rs2478813 (results not shown). Last, conditional SNP association analysis of each of the first 3 SNPs, conditioned on the association with rs2478813, are all nonsignificant, suggesting that the observed SNP single associations all result from the same underlying variant. Further investigation and discussion focuses on the association with rs2478813 only.
The association between rs2478813 and the phenotype DEPorANX (P = .007) is found to be driven mostly by the association with ANX (P<.001), although a tendency toward association is seen for DEP alone (P = .07). The DSM-IV diagnoses of anxiety and depression from Composite International Diagnostic Interview questionnaires are not mutually exclusive (Table 2). Therefore, to provide insight into these results, we considered the mutually exclusive subgroups of DEPandANX (DSM-IV diagnoses for both depression and anxiety), DEPnotANX (any DSM-IV diagnosis for depression but not anxiety), and ANXnotDEP any DSM-IV diagnosis for anxiety but not depression). The numbers of affected individuals and pedigrees for these data sets are listed in Table 2. By reducing the number of affected individuals analyzed, the association between SNP and affectation status must increase to retain the same level of significance. However, evidence of association was found for DEPandANX (P = .004) and ANXnotDEP (P = .009) but not for DEPnotANX (P = .93) (Table 5). Further understanding of the associations can be gained by considering association analysis of the major subdiagnoses that contribute to the ANXandDEP diagnoses (Table 5). All the anxiety diagnoses (generalized anxiety disorder, social phobia, agoraphobia and panic disorder, and obsessive-compulsive disorder) show excessive transmission of allele A to affected offspring (OR, 2.8, 5.4, 2.3, and 1.9, respectively) and some evidence of association with rs2478813 (P = .005, .003, .07, and .13, respectively). In contrast, of the depression diagnoses, single-episode major depressive disorder (code 296.2) shows evidence of association (P = .02) but recurrent major depressive disorder (code 296.3) does not (P>.99). Only 111 individuals are comorbid for code 296.2 and ANX. Separate analysis of code 296.2andANX (OR, 3.0; P = .01) and 296.2notANX (OR, 1.5; P = .30) showed that individuals who are affected for both anxiety and depression are contributing most to the association of code 296.2 with the SNP rs2478813. In contrast, the 97 individuals who are comorbid for 296.3andANX (remembering that diagnoses 296.2 and 296.3 are mutually exclusive) show no significant association with rs2478813 (P = .11), although the minor allele is overtransmitted to affected offspring (OR, 2.1). Differences in age at interview, age at onset of depression, and sex in individuals affected with 296.2andANX vs 296.3andANX provided no further insight into these results. Analysis of the sexes separately for ANX showed no evidence of sex specificity of the association (124 males affected, P = .047; 228 females affected, P<.001; difference, P = .25).
The personality measures of EPQ neuroticism and K10 showed evidence of association with rs2478813. As the x of EPQ-x% and K10-x% increases, 2 conflicting forces affecting the power of association are changed. Phenotypic homogeneity decreases (reflected in the trend toward decreasing ORs) (Table 5) as x increases, but the sample size increase results in lower confidence intervals for the OR (Table 5). As x increases, the proportion of individuals considered affected for the EPQ-x% and the K10-x% who are also affected for ANX decreases (0.71, 0.61, 0.56, and 0.51 for the EPQ and 0.67, 0.57, 0.50, and 0.44 for the K10). An example analysis of K10-15% (the most significantly associated) as K10-15%andANX and K10-15%notANX suggests that it is the anxiety personalities that are driving the associations with the personality measures (P = .002 and P = .54, respectively).
In this study we tested the hypothesis that a gene found to be associated with schizophrenia might also be associated with other psychiatric and psychological conditions. The gene PLXNA2 encodes for plexin 2A, which acts as a receptor for the class 3 semaphorins,14 which as well as showing widespread expression during neuronal development are persistently expressed in the adult nervous system,8 are involved in neuronal apoptosis,10 and have been shown to induce pruning of hippocampal axon branches in the adult brain.11 The present analysis found evidence of an association between rs2478813 (and other SNPs correlated with it) and anxiety, depression, neuroticism, and psychological distress. The association with anxiety can be declared significant after stringent Bonferroni correction for multiple testing. There is no evidence of multiple associated variants or of a 2-SNP haplotype of the genotyped SNPs being more highly associated with any of the phenotypes than rs2478813. The association reported by Mah et al16 was for the SNP rs752016, but it was the major allele that was found to have higher frequency in cases than controls. This SNP is in high |D′| linkage disequilibrium (0.99) and moderate r2 (0.63) with rs2478813, and the minor alleles at both loci tend to be coupled. We found associations for the minor allele of the SNP rs752016 and anxiety and depression, but further investigation suggests that it reflects the same association as with rs2478813, weakened by the incomplete association between the SNPs. Mah et al16 reported an OR ranging from 1.31 to 1.49 for 5 of their discovery and replication samples that showed association with the major allele of the SNP rs752016. Only their Australian replication sample showed association (nonsignificant) with the minor allele. Mah et al16 sequenced exons 10 to 15 in the 40-kb region around the 2 SNPs for 18 affected individuals but found no nonsynonymous SNPs and speculated that a causal variant may be mediated by variations that affect RNA splicing or expression levels. Only further studies will clarify whether the present findings and those of Mah et al16 are true positives and determine whether common or different causal variants underlie these associations.
The initial association analysis phenotypes tested a specific hypothesis. However, the unique nature of the phenotyped study sample is that all participants completed the Composite International Diagnostic Interview, the EPQ, and the K10 so that all individuals were scored for all phenotypes. This allowed us to investigate further the phenotypic subtypes involved in the association. Such analyses, although a true reflection of the information provided by the present study sample, should be seen as hypothesis generating, and interpretation should proceed with caution until the results have been replicated in an independent sample. Association analysis of mutually exclusive diagnostic subgroups suggested that the association of rs2478813 with depression, neuroticism, and psychological distress was being driven by individuals who were comorbid for anxiety. Analysis of the major subclasses of anxiety all showed evidence of association despite their smaller sample sizes (although some comorbidity exists between subtypes). A genetic linkage study42 of anxiety susceptibility found evidence of linkage to 1q (P = .04), a region chosen for study because of its homology to a murine chromosomal region (chromosome 1, 56-106 cM) found to contain quantitative trait loci for anxiety.43 Henderson et al,44 in a genomewide linkage study of behavioral traits in mice, also reported strong evidence (logarithm of odds >20) for a dominant quantitative trait locus on chromosome arm 1q for generalized anxiety in response to relatively safe stimuli as opposed to anxiety resulting from highly anxiogenic stimuli. The powerful design of their study resulted in a tight 95% confidence interval (71-78 cM) for the position of the quantitative trait locus. The mouse ortholog of PLXNA2 is located in this interval (http://www.informatics.jax.org/searches/linkmap_form.shtml), which strengthens the argument that variants of PLXNA2 have a causal role in anxiety disorders.
Semaphorin receptors are plausible candidates for a common etiology of a spectrum of psychiatric and psychological disorders because of their role in the development, maintenance, and apoptosis of the nervous system.8,9 Semaphorins were discovered only 13 years ago, and the more than 20 semaphorin genes that are known to date are highly conserved from invertebrates to humans.9 Understanding of the role of plexins acting together with neuropilins as receptors for semaphorins is also relatively new.12 The first genomewide association study of Parkinson disease was recently published,45 and the most highly associated SNP was in the gene encoding semaphorin 5A. The present results contribute to the growing body of evidence that semaphorins and semaphorin receptors may have an important role in a wide range of neurologic and psychiatric disorders. From association results it is not possible to determine whether the putative functional role of PLXNA2 operates in neurodevelopment or adult neurogenesis. Nonetheless, these results are the first to provide evidence of a specific genetic variant that could contribute to the waning and waxing of adult neurogenesis that has been hypothesized3 to precipitate the onset of and recovery from depression. Suboptimal performance of adult neurogenesis in anxiety disorders has not previously been proposed, but these results suggest that this area merits further investigation.
Correspondence: Naomi R. Wray, PhD, Queensland Institute of Medical Research, Royal Brisbane Hospital, Brisbane QLD 4029, Australia (naomi.wray@qimr.edu.au).
Submitted for Publication: March 9, 2006; final revision received May 11, 2006; accepted June 9, 2006.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: Phenotype collection was funded by grants 971232 and 339450 from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council (Drs Andrews and Martin) and by Gemini Genomics PLC (now defunct). Typing of SNPs in PLXNA2 in Dr James' laboratory was funded by Sequenom Inc.
Acknowledgment: We thank Lorna Peters for her role in preparing the Composite International Diagnostic Interview computer-driven telephone interview and the scoring algorithm; the interviewers and clerical and administrative support staff supervised by Dixie Statham; Scott Gordon and David Smyth for computer support; the laboratory staff, especially Megan Campbell, Anjali Henders, and Leanne McNeil; Peter M. Visscher and Dale R. Nyholt for commenting on the manuscript; and, most of all, the twins and their relatives for their willing participation in the study.
1.Lapin
IPOxenkrug
GF Intensification of the central serotoninergic processes as a possible determinant of the thymoleptic effect.
Lancet 1969;1132- 136
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Schildkraut
JJ The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders: a review of the supporting evidence.
Am J Psychiatry 1965;122509- 522
PubMedGoogle Scholar 3.Jacobs
BLPraag
HGage
FH Adult brain neurogenesis and psychiatry: a novel theory of depression.
Mol Psychiatry 2000;5262- 269
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Kempermann
G Regulation of adult hippocampal neurogenesis: implications for novel theories of major depression.
Bipolar Disord 2002;417- 33
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Eriksson
PSPerfilieva
EBjork-Eriksson
TAlborn
AMNordborg
CPeterson
DAGage
FH Neurogenesis in the adult human hippocampus.
Nat Med 1998;41313- 1317
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Chen
GRajkowska
GDu
FSeraji-Bozorgzad
NManji
HK Enhancement of hippocampal neurogenesis by lithium.
J Neurochem 2000;751729- 1734
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Santarelli
LSaxe
MGross
CSurget
ABattaglia
FDulawa
SWeisstaub
NLee
JDuman
RArancio
OBelzung
CHen
R Requirement of hippocampal neurogenesis for the behavioral effects of antidepressants.
Science 2003;301805- 809
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.He
ZWang
KCKoprivica
VMing
GSong
HJ Knowing how to navigate: mechanisms of semaphorin signaling in the nervous system.
Sci STKE 2002;2002RE1
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.Shirvan
AZiv
IFleminger
GShina
RHe
ZBrudo
IMelamed
EBarzilai
A Semaphorins as mediators of neuronal apoptosis.
J Neurochem 1999;73961- 971
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Bagri
ACheng
HJYaron
APleasure
SJTessier-Lavigne
M Stereotyped pruning of long hippocampal axon branches triggered by retraction inducers of the semaphorin family.
Cell 2003;113285- 299
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Rohm
BOttemeyer
ALohrum
MPuschel
AW Plexin/neuropilin complexes mediate repulsion by the axonal guidance signal semaphorin 3A.
Mech Dev 2000;9395- 104
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Sahay
AKim
CHSepkuty
JPCho
EHuganir
RLGinty
DDKolodkin
AL Secreted semaphorins modulate synaptic transmission in the adult hippocampus.
J Neurosci 2005;253613- 3620
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Tamagnone
LArtigiani
SChen
HHe
ZMing
GISong
HChedotal
AWinberg
MLGoodman
CSPoo
MTessier-Lavigne
MComoglio
PM Plexins are a large family of receptors for transmembrane, secreted, and GPI-anchored semaphorins in vertebrates.
Cell 1999;9971- 80
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Eastwood
SLLaw
AJEverall
IPHarrison
PJ The axonal chemorepellant semaphorin 3A is increased in the cerebellum in schizophrenia and may contribute to its synaptic pathology.
Mol Psychiatry 2003;8148- 155
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Mah
SNelson
MRDelisi
LEReneland
RHMarkward
NJames
MRNyholt
DRHayward
NHandoko
HMowry
BKammerer
SBraun
A Identification of the semaphorin receptor PLXNA2 as a candidate for susceptibility to schizophrenia.
Mol Psychiatry 2006;11471- 478
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Reif
AFritzen
SFinger
MStrobel
ALauer
MSchmitt
ALesch
KP Neural stem cell proliferation is decreased in schizophrenia, but not in depression.
Mol Psychiatry 2006;11514- 522
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Kraepelin
E Clinical Psychiatry. 7th London, England MacMillan & Co Ltd1907;
20.Hafner
HMaurer
KTrendler
Gan der Heiden
WSchmidt
MKonnecke
R Schizophrenia and depression: challenging the paradigm of two separate diseases: a controlled study of schizophrenia, depression and healthy controls.
Schizophr Res 2005;7711- 24
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Angelucci
FBrene
SMathe
AA BDNF in schizophrenia, depression and corresponding animal models.
Mol Psychiatry 2005;10345- 352
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Eysenck
HJEysenck
SBG Manual of the Eysenck Personality Questionnaire. London, England Hodder & Stoughton1975;
24.Eysenck
SBGEysenck
HJBarrett
P A revised version of the psychoticism scale.
Pers Individ Dif 1985;621- 29
Google ScholarCrossref 25.Clark
DABeck
ATBeck
JS Symptom differences in major depression, dysthymia, panic disorder, and generalized anxiety disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 1994;151205- 209
PubMedGoogle Scholar 27.Kirk
KMBirley
AJStatham
DJHaddon
BLake
RIAndrews
JGMartin
NG Anxiety and depression in twin and sib pairs extremely discordant and concordant for neuroticism: prodromus to a linkage study.
Twin Res 2000;3299- 309
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Fanous
AHKendler
KS The genetic relationship of personality to major depression and schizophrenia.
Neurotox Res 2004;643- 50
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Birley
AJGillespie
NAHeath
ACSullivan
PFBoomsma
DIMartin
NG Heritability and nineteen-year stability of long and short EPQ-R Neuroticism scales.
Pers Individ Dif 2006;40737- 747
Google ScholarCrossref 30. Composite International Diagnostic Interview Version 2.1. Geneva, Switzerland World Health Organization1997;
31.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association1994;
32.Andrews
GHenderson
SHall
W Prevalence, comorbidity, disability and service utilisation: overview of the Australian National Mental Health Survey.
Br J Psychiatry 2001;178145- 153
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Kessler
RCMroczek
D Final Version of the Psychological Distress Scale: Technical Note Institute for Social Research. Ann Arbor University of Michigan1994;
34.Jardine
RMartin
NGHenderson
AS Genetic covariation between neuroticism and the symptoms of anxiety and depression.
Genet Epidemiol 1984;189- 107
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.James
MRHayward
NKDumenil
TMontgomery
GWMartin
NGDuffy
DL Epidermal growth factor gene
(EGF) polymorphism and risk of melanocytic neoplasia.
J Invest Dermatol 2004;123760- 762
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Purcell
SCherny
SSSham
PC Genetic Power Calculator: design of linkage and association genetic mapping studies of complex traits.
Bioinformatics 2003;19149- 150
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Martin
ERMonks
SAWarren
LLKaplan
NL A test for linkage and association in general pedigrees: the pedigree disequilibrium test.
Am J Hum Genet 2000;67146- 154
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Clayton
D A generalization of the transmission/disequilibrium test for uncertain-haplotype transmission.
Am J Hum Genet 1999;651170- 1177
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Terwilliger
JDOtt
J A haplotype-based “haplotype relative risk” approach to detecting allelic associations.
Hum Hered 1992;42337- 346
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Smoller
JWRosenbaum
JFBiederman
JSusswein
LSKennedy
JKagan
JSnidman
NLaird
NTsuang
MTFaraone
SVSchwarz
ASlaugenhaupt
SA Genetic association analysis of behavioral inhibition using candidate loci from mouse models.
Am J Med Genet 2001;105226- 235
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Caldarone
BSaavedra
CTartaglia
KWehner
JMDudek
BCFlaherty
L Quantitative trait loci analysis affecting contextual conditioning in mice.
Nat Genet 1997;17335- 337
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Henderson
NDTurri
MGDeFries
JCFlint
J QTL analysis of multiple behavioral measures of anxiety in mice.
Behav Genet 2004;34267- 293
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Maraganore
DMde Andrade
MLesnick
TGStrain
KJFarrer
MJRocca
WAPant
PVFrazer
KACox
DRBallinger
DG High-resolution whole-genome association study of Parkinson disease.
Am J Hum Genet 2005;77685- 693
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref