[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
The prevalence of anorexia nervosa (AN) for the 4 categories of dizygotic twins using narrow criteria: the categories are same-sex female pairs (SS Females), opposite-sex twins with the female member having AN (OS Females), opposite-sex twins with the male member having AN (OS Males), and same-sex male pairs (SS Males). The vertical bars represent the 95% confidence interval for the estimate of the prevalence.

The prevalence of anorexia nervosa (AN) for the 4 categories of dizygotic twins using narrow criteria: the categories are same-sex female pairs (SS Females), opposite-sex twins with the female member having AN (OS Females), opposite-sex twins with the male member having AN (OS Males), and same-sex male pairs (SS Males). The vertical bars represent the 95% confidence interval for the estimate of the prevalence.

Figure 2. 
The prevalence of anorexia nervosa (AN) for the 4 categories of dizygotic twins using broad criteria: the categories are same-sex female pairs (SS Females), opposite-sex twins with the female member having AN (OS Females), opposite-sex twins with the male member having AN (OS Males), and same-sex male pairs (SS Males). The vertical bars represent the 95% confidence interval for the estimate of the prevalence.

The prevalence of anorexia nervosa (AN) for the 4 categories of dizygotic twins using broad criteria: the categories are same-sex female pairs (SS Females), opposite-sex twins with the female member having AN (OS Females), opposite-sex twins with the male member having AN (OS Males), and same-sex male pairs (SS Males). The vertical bars represent the 95% confidence interval for the estimate of the prevalence.

Table 1. Prevalence of Anorexia Nervosa Using Narrow Diagnostic Criteria
Prevalence of Anorexia Nervosa Using Narrow Diagnostic Criteria
Table 2. Prevalence of Anorexia Nervosa Using Broad Diagnostic Criteria
Prevalence of Anorexia Nervosa Using Broad Diagnostic Criteria
1.
Pawluck  DEGorey  KM Secular trends in the incidence of anorexia nervosa: integrative review of population-based studies.  Int J Eat Disord 1998;23 (4) 347- 352PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Fombonne  E Anorexia nervosa, no evidence of an increase.  Br J Psychiatry 1995;166 (4) 462- 471PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Turnbull  SWard  ATreasure  JJick  HDerby  L The demand for eating disorder care: an epidemiological study using the general practice research database.  Br J Psychiatry 1996;169 (6) 705- 712PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Cobb  S Borderlands of Psychiatry.  Cambridge, MA Harvard University Press1943;
5.
Selvini-Palazzoli  M The meaning of body for anorexia nervosa patients: psychotherapeutic implications.  Proceedings of the 6th International Congress on Psychotherapy Basel, Switzerland S Karger AG1965;96- 103Google Scholar
6.
Carlat  DJCamargo  CA  JrHerzog  DB Eating disorders in males: a report on 135 patients.  Am J Psychiatry 1997;154 (8) 1127- 1132PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Herzog  DBNorman  DKGordon  CPepose  M Sexual conflict and eating disorders in 27 males.  Am J Psychiatry 1984;141 (8) 989- 990PubMedGoogle Scholar
8.
Sharp  CWClark  SADunn  JRBlackwood  DHRShapiro  CM Clinical presentation of anorexia nervosa in males: 24 new cases.  Int J Eat Disord 1994;15 (2) 125- 134PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Woodside  DBKaplan  AS Day hospital treatment in males with eating disorders: response and comparison to females.  J Psychosom Res 1994;38 (5) 471- 475PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Crisp  AHBurns  T Primary anorexia nervosa in the male and female: a comparison of clinical features and prognosis. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;77- 99Google Scholar
11.
Edwin  DHAndersen  AE Psychometric testing in 76 males with eating disorders. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;116- 130Google Scholar
12.
Woodside  DBGarner  DMRockert  WGarfinkel  PE Eating disorders in males: insights from a clinical and psychometric comparison with females. Andersen  AEedMales With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;100- 115
13.
Olivardia  RPope  HG  JrMangweth  BHudson  JI Eating disorders in college men.  Am J Psychiatry 1995;152 (9) 1279- 1285PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Striegel-Moore  RHGarvin  VDohm  FARosenheck  RA Psychiatric comorbidity of eating disorders in men: a national study of hospitalized veterans.  Int J Eat Disord 1999;25 (4) 399- 404PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Woodside  DBGarfinkel  PELin  EGoering  PKaplan  ASGoldbloom  DSKennedy  SH Comparison of men with full or partial eating disorders, men without eating disorders and women with eating disorders in the community.  Am J Psychiatry 2001;158 (4) 570- 574PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Burns  TCrisp  A Outcome of anorexia nervosa in males. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;163- 186Google Scholar
17.
Andersen  AE Diagnosis and treatment of males with eating disorders. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;133- 162Google Scholar
18.
Cohen-Bendahan  CCCvan de Beek  CBerenbaum  SA Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: methods and findings.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29 (2) 353- 384PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Resnick  SMGottesman  IIMcGue  M Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: an effect of prenatal hormones?  Behav Genet 1993;23 (4) 323- 329PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Bulik  CMSullivan  PFTozzi  FFurberg  HLichtenstein  PPedersen  NL Prevalence, heritability, and prospective risk factors for anorexia nervosa.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (3) 305- 312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
 The R project for statistical computing. http://www.R-project.org Accessed September 13, 2007
22.
Agresti  A An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis.  London, England John Wiley & Sons1996;
23.
Gowers  SKadambari  SRCrisp  AH Family structure and birth order of patients with anorexia nervosa.  J Psychiatr Res 1985;19 (2-3) 247- 251PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Britto  DJMeyers  DHSmith  JJPalmer  RL Anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: sibling ratio and birth rank—a catchment area study.  Int J Eat Disord 1997;21 (4) 335- 340PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Fairburn  CGCowen  PJHarrison  PJ Twin studies and the aetiology of eating disorders.  Int J Eat Disord 1999;26 (4) 349- 358PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Gorwood  PKipman  AFoulon  C The human genetics of anorexia nervosa.  Eur J Pharmacol 2003;480 (1-3) 163- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Clemens  LG Neurohormonal control of male sexual behavior.  Reproductive Behavior New York, NY Plenum Press1974;23- 53Google Scholar
28.
vom Saal  FSBronson  FH Sexual characteristics of adult female mice are correlated with their blood testosterone levels during prenatal development.  Science 1980;208 (4444) 597- 599PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Sadler  TW Langman's Medical Embryology. 10th ed Philadelphia, PA Lippincott Williams & Wilkins2006;
30.
McDermott  NJGandelman  RReinisch  JM Contiguity to male fetuses influences ano-genital distance and time of vaginal opening in mice.  Physiol Behav 1978;20 (5) 661- 663PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
vom Saal  FSBronson  FH In utero proximity of female mouse fetuses to males: effect on reproductive performance during later life.  Biol Reprod 1978;19 (4) 842- 853PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Gandelman  Rvom Saal  FSReinisch  JM Contiguity to male fetuses affects morphology and behaviour of female mice.  Nature 1977;266 (5604) 722- 724PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
vom Saal  FS Variation in phenotype due to random intrauterine positioning of male and female fetuses in rodents.  J Reprod Fertil 1981;62 (2) 633- 650PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Clemens  LGGladue  BAConiglio  LP Prenatal endogenous androgenic influences on masculine sexual behavior and genital morphology in male and female rats.  Horm Behav 1978;10 (1) 40- 53PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Lephart  EDFleming  DERhees  W Fetal male masculinization in control and prenatally stressed rats.  Dev Psychobiol 1989;22 (7) 707- 716PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
vom Saal  FSGrant  WMMcMullen  CWLaves  KS High fetal estrogen concentrations: correlation with increased adult sexual activity and decreased aggression in male mice.  Science 1983;220 (4603) 1306- 1308PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Tobet  SADunlap  JLGerall  AA Influence of fetal position on neonatal androgen-induced sterility and sexual behavior in female rats.  Horm Behav 1982;16 (3) 251- 258PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Mankes  RFGlick  SDVan der Hoeven  FLeFevre  R Alcohol preference and hepatic alcohol dehydrogenase activity in adult Long-Evans rats is affected by intrauterine sibling contiguity.  Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1991;15 (1) 80- 85PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Clark  MMGalef  BG  Jr Effects of uterine position on rate of sexual development in female mongolian gerbils.  Physiol Behav 1988;42 (1) 15- 18PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Clark  MMMalefant  SAWinter  DAGalef  BG  Jr Fetal uterine position affects copulation and scent marking by adult male gerbils.  Physiol Behav 1990;47 (2) 301- 305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Rohde Parfet  KALamberson  WRRieke  ARCantley  TCGanjam  VKvom Saal  FSDay  BN Intrauterine position effects in male and female swine: subsequent survivability growth rate, morphology and semen characteristics.  J Anim Sci 1990;68 (1) 179- 185PubMedGoogle Scholar
42.
Rohde Parfet  KAGanjam  VKLamberson  WRRieke  ARvom Saal  FSDay  BN Intrauterine position effects in female swine: subsequent reproductive performance and social and sexual behavior.  Appl Anim Behav Sci 1990;26 (4) 349- 362Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Fels  EBosch  LR Effect of prenatal administration of testosterone on ovarian function in rats.  Am J Obstet Gynecol 1971;111 (7) 964- 969PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Meisel  RLWard  IL Fetal female rats are masculinized by male litter mates located caudally in the uterus.  Science 1981;213 (4504) 239- 241PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Richmond  GSachs  BJ Further evidence of masculinization of female rats by males located caudally in utero.  Horm Behav 1984;18 (4) 484- 490PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Miller  EM Evidence from opposite sex twins for the effects of prenatal sex hormones. Ellis  LEbertz  Leds Males, Females, and Behavior: Towards Biological Understanding. London, England Praeger1998;27- 57Google Scholar
47.
McFadden  D A masculinizing effect on the auditory systems of human females having male co-twins.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993;90 (24) 11 900- 11 904PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Dempsey  PJTownsend  GCRichards  LC Increased tooth crown size in females with twin brothers: evidence for hormonal diffusion between human twins in utero.  Am J Hum Biol 1999;11 (5) 577- 586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Miller  EM Prenatal sex hormone transfer: a reason to study opposite-sex twins.  Pers Individ Dif 1994;17 (4) 511- 529Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Boklage  CE Interactions between opposite-sex dizygotic fetuses and the assumptions of Weinberg difference method epidemiology.  Am J Hum Genet 1985;37 (3) 591- 605PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Boklage  CE Race, zygosity, and mortality among twins: interaction of myth and method.  Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) 1987;36 (3) 275- 288PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Miller  EMMartin  N Analysis of the effect of hormones on opposite-sex twin attitudes.  Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) 1995;44 (1) 41- 52PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Zuckerman  MEyseneck  SBGEyseneck  HJ Sensation seeking in England and America: cross-cultural, age and sex comparisons.  J Consult Clin Psychol 1978;46 (1) 139- 149PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Cole-Harding  SMorstad  ALWilson  JR Spatial ability in members of opposite-sex twin pairs [abstract].  Behav Genet 1988;18 (6) 710Google Scholar
55.
Cohen-Bendahan  CCCBuitelaar  JKvan Goozen  SHMCohen-Kettenis  PT Prenatal exposure to testosterone and functional cerebral lateralization: a study in same-sex and opposite-sex twin girls.  Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004;29 (7) 911- 916PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
van de Beek  CThissen  JHCohen-Kettenis  PTvan Goozan  SHBuitelaar  JK Relationship between sex hormones assessed in amniotic fluid, and maternal and umbilical cord serum: what is the best source of information to investigate the effects of fetal hormonal exposure?  Horm Behav 2004;46 (5) 663- 669PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Goy  RWMcEwen  BS Sexual Differentiation of the Brain.  Cambridge, MA MIT Press1980;
58.
Geschwind  NGalaburda  AM Cerebral Lateralization: Biological Mechanisms, Associations and Pathology.  Cambridge, MAMIT Press1980;
59.
Erlanger  DMKutner  KCJacobs  AR Hormones and cognition: current concepts and issues in neuropsychology.  Neuropsychol Rev 1999;9 (4) 175- 207PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
McEwen  BS Permanence of brain sex differences and structural plasticity of the adult brain.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999;96 (13) 7128- 7130PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Klump  KLGobrogge  KLPerkins  PSThorne  DSisk  CLBreedlove  SM Preliminary evidence that gonadal hormones organize and activate disordered eating.  Psychol Med 2006;36 (4) 539- 546PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Culbert  KMBreedlove  SMKlump  KL Protective effects of prenatal testosterone on risk for eating disorders: a comparison of opposite sex and same sex twins. Poster presented at: Eating Disorders Research Society Meeting; September 30, 2005; Toronto, ON
63.
Slof-Op't Landt  MCvan Furth  EFMeulenbelt  ISlagboom  PEBartels  MBoomsma  DIBulik  CM Eating disorders: from twin studies to candidate genes and beyond.  Twin Res Hum Genet 2005;8 (5) 467- 482PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Kläning  UPedersen  CBMortensen  PBKyvik  KOSkytthe  A A possible association between the genetic predisposition for dizygotic twinning and schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 2002;58 (1) 31- 35PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Klump  KLWonderlich  SLehoux  PLilenfeld  LRBulik  CM Does environment matter? a review of nonshared environment and eating disorders.  Int J Eat Disord 2002;31 (2) 118- 135PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
December 2007

Intrauterine Hormonal Environment and Risk of Developing Anorexia Nervosa

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Surrey and Borders Partnership NHS Trust, Chertsey, and Department of Psychiatry, Medical School, University of Sussex, Brighton, England (Dr Procopio); and Department of Statistics and Actuarial Science, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada (Dr Marriott).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2007;64(12):1402-1407. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.64.12.1402
Abstract

Context  Anorexia nervosa (AN) is approximately 10 times more common in females than in males. The reasons for this difference are not yet understood. Several mechanisms have been hypothesized as possible causes.

Objective  To determine whether the different hormonal environments to which male and female fetuses are exposed in utero might contribute to the increased risk of developing AN in females.

Design, Setting, and Participants  The study is based on a large population-based cohort of Swedish twins. The strategy used is to compare the prevalence for AN between same-sex and opposite-sex twins.

Results  The study shows that the risk of developing AN in female twins is higher than in male twins, as expected. The only exception is male members of opposite-sex pairs, who have a higher risk of developing the illness when compared with other males ( = .62 for narrow diagnostic criteria and  = .60 for broad diagnostic criteria). In fact, their risk is at a level that is not statistically significantly different from that of females from such a pair. A plausible explanation for this phenomenon is that in pregnancies bearing a female fetus, a substance is produced, probably hormonal, that increases the risk of having AN in adulthood. Because the male half of an opposite-sex twin pair would also be exposed to this substance, it could account for the observed elevated risk in males with female twins. The most likely candidates are sex steroid hormones.

Conclusions  The results of our study are compatible with the hypothesis that intrauterine exposure to sex hormones might influence neurodevelopment, affecting the risk of developing AN in adult life. This might be a factor contributing to the higher risk of developing AN in females.

Anorexia nervosa (AN) is approximately 10 times more common in females than in males.1-3 The reasons for this difference are not known, and it is likely that their unraveling will represent an important step forward in the understanding of the etiopathogenetic factors involved in the development of eating disorders.

Several hypotheses have been developed to explain the discrepancy in the prevalence of AN between the sexes, but none of these has been completely satisfactory.

One school of thought maintains that, because of its rarity, AN in males must have atypical characteristics or even might be a different disease from the one that affects females.4,5

The other line of reasoning is instead that AN has similar characteristics in males and females, and that males who develop AN have special features that differentiate them from other males not affected by the illness.6,7

The literature available is not unequivocal, but altogether seems to support the second hypothesis. In most studies8-12 that compare how the 2 sexes are affected by AN, the similarities between males and females largely outweigh the differences in symptoms, course, and outcome. Furthermore, several studies13-15 show that males with AN have important differences in multiple parameters when compared with male control subjects. Burns and Crisp16 argue that the characteristics of AN in males are virtually indistinguishable from those of the same disease in females. Andersen17 cautioned that, despite the similarities, there are some specific characteristics of AN in males, especially in regard to diagnosis and treatment.

The differences between males and females deriving from prevalence studies of AN might be an overestimation of the real skew between the 2 sexes.17 There is, in fact, evidence for a higher level of underdiagnosis or misdiagnosis of AN in males than in females, possibly as a result of the popular belief that AN affects only teenaged girls.4,5 This might amplify the reluctance of males affected to seek help and of their families to recognize the disorder.17

The hypothesis tested in this study is that the different hormonal environments to which male and female fetuses are exposed influence neurodevelopment in utero, contributing to the diversity in the risk of developing AN between the 2 sexes. The design used to test the hypothesis is a comparison of the prevalence of AN in members of monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) twin pairs, stratified by the possible different sex permutations. The analysis of the prevalence of AN in members of opposite-sex pairs of DZ twins is essential for testing the hypothesis.

Opposite-sex twins constitute a real-life laboratory for studying the influence of sex hormones on intrauterine development.18 The literature in humans and in animals shows that when a female and a male fetus are contiguous in utero, there is often a masculinization of the female fetus and a feminization of the male fetus.18,19 Furthermore, this can have permanent lifetime consequences.18 There are indications from the literature18,19 that the cause of this phenomenon might be the change of the hormonal environment in utero because of the presence of an opposite-sex twin.

A difference in the risk of developing AN in twins who are part of opposite-sex pairs, when compared with members of same-sex pairs, would be consistent with a prenatal influence of gonadal hormones on the likelihood of developing AN in adult life. This could explain, at least in part, the difference in the prevalence of AN between the sexes. An increased prevalence of AN in males who are members of opposite-sex pairs would be suggestive that the presence of a female fetus influences the intrauterine environment, favoring the development of AN. If there were a decreased prevalence of the disease in female members of opposite-sex pairs, this would be indicative that the presence of a male fetus in utero is a protective factor against the development of AN. The 2 previously stated outcomes are not mutually exclusive.

Methods
Subjects

We have used, for testing the hypothesis, the population identified by Bulik et al20 in their study on the epidemiological features and heritability of AN. We chose this study because the researchers identified the largest series of twin subjects diagnosed as having AN using rigorous diagnostic criteria and because, unlike most twin research, the data set includes opposite-sex twins, which is essential for our analysis.

The population studied by Bulik et al20 is represented by all members of the Swedish Twin Registry who were born between January 1, 1935, and December 31, 1958, and who consented to being interviewed on the telephone. The subjects in this population are those who, at the interview, satisfied the DSM-IV definition of AN20 regardless of the criterion that needed amenorrhea to make a diagnosis. A subpopulation of the previously described group that satisfied the full DSM-IV criteria for AN, including amenorrhea, was defined as fulfilling a narrow definition of AN.20 Therefore, all subjects who satisfied the narrow definition were also part of the group that satisfied the broader definition of AN, but not necessarily vice versa. This distinction applies only for females because the amenorrhea criterion is not relevant in males, in whom the broad and narrow groups coincide.

The participation rate was 76%.20 The test-retest reliability was assessed by Bulik et al,20 who selected at random 105 individual twins who completed the telephone screening. They reinterviewed these subjects within 2 weeks from the first interview and assessed the short-term reliability for 2 key variables associated with eating disorders: low weight and dieting behavior. The Cohen κ value was 0.63 for AN criterion A (“Did you ever weigh less than other people thought you should weigh?”) and 0.48 for the broader variable querying about dieting history. The researchers judged that the κ variables suggest moderate to substantial test-retest reliability.20 The reliability of the dieting history variable may be defined as low.

The data used for our analysis were taken from the summary statistics of Table 4 in the article by Bulik et al.20 We calculated, for each phenotype, the number of individual twins identified as having AN on the telephone interviews. Adding up, for each phenotype, the twins affected by AN and those not affected by the illness, we obtained the total number of twins who participated in the study for each phenotype. From this, we were able to calculate the prevalence of AN for each of the phenotypes. The same operation was done for narrow and broad diagnostic criteria.

Statistical analysis

Table 1 and Table 2 show that there are marked differences in proportions between males and females in the prevalence of AN, except in the case of a member of an opposite-sex DZ twin pair. In the analysis of this article, the null hypotheses of no differences in proportions are compared using tests for equality of proportions for both dependent and independent samples, as implemented by a statistics package.21 Unless otherwise stated, all tests are 2-sided, and a continuity correction was used only if it did not exceed the difference of the sample proportions in absolute value. When the test used is appropriate for independent samples, a large sample (χ2) test for equal proportions was used, which is appropriate because the smallest sample was greater than 2000. For the case of dependent samples (eg, different sex twins), the McNemar test is used. The McNemar test is a test of equality of proportions for matched binary responses.22 It is equivalent to other tests based on linear models, such as the logistic or nonparametric tests, like the Cochran-Mantel-Haenszel test.22

Because the McNemar test is based on the χ2, it is required that the data are not too sparse.22 In general, the total number of positive cases (males and females) needs to be greater than 10. In our data, the smallest value was 36; thus, the test was appropriate.

Results

Table 1 and Table 2 represent the prevalence of AN for the different twin phenotypes using narrow and broad diagnostic criteria, respectively. There were no examples, in the sample analyzed, of couples of mixed-sex twins in which both members were affected by AN.

This study confirms, in a twin population, that females have a much higher prevalence of AN than males (P < .001 for broad and narrow criteria for comparing all males and females). The only exception, which is the main focus of our study, is observed when a man is the member of an opposite-sex DZ twin pair: in this case, his risk of developing AN increases to much higher levels than in same-sex male twins, levels that are not significantly different (P = .62 for narrow diagnostic criteria and P = .60 for broad diagnostic criteria) from that of the females in that pair. The risk of developing AN in females who are part of opposite-sex twin pairs is not significantly different from that of other females. These results are obtained using narrow and broad criteria for the diagnosis of AN (P = .48 for narrow diagnostic criteria and P = .18 for broad diagnostic criteria).

The main question of interest in this investigation is whether different hormonal environments contribute to an increased risk of developing AN. Because this is fundamentally a question about a causal relationship, no definitive conclusions can be drawn from the data, which are observational and archival. The possibility that the observed differences between males in opposite-sex twin pairs compared with those in same-sex pairs could be because of unobserved confounding factors cannot be ruled out and has not been controlled for by any type of randomization.

One possible factor of interest is a cohort effect. This is of particular interest because Bulik et al20 noted a difference in the rate of AN in males before and after 1945. However, for such an effect to be important for this study, there would have to be a difference effect between same- and mixed-sex twin pairs and not just a time change within a sex group. The trend in time in males and females is clear,20 but we have no information about a differential effect across type of twin pair.

Other limitations include the fact that the data came from retrospective interviews and the change in detection rate for AN. Also, because of the rareness of reported male AN, the sample sizes are small, although, as previously discussed, they are adequate for the implementation of the statistical tests used.

Comment

Members of twin pairs are exposed to the same milieu in utero and usually share a similar environment during at least the first 2 decades of their postnatal lives. Therefore, the increased prevalence of AN in males who are members of opposite-sex DZ pairs could be explained either by the effect, on the prenatal environment, of the contemporaneous presence of a female fetus in utero or the effect of growing up with a female twin. It is unlikely that the increased risk observed in male members of opposite-sex twin pairs is because of obstetric complications connected to twin births, because otherwise the risk would be increased also in MZ and DZ pairs composed of 2 males.

As already mentioned, a hypothesis compatible with the results obtained in our study is that the presence of a twin sister in an opposite-sex DZ couple increases the likelihood that the twin brother is exposed to an environment, during childhood and/or adolescence, that increases his risk of developing AN. The literature on family structure in AN does not seem to support this hypothesis, showing that the risk of developing AN is not altered by the sex of siblings, certainly not to the extent of explaining the dramatic phenomenon observed in our study.23,24 These findings are confirmed by the literature25,26 on twins and AN, in which the role of the shared family environment does not seem to be significantly different from 0.

The results obtained are consistent with the hypothesis tested in our study; there exists an intrauterine influence because of gonadal hormones, possibly on neurodevelopment, that affects the risk of developing AN. The research on opposite-sex twins, described later, offers a framework that allows an understanding of the potential mechanisms involved in such a hypothesis.

Both animals and humans have characteristics that differ between sexes. These sexually dimorphic aspects include anatomical and psychological/behavioral features. There is extensive literature on the reciprocal influence of fetuses that are members of opposite-sex twin pairs in humans, and of mixed-sex litters in animals, on these sexually dimorphic characteristics. The indication is that female fetuses can be masculinized and male fetuses feminized by the presence of an adjacent opposite-sex twin in utero, in humans and in animals, with permanent consequences. This means that subjects who developed in utero in the proximity of an opposite-sex fetus can show some of the sexually dimorphic features that are usually present in the opposite sex.18,19

The first studies to unravel the previously mentioned phenomenon showed a masculinized pattern of sexually dimorphic features in female mice that developed in utero in proximity to opposite-sex mice, when compared with other female mice. This phenomenon was explained by the influence on the female fetus of the testosterone produced by the adjacent male fetus(es).27-34 The described findings are consistent with the observation that the differentiation of the reproductive system in mammals depends mainly on the presence or absence of androgens during critical periods of intrauterine life.29

More surprisingly, it has been observed, at times unintentionally, while analyzing data on the masculinization of females, that also the opposite effect occurs: a feminization in the male mice that were surrounded in utero by females.35,36 This phenomenon is difficult to explain when looking at the development of feminine characteristics as simply the passive result of absent or low concentrations of androgens, without involving the action of other hormones.

Observations in rats,27,34,37,38 mongolian gerbils,39,40 and pigs41,42 confirm that the position in utero in relation to opposite-sex littermates contributes to variability of dimorphic features within the same-sex group. It is not yet clear from the literature43-45 if the hormonal influence between opposite-sex littermates takes place through fetofetal diffusion or the local circulation of hormones.

The research conducted in humans is consistent with the previously described animal studies in suggesting that often twins of an opposite-sex pair show sexually dimorphic features discordant with their chromosomal sex in regard to anatomical46-49 and psychological/behavioral19,46,49 characteristics. Anatomical features are important in proving that the reciprocal influence in opposite-sex twins in humans is of prenatal origin. The psychological/behavioral influences between opposite-sex twins could in fact be because of postnatal interactions, while this cannot be the case for anatomical features present from birth.

Males generally have larger teeth in the right jaw than females, and it was relatively easy to separate males from females just based on this measurement. A mathematical classifier was developed that classified correctly 123 of 128 same-sex twins of both zygosities. The same system of classification, however, misclassified 16 of 20 opposite-sex twins, 7 of 10 males and 9 of 10 females, showing a strong reciprocal influence in male and female members of opposite-sex pairs.50

More evidence for feminization of male members of opposite-sex twin pairs derives from the analysis of a database that showed that fetal death rates, a sexually dimorphic feature with lower rates in females, is less frequent in opposite-sex twin males than in other male twins.51

A study52 of childhood sex role behaviors comparing same-sex twins with opposite-sex twins showed that male co-twins of females tended to show a decrease of sex role behaviors when compared with male members of same-sex twin pairs.

Sensation-seeking behavior is a sexually dimorphic trait that is overrepresented in males across cultures.53 Analysis of sensation-seeking behavior in twins showed that it was significantly higher in the female half of opposite-sex twin pairs when compared with female members of same-sex twin pairs.18,19

Two studies49,52 on general attitudes in twin populations, one performed in England and one in Australia, showed that members of opposite-sex twin pairs were more likely to give opinions characteristic of the opposite sex than members of same-sex twin pairs.

A twin study54 on spatial performance, on which males perform on average significantly better than females, has shown that the female part of opposite-sex twin couples had on average significantly higher test scores than members of same-sex female twin pairs. Studies of cerebral lateralization in opposite-sex twins also confirm the effect of prenatal hormones on sexually dimorphic traits.55 As in the animal research, some human studies seem to suggest that the influence between opposite-sex twins takes place through a change in the hormonal levels in the local maternal circulation, while other studies imply a local hormone transfer between the 2 fetuses.19 Studies in humans are more complex for obvious ethical reasons.56

It is unfortunate that information on the “chorionicity” and “amniocity” of the twin couples was not available in the Swedish sample, because this would have added interesting material for discussion on how the hormone transfer between fetuses might take place.

The evidence from the literature on opposite-sex twins in humans and mixed-sex litters in animals, therefore, supports a masculinizing effect on females caused by the presence of a contiguous male fetus in utero, possibly through hormonal percolation. This is not surprising because the evidence on sexual differentiation in mammals shows that a fetus, even if genetically female, develops the sexual characteristics of a male if exposed to androgens during a critical phase of the intrauterine development.29

There is also evidence, although not as robust, for the symmetrical effect: the feminization of males who were contiguous to female fetuses in utero. This phenomenon is not consistent with the view of feminization as an “on-off” passive default process that takes place in the absence of androgens. It is instead in line with a more modern view of feminization as the result of the active interaction of several sex hormones, including estrogens, and not just of the presence or absence of androgens.18 This model is particularly useful for traits that are in a continuum between masculinity and femininity, even within members of the same sex, like in personality traits, and not just either male or female, like in primary sexual characteristics.

A model that takes the presence or absence of androgens as the only factor that affects the masculinization-feminization phenomenon would have predicted that the results in this article would be consistent with the action of androgens as a protective factor against the development of AN. This study is instead compatible with the existence of factors that increase the risk of developing AN in adult life in pregnancies that involve a female fetus. As previously stated, a model with a simple dichotomy between feminine and masculine characteristics, driven only by the presence or absence of androgens, is likely to be simplistic, especially for complex traits like the ones we are studying. Other hormones, including estrogens, are likely to be important in this process, and it has to be expected that their concentration in the body fluids at different stages during the intrauterine development is relevant.18

Based on the results in our study, estrogens are, therefore, tempting candidates for the substance that increases the risk of AN in females through its action during neurodevelopment. This is supported by the evidence in the literature57-60 that neuronal growth and proliferation are coregulated by gonadal hormones during neurodevelopment, with permanent and irreversible structural organizing effects on the central nervous system.

Evidence for an influence of prenatal sex hormones on the future development of eating disorders also comes from other lines of research. A recent study by Klump et al61 has shown that the risk of developing an eating disorder is correlated with the individual's digits' length ratio, which is an indicator of the hormonal environment to which the subject was exposed in utero. In another study, Culbert et al62 examined disordered eating, not just AN, in same-sex and opposite-sex twins, showing evidence that was compatible with an influence of prenatal hormones on the development of eating disorders. The evidence in the latter study was in favor of testosterone being a protective factor, rather than estrogens a predisposing one. The results of this study62 are not incompatible with ours. One possible explanation for the difference is that the populations in the 2 studies are not homogeneous. The study by Culbert et al did not include only patients with AN but a broader group of subjects with a variety of symptoms of disordered eating. Also, in our study, there is a trend, albeit not reaching statistical significance, for a decreased prevalence of AN in the female members of opposite-sex twin pairs when compared with females in same-sex twin pairs. The trend was stronger when using broad diagnostic criteria, therefore analyzing a population more similar to the one in the study by Culbert et al, rather than using narrow criteria. It is, therefore, possible that both effects are present, a protective action of testosterone and a pathogenic action of estrogens, and each study was able to show just 1 of the 2 phenomena at a statistically significant level because of type II errors that are relatively large because of the rarity of AN. Another possible explanation is that both hormones are involved in the development of eating disorders and the proportion between the concentrations of estrogens and androgens in utero could play a part in shaping the symptoms of the eating disorder in adult life.

It is interesting, with regard to the previous discussion, to consider a trend in the prevalence of AN, with same-sex female twins showing the highest rates, followed by opposite-sex female twins, opposite-sex male twins, and eventually same-sex male twins. This trend cannot be tested formally from the statistical point of view, because the explanatory variate is categorical, but it does offer a visual qualitative perspective of the potential influence on the prevalence of AN by male and female sex hormones (Figure 1 and Figure 2).

The prevalence of AN in the study by Bulik et al20 was significantly higher in DZ twins than in MZ twins. Before developing hypotheses on the possible relevance of this result, it will need to be replicated. We are not aware of any other published studies that could verify the previously described result. Twin studies are, in fact, usually engineered with the aim to compare the concordance between MZ and DZ twins and not to compare their prevalence.63 It would be of interest to try to replicate the previously described result in other twin series, also on the evidence that the literature seems to indicate the presence of a similar phenomenon in patients affected by schizophrenia.64

In conclusion, the results obtained after comparing the prevalence of AN in opposite-sex and same-sex twins are compatible with the hypothesis that female fetuses produce a hormonal environment in utero that increases the risk of developing AN in adult life, possibly by influencing neurodevelopment. There is also evidence for a trend, albeit not at a statistically significant level, for a protective effect on the development of AN of hormones produced by male fetuses. The difference in the prevalence of AN between males and females might, therefore, originate, at least in part, in utero.

The finding of an influence of prenatal sex hormones on the cause of AN could be integrated in several ways with the evidence from the literature20,25,26,63 showing that genetic factors have a central role in the cause of the illness. One possibility would be a multifactorial threshold model that could account for genetic and hormonal causes as additional factors contributing, probably jointly with other agents, to reaching the threshold for the development of the illness. Another possibility would be a “2-hit model” in which the genetic endowment would predispose the individual to AN and the prenatal hormonal environment could offer the second hit. It could be hypothesized that individuals who are not exposed to the second hit might show only a partial phenotype or vulnerability to other “second hits” later in life. The 2 previously described explanations, no matter how attractive from the theoretical point of view, are in contrast with the literature25,26 that consistently shows that shared environmental factors have only a marginal influence on the origin of AN in adulthood. It is more likely that the hormonal environment in utero might account for the significant effect of nonshared environmental factors on the origin of AN.65

A third possibility, which is not mutually exclusive from the previously described possibilities, is that the genetic predisposition for AN coincides with the genetic predisposition to a certain prenatal hormonal setting, unifying the evidence for a genetic cause and our findings.

Correspondence: Marco Procopio, MD, MRCPsych, Napier House, 62-64 Bridge St, Walton on Thames, Surrey KT12 1AP, England (marcoprocopio00@hotmail.com).

Submitted for Publication: October 8, 2006; final revision received June 15, 2007; accepted June 15, 2007.

Financial Disclosure: None reported.

References
1.
Pawluck  DEGorey  KM Secular trends in the incidence of anorexia nervosa: integrative review of population-based studies.  Int J Eat Disord 1998;23 (4) 347- 352PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
2.
Fombonne  E Anorexia nervosa, no evidence of an increase.  Br J Psychiatry 1995;166 (4) 462- 471PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Turnbull  SWard  ATreasure  JJick  HDerby  L The demand for eating disorder care: an epidemiological study using the general practice research database.  Br J Psychiatry 1996;169 (6) 705- 712PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Cobb  S Borderlands of Psychiatry.  Cambridge, MA Harvard University Press1943;
5.
Selvini-Palazzoli  M The meaning of body for anorexia nervosa patients: psychotherapeutic implications.  Proceedings of the 6th International Congress on Psychotherapy Basel, Switzerland S Karger AG1965;96- 103Google Scholar
6.
Carlat  DJCamargo  CA  JrHerzog  DB Eating disorders in males: a report on 135 patients.  Am J Psychiatry 1997;154 (8) 1127- 1132PubMedGoogle Scholar
7.
Herzog  DBNorman  DKGordon  CPepose  M Sexual conflict and eating disorders in 27 males.  Am J Psychiatry 1984;141 (8) 989- 990PubMedGoogle Scholar
8.
Sharp  CWClark  SADunn  JRBlackwood  DHRShapiro  CM Clinical presentation of anorexia nervosa in males: 24 new cases.  Int J Eat Disord 1994;15 (2) 125- 134PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Woodside  DBKaplan  AS Day hospital treatment in males with eating disorders: response and comparison to females.  J Psychosom Res 1994;38 (5) 471- 475PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Crisp  AHBurns  T Primary anorexia nervosa in the male and female: a comparison of clinical features and prognosis. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;77- 99Google Scholar
11.
Edwin  DHAndersen  AE Psychometric testing in 76 males with eating disorders. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;116- 130Google Scholar
12.
Woodside  DBGarner  DMRockert  WGarfinkel  PE Eating disorders in males: insights from a clinical and psychometric comparison with females. Andersen  AEedMales With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;100- 115
13.
Olivardia  RPope  HG  JrMangweth  BHudson  JI Eating disorders in college men.  Am J Psychiatry 1995;152 (9) 1279- 1285PubMedGoogle Scholar
14.
Striegel-Moore  RHGarvin  VDohm  FARosenheck  RA Psychiatric comorbidity of eating disorders in men: a national study of hospitalized veterans.  Int J Eat Disord 1999;25 (4) 399- 404PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Woodside  DBGarfinkel  PELin  EGoering  PKaplan  ASGoldbloom  DSKennedy  SH Comparison of men with full or partial eating disorders, men without eating disorders and women with eating disorders in the community.  Am J Psychiatry 2001;158 (4) 570- 574PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
16.
Burns  TCrisp  A Outcome of anorexia nervosa in males. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;163- 186Google Scholar
17.
Andersen  AE Diagnosis and treatment of males with eating disorders. Andersen  AEed Males With Eating Disorders. New York, NY Brunner/Mazel1990;133- 162Google Scholar
18.
Cohen-Bendahan  CCCvan de Beek  CBerenbaum  SA Prenatal sex hormone effects on child and adult sex-typed behavior: methods and findings.  Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2005;29 (2) 353- 384PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Resnick  SMGottesman  IIMcGue  M Sensation seeking in opposite-sex twins: an effect of prenatal hormones?  Behav Genet 1993;23 (4) 323- 329PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Bulik  CMSullivan  PFTozzi  FFurberg  HLichtenstein  PPedersen  NL Prevalence, heritability, and prospective risk factors for anorexia nervosa.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (3) 305- 312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
 The R project for statistical computing. http://www.R-project.org Accessed September 13, 2007
22.
Agresti  A An Introduction to Categorical Data Analysis.  London, England John Wiley & Sons1996;
23.
Gowers  SKadambari  SRCrisp  AH Family structure and birth order of patients with anorexia nervosa.  J Psychiatr Res 1985;19 (2-3) 247- 251PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Britto  DJMeyers  DHSmith  JJPalmer  RL Anorexia nervosa and bulimia nervosa: sibling ratio and birth rank—a catchment area study.  Int J Eat Disord 1997;21 (4) 335- 340PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Fairburn  CGCowen  PJHarrison  PJ Twin studies and the aetiology of eating disorders.  Int J Eat Disord 1999;26 (4) 349- 358PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Gorwood  PKipman  AFoulon  C The human genetics of anorexia nervosa.  Eur J Pharmacol 2003;480 (1-3) 163- 170PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Clemens  LG Neurohormonal control of male sexual behavior.  Reproductive Behavior New York, NY Plenum Press1974;23- 53Google Scholar
28.
vom Saal  FSBronson  FH Sexual characteristics of adult female mice are correlated with their blood testosterone levels during prenatal development.  Science 1980;208 (4444) 597- 599PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Sadler  TW Langman's Medical Embryology. 10th ed Philadelphia, PA Lippincott Williams & Wilkins2006;
30.
McDermott  NJGandelman  RReinisch  JM Contiguity to male fetuses influences ano-genital distance and time of vaginal opening in mice.  Physiol Behav 1978;20 (5) 661- 663PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
vom Saal  FSBronson  FH In utero proximity of female mouse fetuses to males: effect on reproductive performance during later life.  Biol Reprod 1978;19 (4) 842- 853PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
32.
Gandelman  Rvom Saal  FSReinisch  JM Contiguity to male fetuses affects morphology and behaviour of female mice.  Nature 1977;266 (5604) 722- 724PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
vom Saal  FS Variation in phenotype due to random intrauterine positioning of male and female fetuses in rodents.  J Reprod Fertil 1981;62 (2) 633- 650PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Clemens  LGGladue  BAConiglio  LP Prenatal endogenous androgenic influences on masculine sexual behavior and genital morphology in male and female rats.  Horm Behav 1978;10 (1) 40- 53PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Lephart  EDFleming  DERhees  W Fetal male masculinization in control and prenatally stressed rats.  Dev Psychobiol 1989;22 (7) 707- 716PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
vom Saal  FSGrant  WMMcMullen  CWLaves  KS High fetal estrogen concentrations: correlation with increased adult sexual activity and decreased aggression in male mice.  Science 1983;220 (4603) 1306- 1308PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Tobet  SADunlap  JLGerall  AA Influence of fetal position on neonatal androgen-induced sterility and sexual behavior in female rats.  Horm Behav 1982;16 (3) 251- 258PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Mankes  RFGlick  SDVan der Hoeven  FLeFevre  R Alcohol preference and hepatic alcohol dehydrogenase activity in adult Long-Evans rats is affected by intrauterine sibling contiguity.  Alcohol Clin Exp Res 1991;15 (1) 80- 85PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
39.
Clark  MMGalef  BG  Jr Effects of uterine position on rate of sexual development in female mongolian gerbils.  Physiol Behav 1988;42 (1) 15- 18PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Clark  MMMalefant  SAWinter  DAGalef  BG  Jr Fetal uterine position affects copulation and scent marking by adult male gerbils.  Physiol Behav 1990;47 (2) 301- 305PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Rohde Parfet  KALamberson  WRRieke  ARCantley  TCGanjam  VKvom Saal  FSDay  BN Intrauterine position effects in male and female swine: subsequent survivability growth rate, morphology and semen characteristics.  J Anim Sci 1990;68 (1) 179- 185PubMedGoogle Scholar
42.
Rohde Parfet  KAGanjam  VKLamberson  WRRieke  ARvom Saal  FSDay  BN Intrauterine position effects in female swine: subsequent reproductive performance and social and sexual behavior.  Appl Anim Behav Sci 1990;26 (4) 349- 362Google ScholarCrossref
43.
Fels  EBosch  LR Effect of prenatal administration of testosterone on ovarian function in rats.  Am J Obstet Gynecol 1971;111 (7) 964- 969PubMedGoogle Scholar
44.
Meisel  RLWard  IL Fetal female rats are masculinized by male litter mates located caudally in the uterus.  Science 1981;213 (4504) 239- 241PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Richmond  GSachs  BJ Further evidence of masculinization of female rats by males located caudally in utero.  Horm Behav 1984;18 (4) 484- 490PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Miller  EM Evidence from opposite sex twins for the effects of prenatal sex hormones. Ellis  LEbertz  Leds Males, Females, and Behavior: Towards Biological Understanding. London, England Praeger1998;27- 57Google Scholar
47.
McFadden  D A masculinizing effect on the auditory systems of human females having male co-twins.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1993;90 (24) 11 900- 11 904PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Dempsey  PJTownsend  GCRichards  LC Increased tooth crown size in females with twin brothers: evidence for hormonal diffusion between human twins in utero.  Am J Hum Biol 1999;11 (5) 577- 586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Miller  EM Prenatal sex hormone transfer: a reason to study opposite-sex twins.  Pers Individ Dif 1994;17 (4) 511- 529Google ScholarCrossref
50.
Boklage  CE Interactions between opposite-sex dizygotic fetuses and the assumptions of Weinberg difference method epidemiology.  Am J Hum Genet 1985;37 (3) 591- 605PubMedGoogle Scholar
51.
Boklage  CE Race, zygosity, and mortality among twins: interaction of myth and method.  Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) 1987;36 (3) 275- 288PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Miller  EMMartin  N Analysis of the effect of hormones on opposite-sex twin attitudes.  Acta Genet Med Gemellol (Roma) 1995;44 (1) 41- 52PubMedGoogle Scholar
53.
Zuckerman  MEyseneck  SBGEyseneck  HJ Sensation seeking in England and America: cross-cultural, age and sex comparisons.  J Consult Clin Psychol 1978;46 (1) 139- 149PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Cole-Harding  SMorstad  ALWilson  JR Spatial ability in members of opposite-sex twin pairs [abstract].  Behav Genet 1988;18 (6) 710Google Scholar
55.
Cohen-Bendahan  CCCBuitelaar  JKvan Goozen  SHMCohen-Kettenis  PT Prenatal exposure to testosterone and functional cerebral lateralization: a study in same-sex and opposite-sex twin girls.  Psychoneuroendocrinology 2004;29 (7) 911- 916PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
van de Beek  CThissen  JHCohen-Kettenis  PTvan Goozan  SHBuitelaar  JK Relationship between sex hormones assessed in amniotic fluid, and maternal and umbilical cord serum: what is the best source of information to investigate the effects of fetal hormonal exposure?  Horm Behav 2004;46 (5) 663- 669PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Goy  RWMcEwen  BS Sexual Differentiation of the Brain.  Cambridge, MA MIT Press1980;
58.
Geschwind  NGalaburda  AM Cerebral Lateralization: Biological Mechanisms, Associations and Pathology.  Cambridge, MAMIT Press1980;
59.
Erlanger  DMKutner  KCJacobs  AR Hormones and cognition: current concepts and issues in neuropsychology.  Neuropsychol Rev 1999;9 (4) 175- 207PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
60.
McEwen  BS Permanence of brain sex differences and structural plasticity of the adult brain.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1999;96 (13) 7128- 7130PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
61.
Klump  KLGobrogge  KLPerkins  PSThorne  DSisk  CLBreedlove  SM Preliminary evidence that gonadal hormones organize and activate disordered eating.  Psychol Med 2006;36 (4) 539- 546PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
62.
Culbert  KMBreedlove  SMKlump  KL Protective effects of prenatal testosterone on risk for eating disorders: a comparison of opposite sex and same sex twins. Poster presented at: Eating Disorders Research Society Meeting; September 30, 2005; Toronto, ON
63.
Slof-Op't Landt  MCvan Furth  EFMeulenbelt  ISlagboom  PEBartels  MBoomsma  DIBulik  CM Eating disorders: from twin studies to candidate genes and beyond.  Twin Res Hum Genet 2005;8 (5) 467- 482PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
64.
Kläning  UPedersen  CBMortensen  PBKyvik  KOSkytthe  A A possible association between the genetic predisposition for dizygotic twinning and schizophrenia.  Schizophr Res 2002;58 (1) 31- 35PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
65.
Klump  KLWonderlich  SLehoux  PLilenfeld  LRBulik  CM Does environment matter? a review of nonshared environment and eating disorders.  Int J Eat Disord 2002;31 (2) 118- 135PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×