[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. 
Schematic of the gene encoding regulator of G protein signaling 2 (RGS2)
locus, depicting positions of genotyped single-nucleotide polymorphisms.
The lower portion of the figure shows a magnification of the RGS2 locus. Kb indicates kilobase.

Schematic of the gene encoding regulator of G protein signaling 2 (RGS2) locus, depicting positions of genotyped single-nucleotide polymorphisms. The lower portion of the figure shows a magnification of the RGS2 locus. Kb indicates kilobase.

Figure 2. 
Linkage disequilibrium (LD)
and association analyses for the gene encoding regulator of G protein signaling 2 (RGS2) in the behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar family sample. Gene position for the RGS2 locus is shown above the chromosome bar. Relative positions of genotyped single-nucleotide polymorphism markers are shown, and significant single-marker association results are depicted with colored circles. Haplotypes with frequencies greater than 5% are shown below the markers, and the associated haplotype is shaded in green. Pairwise marker LD using the D′ statistic is indicated by the matrix at bottom (red indicates strong LD; white, weak LD). LOD indicates logarithm of the odds. Figure was drawn with Locusview (T.
Petryshen, A. Kirby, and M. Ainscow, unpublished software; http://www.broad.mit.edu/mpg/locusview). Kb indicates kilobase; Mb, megabus.

Linkage disequilibrium (LD) and association analyses for the gene encoding regulator of G protein signaling 2 (RGS2) in the behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar family sample. Gene position for the RGS2 locus is shown above the chromosome bar. Relative positions of genotyped single-nucleotide polymorphism markers are shown, and significant single-marker association results are depicted with colored circles. Haplotypes with frequencies greater than 5% are shown below the markers, and the associated haplotype is shaded in green. Pairwise marker LD using the D′ statistic is indicated by the matrix at bottom (red indicates strong LD; white, weak LD). LOD indicates logarithm of the odds. Figure was drawn with Locusview (T. Petryshen, A. Kirby, and M. Ainscow, unpublished software; http://www.broad.mit.edu/mpg/locusview). Kb indicates kilobase; Mb, megabus.

Figure 3. 
Activation differences associated with the rs4606 genotype in the left amygdala for the combined 1.5-T and 3-T samples. The marginal means are obtained relative to the covaried ancestral proportion weights and field strength indicators. A, Active voxels are volume thresholded at an a posteriori P < .05. Red area indicates significant difference across genotype. B, Associated bar graphs show the signal difference between emotion face processing and the sensorimotor control condition (adjusted for ancestral proportion weights and field strength indicators).
Error bars indicate SD.

Activation differences associated with the rs4606 genotype in the left amygdala for the combined 1.5-T and 3-T samples. The marginal means are obtained relative to the covaried ancestral proportion weights and field strength indicators. A, Active voxels are volume thresholded at an a posteriori P < .05. Red area indicates significant difference across genotype. B, Associated bar graphs show the signal difference between emotion face processing and the sensorimotor control condition (adjusted for ancestral proportion weights and field strength indicators). Error bars indicate SD.

Figure 4. 
Activation differences associated with the rs4606 genotype in bilateral insular cortices for the combined 1.5-T and 3-T samples. The marginal means are obtained relative to the covaried ancestral proportion weights and field strength indicators.
For the statistical effect, see  eTable 1. A and B, The bar graphs show the larger of the 2 areas within the right anterior insular cortex (A) and the left anterior insular cortex (B). Error bars indicate SD. C and D, Axial sections in Talairach coordinates at z = 0 and z = 4 show 2 regions on the left (D) and 1 on the right (C) (in red).

Activation differences associated with the rs4606 genotype in bilateral insular cortices for the combined 1.5-T and 3-T samples. The marginal means are obtained relative to the covaried ancestral proportion weights and field strength indicators. For the statistical effect, see eTable 1. A and B, The bar graphs show the larger of the 2 areas within the right anterior insular cortex (A) and the left anterior insular cortex (B). Error bars indicate SD. C and D, Axial sections in Talairach coordinates at z = 0 and z = 4 show 2 regions on the left (D) and 1 on the right (C) (in red).

Figure 5. 
The rs10801152 genotype effect—marginal signal difference (face shape)—as a function of genotype adjusted for magnet strength and ancestral proportion. A, The axial section and bar graphs show activation differences (in red) associated with the rs10801152 genotype in the left anterior insular cortex (circle).
B, The axial section and bar graphs show activation differences associated with the rs10801152 genotype in the left amygdala. Red areas indicate the voxels within the amygdala and insular cortex that differ by genotype;
error bars, SD.

The rs10801152 genotype effect—marginal signal difference (face shape)—as a function of genotype adjusted for magnet strength and ancestral proportion. A, The axial section and bar graphs show activation differences (in red) associated with the rs10801152 genotype in the left anterior insular cortex (circle). B, The axial section and bar graphs show activation differences associated with the rs10801152 genotype in the left amygdala. Red areas indicate the voxels within the amygdala and insular cortex that differ by genotype; error bars, SD.

eFigure. 
Results of analysis of covariance with magnet and rs4606 genotype as the main variables and ancestral-informative markers 1 and 2 as covariates. All values for the bar graphs are corrected for the covariates. The risk allele rs4606-G was associated with increased activation during the faces relative to the sensorimotor control condition in all areas identified in the pooled magnet analyses described in the text. Error bars indicate SD.

Results of analysis of covariance with magnet and rs4606 genotype as the main variables and ancestral-informative markers 1 and 2 as covariates. All values for the bar graphs are corrected for the covariates. The risk allele rs4606-G was associated with increased activation during the faces relative to the sensorimotor control condition in all areas identified in the pooled magnet analyses described in the text. Error bars indicate SD.

eTable 1. Multiple Regression Analyses of the rs4606 Genotype for the Constrained Regions of Interest in the Amygdala and Left and Right Insular Cortex
eTable 1. Multiple Regression Analyses of the rs4606 Genotype for the Constrained Regions of Interest in the Amygdala and Left and Right Insular Cortex
Table 1. 
Association of Behavioral Inhibition to the Unfamiliar With Markers Spanning the RGS2 Locus: Single Marker Results
Association of Behavioral Inhibition to the Unfamiliar With Markers Spanning the RGS2 Locus: Single Marker Results
Table 2. 
Association of Behavioral Inhibition to the Unfamiliar With Markers Spanning the RGS2 Locus: Haplotype Results (>5% Frequency) With Permuted P Values
Association of Behavioral Inhibition to the Unfamiliar With Markers Spanning the RGS2 Locus: Haplotype Results (>5% Frequency) With Permuted P Values
Table 3. 
Single Marker and Haplotype Association of 4 RGS2 Markers With Introversion
Single Marker and Haplotype Association of 4 RGS2 Markers With Introversion
eTable 2. Multiple Regression Analyses of the rs10801152 Genotype for the Constrained Regions of Interest in the Amygdala and the Left and Right Insular Cortex Defined by the rs4606 Results
eTable 2. Multiple Regression Analyses of the rs10801152 Genotype for the Constrained Regions of Interest in the Amygdala and the Left and Right Insular Cortex Defined by the rs4606 Results
1.
Smoller  JWTsuang  M Panic and phobic anxiety: defining phenotypes for genetic studies.  Am J Psychiatry 1998;155 (9) 1152- 1162PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Kaabi  BGelernter  JWoods  SWGoddard  APage  GPElston  RC Genome scan for loci predisposing to anxiety disorders using a novel multivariate approach: strong evidence for a chromosome 4 risk locus.  Am J Hum Genet 2006;78 (4) 543- 553PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Smoller  JWAcierno  JS  JrRosenbaum  JFBiederman  JPollack  MHMeminger  SPava  JAChadwick  LHWhite  CBulzacchelli  MSlaugenhaupt  SA Targeted genome screen of panic disorder and anxiety disorder proneness using homology to murine QTL regions.  Am J Med Genet 2001;105 (2) 195- 206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Flint  J Analysis of quantitative trait loci that influence animal behavior.  J Neurobiol 2003;54 (1) 46- 77PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Yalcin  BWillis-Owen  SAFullerton  JMeesaq  ADeacon  RMRawlins  JNCopley  RRMorris  APFlint  JMott  R Genetic dissection of a behavioral quantitative trait locus shows that Rgs2 modulates anxiety in mice.  Nat Genet 2004;36 (11) 1197- 1202PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Neubig  RRSiderovski  DP Regulators of G-protein signalling as new central nervous system drug targets.  Nat Rev Drug Discov 2002;1 (3) 187- 197PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kagan  J Galen's Prophecy.  New York, NY BasicBooks1994;
8.
Flint  JCorley  RDeFries  JCFulker  DWGray  JAMiller  SCollins  AC A simple genetic basis for a complex psychological trait in laboratory mice.  Science 1995;269 (5229) 1432- 1435PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Schwartz  CEWright  CIShin  LMKagan  JRauch  SL Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grown up”: adult amygdalar response to novelty.  Science 2003;300 (5627) 1952- 1953PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Stein  MBSimmons  ANFeinstein  JSPaulus  MP Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects.  Am J Psychiatry 2007;164 (2) 318- 327PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Phan  KLFitzgerald  DANathan  PJTancer  ME Association between amygdala hyperactivity to harsh faces and severity of social anxiety in generalized social phobia.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (5) 424- 429PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Stein  MBGoldin  PRSareen  JZorrilla  LTBrown  GG Increased amygdala activation to angry and contemptuous faces in generalized social phobia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59 (11) 1027- 1034PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Rosenbaum  JFBiederman  JHirshfeld-Becker  DRKagan  J A controlled study of behavioral inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and depression.  Am J Psychiatry 2000;157 (12) 2002- 2010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Smoller  JWRosenbaum  JBiederman  JKennedy  JDai  DRacette  SRLaird  NMKagan  JSnidman  NHirshfeld-Becker  DTsuang  MTSklar  PBSlaugenhaupt  SA Association of a genetic marker at the corticotropin-releasing hormone locus with behavioral inhibition.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;54 (12) 1376- 1381PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Costa  PTMcCrae  RR NEO-PI-R Professional Manual.  Odessa, FL Psychological Assessment Resources1992;
16.
Paulus  MPFeinstein  JSCastillo  GSimmons  ANStein  MB Dose-dependent decrease of activation in bilateral amygdala and insula by lorazepam during emotion processing.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62 (3) 282- 288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Hariri  ARMattay  VSTessitore  AKolachana  BFera  FGoldman  DEgan  MFWeinberger  DR Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala.  Science 2002;297 (5580) 400- 403PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Cooney  REAtlas  LYJoormann  JEugene  FGotlib  IH Amygdala activation in the processing of neutral faces in social anxiety disorder: is neutral really neutral?  Psychiatry Res 2006;148 (1) 55- 59PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Semplicini  ALenzini  LSartori  MPapparella  ICalo  LAPagnin  EStrapazzon  GBenna  CCosta  RAvogaro  ACeolotto  GPessina  AC Reduced expression of regulator of G-protein signaling 2 (RGS2) in hypertensive patients increases calcium mobilization and ERK1/2 phosphorylation induced by angiotensin II.  J Hypertens 2006;24 (6) 1115- 1124PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Yang  BZZhao  HKranzler  HRGelernter  J Practical population group assignment with selected informative markers: characteristics and properties of Bayesian clustering via STRUCTURE.  Genet Epidemiol 2005;28 (4) 302- 312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Cox  RW AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.  Comput Biomed Res 1996;29 (3) 162- 173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Cohen  MS Parametric analysis of fMRI data using linear systems methods.  Neuroimage 1997;6 (2) 93- 103PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Friston  KJFrith  CDTurner  RFrackowiak  RS Characterizing evoked hemodynamics with fMRI.  Neuroimage 1995;2 (2) 157- 165PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Lancaster  JLWoldorff  MGParsons  LMLiotti  MFreitas  CSRainey  LKochunov  PVNickerson  DMikiten  SAFox  PT Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping.  Hum Brain Mapp 2000;10 (3) 120- 131PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Kagan  JReznick  SSnidman  N Biological bases of childhood shyness.  Science 1988;240 (4849) 167- 171PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Biederman  JHirshfeld-Becker  DRRosenbaum  JFHérot  CFriedman  DSnidman  NKagan  JFaraone  SV Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children.  Am J Psychiatry 2001;158 (10) 1673- 1679PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Robinson  JLKagan  JReznick  JSCorley  R The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: a twin study.  Dev Psychol 1992;28 (6) 1030- 1037Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Jang  KLLivesley  WJVernon  PA Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: a twin study.  J Pers 1996;64 (3) 577- 591PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Caspi  AHarrington  HMilne  BAmell  JWTheodore  RFMoffitt  TE Children's behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26.  J Pers 2003;71 (4) 495- 513PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Bienvenu  OJNestadt  GSamuels  JFCosta  PTHoward  WTEaton  WW Phobic, panic, and major depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality.  J Nerv Ment Dis 2001;189 (3) 154- 161PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Phan  KLWager  TDTaylor  SFLiberzon  I Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions.  CNS Spectr 2004;9 (4) 258- 266PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Killgore  WDYurgelun-Todd  DA Social anxiety predicts amygdala activation in adolescents viewing fearful faces.  Neuroreport 2005;16 (15) 1671- 1675PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Paulus  MPStein  MB An insular view of anxiety.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;60 (4) 383- 387PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Drabant  EMHariri  ARMeyer-Lindenberg  AMunoz  KEMattay  VSKolachana  BSEgan  MFWeinberger  DR Catechol O-methyltransferase val158met genotype and neural mechanisms related to affective arousal and regulation.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (12) 1396- 1406PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Pezawas  LMeyer-Lindenberg  ADrabant  EMVerchinski  BAMunoz  KEKolachana  BSEgan  MFMattay  VSHariri  ARWeinberger  DR 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.  Nat Neurosci 2005;8 (6) 828- 834PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Meyer-Lindenberg  AWeinberger  DR Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders.  Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7 (10) 818- 827PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Ingi  TAoki  Y Expression of RGS2, RGS4 and RGS7 in the developing postnatal brain.  Eur J Neurosci 2002;15 (5) 929- 936PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ingi  TKrumins  AMChidiac  PBrothers  GMChung  SSnow  BEBarnes  CALanahan  AASiderovski  DPRoss  EMGilman  AG Dynamic regulation of RGS2 suggests a novel mechanism in G-protein signaling and neuronal plasticity.  J Neurosci 1998;18 (18) 7178- 7188PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Ghavami  AHunt  RAOlsen  MAZhang  JSmith  DLKalgaonkar  SRahman  ZYoung  KH Differential effects of regulator of G protein signaling (RGS) proteins on serotonin 5-HT1A, 5-HT2A, and dopamine D2 receptor-mediated signaling and adenylyl cyclase activity.  Cell Signal 2004;16 (6) 711- 721PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Han  JMark  MDLi  XXie  MWaka  SRettig  JHerlitze  S RGS2 determines short-term synaptic plasticity in hippocampal neurons by regulating G(i/o)-mediated inhibition of presynaptic Ca(2+) channels.  Neuron 2006;51 (5) 575- 586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Oliveira-Dos-Santos  AJMatsumoto  GSnow  BEBai  DHouston  FPWhishaw  IQMariathasan  SSaski  TWakeham  AOhaski  PSRoder  JCBarnes  CASiderovski  DPPenninger  JM Regulation of T cell activation, anxiety, and male aggression by RGS2.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2000;97 (22) 12272- 12277PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Gross  VTank  JObst  M  et al.  Autonomic nervous system and blood pressure regulation in RGS2-deficient mice.  Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2005;288 (5) R1134- R1142PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Riddle  ELRana  BKMurthy  KKPlehm  RBlumer  KJDiedrich  AJordan  JLuft  FC Polymorphisms and haplotypes of the regulator of G protein signaling-2 gene in normotensives and hypertensives.  Hypertension 2006;47 (3) 415- 420PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Gladstone  GParker  G Measuring a behaviorally inhibited temperament style: development and initial validation of new self-report measures.  Psychiatry Res 2005;135 (2) 133- 143PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Pérez-Edgar  KRoberson-Nay  RHardin  MGPoeth  KGuyer  AENelson  EEMcClure  EBHenderson  HAFox  NAPine  DSErnst  M Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents.  Neuroimage 2007;35 (4) 1538- 1546PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hayward  CKillen  JKraemer  HTaylor  C Linking self-reported childhood behavioral inhibition to adolescent social phobia.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998;37 (12) 1308- 1316PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Gladstone  GLParker  GBMitchell  PBWilhelm  KAMalhi  GS Relationship between self-reported childhood behavioral inhibition and lifetime anxiety disorders in a clinical sample.  Depress Anxiety 2005;22 (3) 103- 113PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Hirshfeld-Becker  DRBiederman  JHenin  AFaraone  SVDavis  SHarrington  KRosenbaum  JF Behavioral inhibition in preschool children at risk is a specific predictor of middle childhood social anxiety: a five-year follow-up.  J Dev Behav Pediatr 2007;28 (3) 225- 233PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Schwartz  CESnidman  NKagan  J Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999;38 (8) 1008- 1015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Bienvenu  OJSamuels  JFCosta  PTReti  IMEaton  WWNestadt  G Anxiety and depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality: a higher- and lower-order personality trait investigation in a community sample.  Depress Anxiety 2004;20 (2) 92- 97PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Lorberbaum  JPKose  SJohnson  MRArana  GWSullivan  LKHamner  MBBallenger  JCLydiard  RBBrodrick  PSBohning  DEGeorge  MS Neural correlates of speech anticipatory anxiety in generalized social phobia.  Neuroreport 2004;15 (18) 2701- 2705PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Fullerton  JCubin  MTiwari  HWang  CBomhra  ADavidson  SMiller  SFairburn  CGoodwin  GNeale  MCFiddy  SMott  RAllison  DBFlint  J Linkage analysis of extremely discordant and concordant sibling pairs identifies quantitative-trait loci that influence variation in the human personality trait neuroticism.  Am J Hum Genet 2003;72 (4) 879- 890PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Shifman  SBhomra  ASmiley  SWray  NRJames  MRMartin  NGHettema  JMAn  SSNeale  MCvan den Oord  EFKendler  KSChen  XBoomsma  DIMiddeldorp  CMHottenga  JJSlagboom  PEFlint  J A whole genome association study of neuroticism using DNA pooling [published ahead of print July 21, 2007].  Mol Psychiatry 2008;13 (3) 302- 31210.1038/sj.mp.4002048PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Bienvenu  OJBrown  CSamuels  JFLiang  KYCosta  PTEaton  WWNestadt  G Normal personality traits and comorbidity among phobic, panic and major depressive disorders.  Psychiatry Res 2001;102 (1) 73- 85PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Hummelen  BWilberg  TPedersen  GKarterud  S The relationship between avoidant personality disorder and social phobia.  Compr Psychiatry 2007;48 (4) 348- 356PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Hettema  JMPrescott  CAKendler  KS Genetic and environmental sources of covariation between generalized anxiety disorder and neuroticism.  Am J Psychiatry 2004;161 (9) 1581- 1587PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Kendler  KSGardner  COGatz  MPedersen  NL The sources of co-morbidity between major depression and generalized anxiety disorder in a Swedish national twin sample.  Psychol Med 2007;37 (3) 453- 462PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Stein  MBSchork  NJGelernter  J A polymorphism of the beta1-adrenergic receptor is associated with low extraversion.  Biol Psychiatry 2004;56 (4) 217- 224PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Leygraf  AHohoff  CFreitag  CWillis-Owen  SAKrakowitzky  PFritze  JFranke  PBandelow  BFimmers  RFlint  JDeckert  J Rgs 2 gene polymorphisms as modulators of anxiety in humans?  J Neural Transm 2006;113 (12) 1921- 1925PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
Original Article
March 2008

Influence of RGS2 on Anxiety-Related Temperament, Personality, and Brain Function

Author Affiliations

Author Affiliations: Psychiatric Genetics Program in Mood and Anxiety Disorders (Dr Smoller and Ms Yamaki), Psychiatric and Neurodevelopmental Genetics Unit, Center for Human Genetic Research (Drs Smoller and Purcell, Mr Fagerness, and Ms Yamaki), and Department of Psychiatry (Drs Smoller, Purcell, Hirshfeld-Becker, Biederman, and Rosenbaum, Mr Fagerness, and Ms Yamaki), Massachusetts General Hospital, Harvard Medical School, Boston; Departments of Psychiatry (Drs Paulus and Stein) and Family and Preventive Medicine (Dr Stein), University of California, San Diego, and VA San Diego Healthcare System; and Departments of Psychiatry, Genetics, and Neurobiology, Division of Human Genetics in Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Connecticut (Dr Gelernter).

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(3):298-308. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.48
Abstract

Context  Although anxiety disorders are heritable, their genetic and phenotypic complexity has made the identification of susceptibility genes difficult. Well-validated animal models and intermediate phenotypes provide crucial tools for genetic dissection of anxiety. The gene encoding regulator of G protein signaling 2 (Rgs2) is a quantitative trait gene that influences mouse anxiety behavior, making its human ortholog (RGS2) a compelling candidate gene for human anxiety phenotypes.

Objective  To examine whether variation in RGS2 is associated with intermediate phenotypes for human anxiety disorders.

Design  Family-based and case-control association analysis of single-nucleotide polymorphisms at the RGS2 locus in 3 independent samples.

Setting  Massachusetts General Hospital, University of California, San Diego, and San Diego State University.

Participants  Study participants included a family-based sample (n = 119 families) of children who underwent laboratory-based assessments of temperament (behavioral inhibition), a sample of 744 unrelated adults who completed assessments of extraversion and introversion, and 55 unrelated adults who underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging measures of response to emotional faces.

Main Outcome Measures  Laboratory-based behavioral measures of childhood temperament, self-report measure of personality, and functional magnetic resonance imaging response to emotion processing.

Results  Markers spanning RGS2 were associated with childhood behavioral inhibition, a temperamental precursor of social anxiety disorder (haplotype P = 3 × 10−5; odds ratio, 2.99 in complete trios). In independent samples, RGS2 markers, including rs4606, which has previously been associated with RGS2 expression, were also associated with introversion (a core personality trait in social anxiety disorder) and with increased limbic activation (insular cortex and amygdala) during emotion processing (brain phenotypes correlated with social anxiety). The genotype at rs4606 explained 10% to 15% of the variance in amygdala and insular cortex activation to emotional faces.

Conclusions  These results provide the first evidence that a gene that influences anxiety in mice is associated with intermediate phenotypes for human anxiety disorders across multiple levels of assessment, including childhood temperament, adult personality, and brain function. This translational research suggests that some genetic influences on anxiety are evolutionarily conserved and that pharmacologic modulation of RGS2 function may provide a novel therapeutic approach for anxiety disorders.

Anxiety disorders are the most common class of psychiatric disorders and are associated with a substantial burden of illness and more than $80 billion in annual costs to society. Anxiety disorders are known to be familial and heritable,1 but the identification of susceptibility genes has been difficult to accomplish for several reasons. The etiology of these disorders is thought to reflect the effects of multiple genes of individually modest effect interacting with environmental factors. Limited understanding of the underlying neurobiological mechanism means that many genes are plausible risk candidates, but few are compelling. Beyond this genetic and biological complexity, considerable phenotypic complexity exists for anxiety disorders. Although the constellations of symptoms used as diagnostic criteria in the DSM-IV have been useful for clinical practice, it is unlikely that they are the optimal phenotype definitions for genetic analyses.1 Family, twin, and linkage studies1-3 suggest that genes confer susceptibility to anxiety proneness in a manner that cuts across clinical diagnostic labels. Although identifying anxiety susceptibility genes is a formidable challenge, well-validated animal models and intermediate phenotypes, including anxious temperament and functional neuroimaging phenotypes, provide crucial tools.

A quantitative trait locus on mouse chromosome 1 has been the most widely replicated locus linked to anxious temperament phenotypes in mice.4 Yalcin et al5 fine-mapped this locus and identified the gene encoding regulator of G protein signaling 2 (Rgs2) as a quantitative trait gene underlying this linkage signal. Rgs2 knockout mice exhibit increased anxiety and fear behavior, altered hippocampal synaptic plasticity, and elevated sympathetic tone. The Rgs2 protein, which is expressed in cortical and limbic brain regions, is part of a family of proteins that accelerate deactivation of G proteins to reduce G protein–coupled receptor (GPCR) signaling.6 Neurotransmitters strongly implicated in the biological basis of anxiety, including serotonin and norepinephrine, act at GPCRs. We previously observed modest evidence of linkage between markers that encompass RGS2 and a phenotype of anxiety disorder proneness in a targeted genome screen.3 Thus, convergent evidence implicates RGS2 as a compelling candidate locus underlying anxiety proneness in humans. We examined whether variation at the RGS2 locus influences intermediate phenotypes for anxiety disorder at the level of behavior and brain function. We first examined the association of RGS2 markers with behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar (BI), an anxiety-related form of temperament characterized by a tendency to be shy, avoidant, and behaviorally restrained in situations that are novel or unfamiliar.7 Mouse models of unconditioned and novelty-induced fear responses closely parallel behavioral and biological features of human BI, including inhibition of behavior and increased sympathetic nervous system reactivity.8 We next examined the BI-related adult personality trait of introversion (low extraversion), which is also characterized by inhibition and avoidant behavior and, finally, limbic responses to emotional faces on functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), a neuroimaging phenotype linked to BI,9 anxiety proneness,10 and social anxiety disorder (SAD).11,12

Methods
Sample
Behavioral Inhibition Sample

Participants were recruited from a sample of families who had participated in a study of children at risk for anxiety disorders conducted at Massachusetts General Hospital. Details of the study sample and behavioral assessments are provided elsewhere.13 Children from these families were classified as “inhibited” or “not inhibited” on the basis of a behavioral assessment conducted at the ages of 21 months, 4 years, or 6 years. A total of 119 families that included at least 1 child who had undergone behavioral assessments were available at the time of these analyses. The self-reported race of all but 9 families was white. The protocol was approved by the Massachusetts General Hospital institutional review board. After complete description of the study, parents provided written informed consent for themselves and their children, who also provided oral or written assent.

Personality Sample

Participants (n = 744; 516 female) were recruited from among undergraduate psychology students at San Diego State University (SDSU). Participants had blood drawn for genetic studies and completed questionnaires. Participants gave informed written consent to participate in this part of the study, which was approved by the Human Research Protection Programs at both the SDSU and University of California, San Diego. Participants received $25 for providing the blood sample. Power estimates indicate that the sample provides more than 85% power to detect an association at α <.05 with a quantitative trait locus explaining as little as 1.5% of the trait variance.

Neuroimaging Sample

Participants. Initially, approximately 3000 SDSU undergraduate students participated in screening for a behavioral experiment in return for course credits. Of those individuals who participated in the behavioral study, approximately 1 of 3 expressed a willingness to participate in an fMRI study; an estimated 1 of 2 provided consent to be contacted for further assessment, and 1 of 2 of these proved eligible. We obtained 2 different samples, which had been collected during another ongoing project. During this time, our imaging studies shifted from a 1.5-T scanner (Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) to a 3.0-T scanner (Signa EXCITE; GE Healthcare, Milwaukee, Wisconsin). Sample 1 (1.5-T scanner) consisted of 29 healthy, right-handed individuals (17 women) with a mean (SD) age of 18.2 (0.62) years with a mean (SD) educational level of 12.6 (0.60) years. Sample 2 (3.0-T scanner) consisted of 26 healthy, right-handed individuals (24 women) with a mean (SD) age of 21.0 (2.6) years with a mean (SD) educational level of 14.5 (1.4) years. All study participants underwent the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV to identify anxiety and mood disorders and were excluded if they were currently seeking, or had ever sought, treatment for their anxiety or mood symptoms. None of the participants had taken any psychotropic medications in the prior 12 months. Participants consumed less than 400 mg of caffeine daily. This study was approved by the SDSU and University of California, San Diego, institutional review boards. All participants gave their informed, written consent to participate and perform the emotion face-processing task during fMRI.

Behavioral Temperament Assessment. As described previously,13 children underwent laboratory-based temperament assessments at 1 of 3 ages (21 months, 4 years, or 6 years) using age-specific measurement protocols. In brief, the evaluation consisted of behavioral protocols designed to assess the child's reaction to unfamiliar persons and events during a 90-minute battery. In these protocols, the child, with the mother present, encountered a variety of unfamiliar procedures and tasks, including physiologic measurements and cognitively challenging tasks administered by an unfamiliar female examiner, and their behavioral responses were observed and quantified. The assessments were videotaped and scored by raters who were blind to the assessment of psychopathologic conditions in the children and their parents and blind to genotype status. The relevant dependent variables were behavioral signs of uncertainty, including fretting and crying, cessation of vocalization or activity, retreat or withdrawal from an unfamiliar event, and frequency of smiles and spontaneous comments (see Rosenbaum et al13 for full description of coded variables). Studies conducted during the past 20 years have established that these variables differentiate inhibited from uninhibited children between the ages of 1 year and 8 years. As in our previous genetic studies of BI, children were classified as inhibited if they met at least 1 of 3 prespecified categorical definitions of BI14; children who met none of these definitions were considered unaffected. The genotyped families included 73 children with BI and 89 children without BI (total, 162 children). The sample included 77 two-parent families that comprised 114 trios, 41 single-parent families (including 17 with more than 1 genotyped offspring and 6 with phenotyped sibling pairs), and 1 sibling pair with no parental genotypes.

Self-report Personality Assessment. The NEO-Personality Inventory–Revised is a widely used, 240-item (plus 3 validity items) self-report measure of personality, grouped into 5 major domains: neuroticism (N), extraversion (E), openness to experience (O), conscientiousness (C), and agreeableness (A).15 Some individuals completed a shorter (60-item) version of the NEO, the NEO-Five Factor Inventory,15 which provides domain scores (expressed as T scores) that are highly correlated with those obtained from the full instrument. T scores were calculated directly from college-age, sex-specific norms. The phenotype of interest was extraversion T scores.

Functional mri
Task

During fMRI, each participant was tested with a slightly modified16 version of the emotion face assessment task (see Hariri et al17). During each 5-second trial, a participant is presented with a target face (on the top of the computer screen) and 2 probe faces (on the bottom of the screen) and is instructed to match the probe with the same emotional expression to the target by pressing the left or right key on a button box. A block consists of 6 consecutive trials during which the target face is angry, fearful, or happy. During the sensorimotor control task, individuals were presented with 5-second trials of either wide or tall ovals or circles in an analogous configuration and instructed to match the shape of the probe to the target. We did not use neutral faces as a comparator condition because there is mounting evidence that neutral faces are not actually processed as neutral.18 Each block of faces and of the sensorimotor control task was presented 3 times in a pseudorandomized order. A fixation cross that lasted 8 seconds was interspersed between each block presented at the beginning and end of the task (resulting in 14 fixation periods). For each trial, response accuracy and reaction time data were obtained. There were 18 trials (3 blocks of 6 trials) for each face set and for shapes. The whole task lasted 512 seconds (matching the scan length).

Image Acquisition (1.5 T)

During the task, 1 blood oxygenation level–dependent (BOLD) fMRI run was collected for each study participant using a 1.5-T scanner (Siemens; T2-weighted echo planar imaging; repetition time [TR], 2000 milliseconds; echo time [TE], 40 milliseconds; 64 × 64 matrix; 20 4-mm axial sections; 256 repetitions). During the same experimental session, a T1-weighted image (MPRAGE; TR, 11.4 milliseconds; TE, 4.4 milliseconds; flip angle, 10°; field of vision [FOV], 256 × 256; 1-mm3 voxels) was obtained for anatomical reference. For preprocessing, voxel time series were interpolated to correct for nonsimultaneous section acquisition within each volume and corrected for 3-dimensional motion.

Image Acquisition (3 T)

During the task, an fMRI run sensitive to BOLD contrast was collected for each participant using a 3.0-T scanner (Signa EXCITE) (T2-weighted echo planar imaging; TR, 2000 milliseconds; TE, 32 milliseconds; FOV, 250 × 250 mm3; 64 × 64 matrix; 30 2.6-mm axial sections with a 1.4-mm gap; 290 scans). The fMRI acquisitions were time-locked to the onset of each trial. During the same experimental session, a high-resolution T1-weighted image (spoiled gradient recalled; T1 relaxation time, 450; TR, 8 milliseconds; TE, 4 milliseconds; flip angle, 12°; FOV, 256 × 256; 1-mm3 voxels) was obtained for anatomical reference.

Genetic Methods

Selection of Single-Nucleotide Polymorphisms. For the analysis of BI temperament, single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were selected to capture genetic variation across the RGS2 locus. The SNPs were selected from a genomic region that comprised the RGS2 gene, 25 kb of the 5′ flanking sequence, and 10 kb of the 3′ flanking sequence as defined in genome build hg17 using the phase 1 International HapMap Web site (http://www.hapmap.org). Ten tagging SNPs were selected from this region using Tagger (http://www.broad.mit.edu/mpg/tagger/) with r2 > 0.8 and the “aggressive” tagging algorithm. We also included 5 SNPs in the gene (rs2746071, rs2746073, rs17647363, rs4606, and rs3767488) that were not available in HapMap at the time of selection but were found in dbSNP (http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/SNP). Linkage disequilibrium relationships within the SNP set were calculated using the Gabriel criteria as implemented in Haploview (http://www.broad.mit.edu/mpg/haploview/index.php). A single haplotype block encompassed the gene and all markers with the exception of the 5′ marker rs3856223, which nevertheless was in strong linkage disequilibrium with markers in the block and was thus included in the haplotype analysis. The final set of 15 SNPs achieved an average density of 1 SNP per 2 kb across a 32-kb region and an average r2 of 0.90 with untyped HapMap markers that have minor allele frequency of 10% or more, indicating excellent coverage of variation across the locus. Because our marker set included 5 SNPs not included in HapMap, this average r2 is an underestimate of the true variation captured by our set. For the NEO-E analyses, we selected a subset of the 15 markers based on the results of the BI analysis, and for the fMRI analyses, we selected rs4606 because of the recent report of its functional significance.19

Genotyping Method. Genotyping of SNPs in the BI family sample was performed by mass spectrometry (Sequenom, San Diego, California). Markers were retained for analysis if they met the following criteria: (1) no significant deviation from Hardy Weinberg equilibrium (P > .01) and (2) minimum call rate of 85% (average call rate, 96.4%). For 1 SNP (rs4606), genotypes were repeated and the resulting genotypes combined (overall call rate, 98%). All 15 markers were thus retained. Affected and unaffected individuals were spread across genotyping plates to avoid bias due to plate-specific genotyping error.

Genotyping for the NEO-E and fMRI Samples. The SNPs were genotyped with a fluorogenic 5′ nuclease assay method (ie, the TaqMan technique) using the ABI PRISM 7900 Sequence Detection System (ABI, Foster City, California). All genotypes were assayed in duplicate, and discordant genotypes (which ranged from 0% to 0.3%, depending on the marker) were discarded. All markers were in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium.

Ancestral Proportion Scoresw. The ancestries of the study participants were estimated using a set of unlinked genetic markers by Bayesian cluster analysis, using STRUCTURE software (http://pritch.bsd.uchicago.edu/software.html). The markers were the set of short tandem repeats selected for ancestry information and described previously.20 STRUCTURE implements Bayesian cluster modeling that can infer population genetic patterns without prior information of population origins. The model was specified as “admixture” and “allele frequencies correlated,” with 100 000 burn-in and 100 000 Markov chain Monte Carlo iterations. Analysis with STRUCTURE indicated adequate fit for a 3-class solution, which was used in the NEO-E and fMRI analyses.

Statistical analyses
Family-Based Association Analysis of Temperament

Family-based association analyses of the BI sample were performed using the Family Based Association Test (FBAT) Program 1.7.1 (http://www.biostat.harvard.edu/~fbat/default.html). The offset option of FBAT (using an offset equal to the sample prevalence) was used to incorporate all offspring who were phenotyped and genotyped. Haplotype-specific and global haplotype tests were performed using permutation (N = 100 000 cycles) by the hbat-p option. The min-p test was used to calculate a global haplotype test. This test evaluates the statistical significance of the smallest observed P value among all the individual haplotypes and estimates a P value by permutation. The odds ratio associated with the risk haplotype was calculated from complete trios using WHAP (http://pngu.mgh.harvard.edu/purcell/whap/).

NEO-E (Introversion) Association

Single-marker and haplotype-based analyses of the NEO-E quantitative trait were performed using WHAP, with empirical P values determined by permutation testing. Sample mean and variance were fixed to optimize model stability in single-marker and haplotype analyses. Analyses incorporated a 3-class population solution derived from STRUCTURE to avoid confounding due to population stratification.

Our analytic strategy was based on sequentially maximizing the prior probability of association and reducing multiple testing by first testing the full set of markers for the BI phenotype, retaining SNPs with the strongest signals for the introversion analysis, and then focusing on the most relevant markers for the fMRI analyses. The fMRI analysis focused on rs4606 in particular because it is the marker that has been associated with gene expression levels. To further support these results and confirm they were not a function of genotyping error, we also examined rs10801152 (the SNP that showed the strongest statistical evidence across the BI and introversion analyses).

fMRI Analysis

All structural and functional image processing was performed with the Analysis of Functional Neuroimages (AFNI) software package.21 Echoplanar intensity images were coregistered to the 128th image using a 3-dimensional coregistration algorithm. The time series of the alignments in the x, y, z and roll, pitch, yaw direction was used to obtain motion regressors for each study participant. Because small motion corrections are similar in angle (eg, roll) and displacement (eg, x), we used only 3 motion parameters (roll, pitch, yaw) as nuisance regressors to account for motion artifacts. The 4 orthogonal regressors of interest were (1) happy, (2) angry, (3) fearful, and (4) circle or oval (ie, shape) sensorimotor condition. These regressors were convolved with a modified γ variate function to account for the delay and the dispersion brain response of the BOLD-fMRI signal due to hemodynamics.22,23 Additional regressors were used to model residual motion in the roll, pitch, and yaw directions and baseline and linear trends. The AFNI program 3dDeconvolve was used to calculate the estimated voxel-wise response amplitude. A gaussian filter with a full width at half maximum of 4 mm was applied to the voxelwise percentage of signal change data to account for individual variations in the anatomical landmarks.

Data from each participant were normalized to Talairach coordinates. Whole-brain analyses were followed by a priori analysis of regions of interest using masks (defined by the Talairach demon atlas)24 in the bilateral amygdala, insular cortices, ventromedial prefrontal cortex, and primary visual cortex. On the basis of these areas of interest, it was determined via simulations that a voxel-wise a priori probability of .05 would result in a corrected clusterwise activation probability of .05 if a minimum volume of 128 μL and 2 connected voxels (in the amygdala, which is a small structure) or 512 μL and 8 connected voxels (in all other regions of interest) was considered. The areas of interest were superimposed on each individual's voxelwise percentage of signal change brain image. Only activations within the areas of interest, which also satisfied the volume and voxel connection criteria, were extracted and used for further analysis. The corrected voxelwise probabilities are as follows: amygdala, P < .012; insular cortex, P < .000069; medial prefrontal cortex, P < .00014; and visual cortex, P < .000070. These corrected voxel probabilities are based on Monte Carlo simulations using AFNI's program AlphaSim using the filtered data and the a priori defined regions of interest.

To maximize power by increasing the number of individuals homozygous for the putative rs4606 risk (G) allele, we pooled the 1.5-T and 3-T data sets rather than considering them separately. Specifically, for these analyses we used a regression approach with magnet type (1.5 T or 3 T) and ancestral informative markers (AIM 1 and AIM 2) coefficients as covariates and genotype for rs4606 (CC = 0, CG = 1, GG = 2) as the variable of interest. Areas with a significant gene effect (ie, voxelwise partial correlation coefficient with P < .05) that also fulfilled the volume-threshold cluster condition within the regions of interest were extracted, and additional statistical analyses were conducted using SPSS statistical software, version 15.0 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Illinois). Results of the regression analyses are presented in eTable 1.

Results
ASSOCIATION OF RGS2 WITH CHILDHOOD ANXIOUS TEMPERAMENT

We first examined whether RGS2 influences behavioral and neurobiological phenotypes underlying human anxiety by examining a form of human anxious temperament that shares core phenotypic and biologic features with mouse models of unconditioned and novelty-induced fear behavior. Behavioral inhibition to the unfamiliar is a heritable temperamental profile characterized by a tendency to be shy, avoidant, and behaviorally restrained in situations that are novel or unfamiliar.25 Biological features of BI include evidence of increased sympathetic tone and limbic hyperreactivity to novel stimuli.9,25 In addition, BI is a familial and developmental risk factor for anxiety disorders (and, in particular, SAD)26 but has greater estimated heritability than the diagnostic categories.27 The sample comprised 119 families in which children underwent standardized laboratory-based behavioral assessments of BI, as previously described.13 To capture genetic variation across the RGS2 locus, we genotyped a set of 15 SNPs with an average density of 1 SNP per 2 kb across a 32-kb region spanning RGS2 (Figure 1). As indicated in Table 1, Table 2, and Figure 2, 9 of the 15 SNPs tested were associated with BI, including the G allele of the 3′ UTR SNP rs4606 (P = .0026), which has been shown to be associated with reduced RGS2 expression in vitro.19 A haplotype that comprised all 15 markers was also associated with BI (permuted P = 3.0 × 10−5). The odds ratio for BI associated with the risk haplotype, calculated from complete family trios, was 2.99 (95% confidence interval, 1.31-6.84).

ASSOCIATION OF RGS2 WITH SOCIAL ANXIETY–RELATED PERSONALITY IN ADULTS

In adults, social inhibition can be indexed by the personality trait of introversion (low extraversion), which, like BI, is a heritable trait28 characterized by low levels of sociability and aversion to large groups. Longitudinal data suggest that childhood BI is a developmental precursor of introversion (but not neuroticism).29 Like BI, introversion is associated with risk for anxiety disorders, including SAD.30 If variants in RGS2 are associated with temperamental shyness, we hypothesized that these variants would also be associated with introversion (low extraversion). We genotyped the 4 markers that showed the strongest signal in the BI sample in an independent sample of 744 college undergraduates (228 men and 516 women) who completed the NEO-Personality Inventory–Revised, from which the extraversion scale (NEO-E) can be derived.15 Consistent with our prediction, we observed an association between NEO-E and the same alleles of these 4 markers that were associated with BI (Table 3). A haplotype of these 4 alleles was also associated with introversion (global and haplotype-specific P = .04). Although our primary hypothesis was that RGS2 would be associated with introversion, we performed secondary analyses to determine whether the effect was specific to this trait. In those analyses, we observed no association between RGS2 markers and the other NEO subscales (neuroticism, openness, conscientiousness, or agreeableness).

ASSOCIATION OF RGS2 WITH SOCIAL ANXIETY–RELATED BRAIN FUNCTION

In light of previous studies that suggest that BI and social anxiety are mediated by hyperreactivity of brain structures (especially amygdala and insular cortices) thought to underlie anxiety proneness,9-12 we hypothesized that RGS2 variants associated with BI and introversion would also show association with functional reactivity of these structures during emotion processing. To investigate this, we examined genotype effects on limbic brain reactivity to emotional faces, a neuroimaging assay of anxious temperament. Previous fMRI studies31 have shown that limbic brain circuits involved in anxiety are activated when individuals view novel or emotional faces. In particular, increased amygdala activation to novel or emotional faces has been associated with inhibited temperament,9 social anxiety traits,32 and SAD,12 although other areas, including the anterior cingulate cortex and the insular cortex, have also been implicated.31,33 By directly indexing brain function, anxiety-related fMRI phenotypes may provide more proximal and therefore more powerful measures of gene action.

For these analyses, we selected the 3′ UTR SNP rs4606, which has been associated with variation in RGS2 messenger RNA expression.19 We genotyped rs4606 in 2 independent groups (29 tested in a 1.5-T magnet and 26 tested in a 3-T magnet) of healthy volunteers drawn from an ongoing study with college-age individuals. To maximize power for analyses of the putative risk allele (rs4606-G), we pooled the samples and included a covariate for the magnets. Individuals in both magnets were tested during fMRI using the same version of a slightly modified emotion face assessment task that has been shown to be sensitive to genetic influence.17 For each 5-second trial, an individual is presented with a target face (on the top of the computer screen) and 2 probe faces (on the bottom of the screen) and is instructed to match the probe with the same emotional expression (happy, sad, or angry) to the target by pressing the left or right key on a button box. During the sensorimotor control task, study participants were presented with 5-second trials of either wide or tall ovals or circles in an analogous configuration and instructed to match the shape of the probe to the target. Several investigators have used this task to show significant activations in the amygdala during the presentation of faces vs the sensorimotor control condition.17,34 Moreover, we have found previously that the degree of insular cortex activation during this task was modulated by both short-term administration of an anxiolytic and by the degree of anxiety proneness.10,16 The rs4606 G allele, which showed association with BI and introversion in the analyses described herein, was significantly associated with the degree of left amygdala and bilateral insular cortex activation (Figure 3, Figure 4, and eTable 1). Specifically, in models that control for magnet (1.5 T vs 3 T) and ancestry-informative marker clusters,20 the rs4606 G allele was independently associated with the extracted average activation of 2 clusters in the left insular cortex (P = .009 and P = .007, respectively) and a cluster in the right insular cortex (P = .005) and left amygdala (P = .02). The rs4606 SNP accounted for approximately 15% of the variance in amygdala activation and approximately 10% to 15% of the variance in the insular cortex activation. To corroborate the finding with rs4606, we also examined rs1081152, which in our analyses showed a strong association with both inhibited temperament and introversion. As with rs4606, we observed significant association of the rs1081152 risk allele (T) with left insular cortex (P = .009) and amygdala (P = .03) activation (Figure 5 and eTable 2). On the basis of the combined volume and voxelwise P value threshold, we did not find any other clusters in the brain associated with the RGS2 SNPs. Finally, in a secondary analysis, we confirmed that the rs4606 genotype effect on insula-amygdala activation was similar in each magnet (1.5 T and 3 T) considered separately (eFigure).

Comment

On the basis of consistent results derived from a set of different but interrelated anxiety paradigms in independent samples, we observed compelling evidence that RGS2, the ortholog of a mouse anxiety quantitative trait gene, is also associated with anxiety-related phenotypes in humans. A particularly strong effect was seen for childhood BI, which closely parallels behavioral and biologic features of mouse phenotypes influenced by murine Rgs2. Our findings are the first, to our knowledge, to document association of a specific gene with social anxiety across 3 levels of phenotypic analysis: a laboratory-based behavioral measure of childhood temperament, a self-report measure of adult personality, and a neuroimaging measure of functional brain activity.

The finding of RGS2-related activation in the amygdala is analogous to similar findings with the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism17 and the catechol O-methyltransferase val-met variant.34 In addition, our results are the first, to our knowledge, to demonstrate an association between the insular cortex, a limbic brain region involved in emotional processing,33 and a gene implicated in anxiety. The insular cortex is part of a neural system involved in homeostatic processing of autonomic arousal and visceral changes, signaling executive areas to initiate avoidant behavior and altering self-awareness.33 The insular cortex, medial prefrontal cortex, and amygdala play crucial roles in linking internal physiologic states to external cues or events. Although some investigators have proposed that the connectivity between the amygdala and the medial prefrontal cortex or anterior cingulate is a critical genetically determined factor, dysfunction of which predisposes individuals to anxiety or depression,35 the limited number of high-risk allele individuals in our sample prevented a rigorous test of this hypothesis using functional connectivity measures. Clearly, future investigation will need to examine genetic determinants of functional connectivity between amygdala or insular cortex and other areas that are important for emotion regulation.36

RGS2 is one of a family of regulators of G protein signaling that function as guanosine triphosphatase (GTPase) accelerating proteins, terminating G protein signaling by binding to activated Gα subunits and accelerating the rate of guanosine triphosphate (GTP) hydrolysis.6RGS2 regulates Gi/o and Gqα and is expressed in brain regions thought to underlie anxiety, including the hippocampus, amygdala, cerebral cortex, hypothalamus, and dorsal raphe nuclei.6,37,38 Neurotransmitters implicated in anxiety, including serotonin, norepinephrine, and dopamine, act at GPCRs. RGS2 has been shown to markedly decrease Gqα signaling by serotonin 2A receptors,39 which, in turn, play a key role in anxiety and stress responses, as well as response to serotonergic antidepressants. RGS2 has also been shown to regulate hippocampal synaptic plasticity by increasing neurotransmitter release via presynaptic Gi/o-mediated Ca2+ channel inhibition.40 Neuronal RGS2 transcription is modulated by plasticity-inducing synaptic stimuli and by agents known to affect anxiety and mood symptoms,6,38 and RGS2 expression has been implicated in experience-dependent development of neural circuits.37Rgs2-deficient mice exhibit increased anxiety behavior,41 increased sympathetic tone, reduced heart rate variability, altered blood pressure response to a novel environment, and increased urinary norepinephrine excretion42—features also reported in human BI. Our results suggest that at least some genetic influences on fear responses to novelty are evolutionarily conserved. The identity of the specific phenotype-influencing variant(s) mediating RGS2 effects on human anxiety phenotypes cannot be determined from these data, although the dense map of SNPs examined in the analysis of BI captures a minimum of 90% of the genetic variation in the region and is likely to have directly or indirectly assayed the relevant variants. Resequencing of the gene in previous studies19,43 has not revealed common coding sequence variants. However, the G allele of rs4606, which was associated with anxiety phenotypes in our study, has been associated with reduced RGS2 expression in both peripheral blood mononuclear cells and fibroblasts in hypertensive patients.19 Reduced RGS2 expression is expected to be associated with anxiety given that deletion of the gene is associated with anxious temperament in mice.5,41

Taken together, our results suggest a model in which genetic variation associated with reduced expression of RGS2 contributes to increased reactivity of limbic brain structures modulating anxious temperament and social anxiety. At a behavioral level, this genetic effect is most evident in direct measurements of inhibited temperament (which itself has been shown to be associated with amygdala reactivity in previous research9), with a weaker effect detectable on adult social anxiety–related personality. This model rests in part on the premise that our measures of temperament, personality, and brain function are phenotypically convergent. One way to verify this would be to measure all 3 phenotypes in the same individuals and examine their relationship; this was not possible because BI is based on laboratory-based behavioral temperament observations in young children (who could not complete the NEO or undergo functional imaging). However, substantial evidence indicates that BI, introversion, and limbic reactivity are, in fact, convergent phenotypes: prior studies have demonstrated an association between BI measured and introversion,29,44 between BI and limbic reactivity to emotional faces,9,45 and among all 3 traits (BI, introversion, limbic reactivity to emotional faces) and SAD.11,12,30,46-51

In secondary analyses that examined the specificity of RGS2 effects on personality, we did not observe association with neuroticism, which has been reported to be linked to the 1q region syntenic with mouse chromosome 1.52 Our data do not support the hypothesis that RGS2 underlies this linkage signal because our adult sample was powered to detect loci explaining as little as 1.5% of the variance in neuroticism. However, the linked region contains many genes, and it may be that 1 or more of these genes contribute to neuroticism. Of note, however, a recent whole genome association study of neuroticism failed to detect any loci at this region.53 To our knowledge, no linkage or association studies that include loci on 1q have examined the phenotype of introversion. Prior studies suggest that introversion is more specifically related to BI and social anxiety,44,50,54,55 whereas neuroticism appears to be a nonspecific risk factor for depression and anxiety disorders (especially generalized anxiety disorder).56,57 Although neuroticism mainly captures negative emotionality and worry, introversion is more directly related to social inhibition and shyness, core features of BI. Caspi et al29 examined temperament in 3-year-old children and followed up these children to adulthood. Inhibited temperament at the age of 3 years was associated with introversion at the age of 26 years but was unrelated to adult neuroticism. Gladstone and Parker44 found that an adult measure of BI was strongly and similarly correlated with introversion (r = 0.75) and social anxiety (r = 0.77). In a subsequent study,47 retrospective childhood BI was significantly associated with social phobia but not panic disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, or agoraphobia. Numerous other studies,26,48,49 including longitudinal studies, have confirmed the specific relationship between BI and social anxiety, and we have previously shown in a college sample that introversion is highly and significantly correlated with measures of shyness and social anxiety.58

Further studies will be needed to determine which, if any, anxiety disorder phenotypes are most tightly related to RGS2. Given the association with BI and introversion, 2 traits that are risk factors for SAD, we would predict that SAD is the most likely anxiety disorder to be associated with RGS2. A recent study by Leygraf et al59 reported nominally significant evidence of association between RGS2 markers and panic disorder, although these results would not survive correction for multiple testing. To the extent that genetic effects on DSM-IV anxiety disorders may be smaller than effects on the intermediate phenotypes examined herein, much larger samples may be needed for studies of the clinical disorders.

In conjunction with studies in mouse models, our findings suggest that RGS2 modulators could provide a novel therapeutic approach for the treatment of anxiety disorders. For example, agents that facilitate RGS2 would be expected to inhibit GPCR signaling in response to neurotransmitters targeted by antidepressants that effectively treat anxiety disorders. The hypothesis that anxiety proneness is related to reduced RGS2 expression implies that agents that enhance RGS2 activity would be anxiolytic. The regional expression of RGS2 in limbic and paralimbic brain areas coupled with its selectivity for Gqα-mediated and Gi/o signaling might enhance the therapeutic action of GPCR-based treatments of anxiety and mood disorders.6

Correspondence: Jordan W. Smoller, MD, ScD, Center for Human Genetic Research, Simches Research Bldg, 185 Cambridge St, Boston, MA 02114 (jsmoller@hms.harvard.edu).

Submitted for Publication: June 25, 2007; final revision received October 10, 2007; accepted October 10, 2007.

Author Contributions: Drs Gelernter and Stein contributed equally to this work.

Financial Disclosure: Dr Smoller has served as a consultant for Eli Lilly and Company; has received honoraria from Hoffmann-La Roche, Inc, Enterprise Analysis Corp, and MPM Capital; and has served on an advisory board for Roche Diagnostics Corporation. A patent application related to work described in this article is pending. Dr Paulus has been a paid consultant for GlaxoSmithKline Pharmaceuticals; has received research support from Forest Laboratories, GlaxoSmithKline, Roche, Sepracor, the National Institute on Drug Abuse (NIDA), the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), and the VA Merit System; and has received speaking honoraria from Pfizer. Dr Biederman currently receives research support from Bristol-Myers Squibb, Eli Lilly and Company, Janssen Pharmaceuticals Inc, McNeil, Otsuka, Shire, the NIMH, and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development; is currently a consultant/advisory board member for Janssen, McNeil, Novartis, and Shire; and currently serves on the speakers bureaus of Janssen, McNeil, Novartis, Shire, and UCB Pharma, Inc. In addition, he has received research support, consultation fees, or speaker's fees from Abbott, AstraZeneca, Celltech, Cephalon, Eli Lilly and Company, Esai, Forest, Gliatech, NARSAD, Neurosearch, New River, the NIDA, Novartis, Noven, Pfizer, Pharmacia, the Prechter Foundation, the Stanley Foundation, and Wyeth. Dr Rosenbaum has served on one or more advisory boards for AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Cyberonics, Eli Lilly and Company, Forest, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, MedAvante, Novartis, Orexigen, Pfizer, Roche Diagnostics, Sanofi, Shire, Sepracor, Somerset, and Wyeth; has offered consultation to Compellis, EPIX, Neuronetics, Organon, Somaxon, and Supernus; and has received one or more honoraria for lectureship from Boehringer-Ingelheim, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cyberonics, Eli Lilly and Company, Forest, and Schwartz. He also attended the Massachusetts General Hospital Psychiatry Academy, sponsored by AstraZeneca, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Eli Lilly and Company, GlaxoSmithKline, Janssen, and Wyeth. He has equity holdings in Compellis, MedAvante, and Somaxon. Dr Stein has been a paid consultant for AstraZeneca, Avera Pharmaceuticals, BrainCells Inc, Bristol-Myers Squibb, Cephalon, Eli Lilly and Company, EPI-Q Inc, Forest Laboratories, GlaxoSmithKline Pharmaceuticals, Hoffmann-La Roche Pharmaceuticals, Integral Health Decisions Inc, Jazz Pharmaceuticals, Johnson & Johnson, Mindsite, Pfizer, Sanofi-Aventis, Solvay Pharmaceuticals, Transcept Pharmaceuticals Inc, UCB Pharma, Virtual Reality Medical Center, and Wyeth and has received research support from Eli Lilly and Company, Forest Laboratories, GlaxoSmithKline, and UCB Pharma.

Funding/Support: This study was supported in part by NIMH grant K-08 MH01770 (Dr Smoller); a NARSAD Young Investigator Award (Dr Smoller); NIMH grant R01MH47077-11(Dr Rosenbaum) and a gift from the Rose and Eugene Kleiner Family Foundation; funds from the US Department of Veterans Affairs (the VA Medical Research Program [Merit Review to Dr Gelernter]); NIDA grants DA12690, DA12849, and DA15105; and NIMH grants K24 MH64122 and R01 MH65413 (Dr Stein).

Additional Contributions: The following assisted in this work: Pamela Sklar, MD, PhD, Lisa Watras, BA, Brian Galloway, BA, Priya Moorjani, MS, Alan Simmons, PhD, Jerome Kagan, PhD, and Nancy Snidman, PhD. We are grateful to the individuals and families who participated in this work.

References
1.
Smoller  JWTsuang  M Panic and phobic anxiety: defining phenotypes for genetic studies.  Am J Psychiatry 1998;155 (9) 1152- 1162PubMedGoogle Scholar
2.
Kaabi  BGelernter  JWoods  SWGoddard  APage  GPElston  RC Genome scan for loci predisposing to anxiety disorders using a novel multivariate approach: strong evidence for a chromosome 4 risk locus.  Am J Hum Genet 2006;78 (4) 543- 553PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
3.
Smoller  JWAcierno  JS  JrRosenbaum  JFBiederman  JPollack  MHMeminger  SPava  JAChadwick  LHWhite  CBulzacchelli  MSlaugenhaupt  SA Targeted genome screen of panic disorder and anxiety disorder proneness using homology to murine QTL regions.  Am J Med Genet 2001;105 (2) 195- 206PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
4.
Flint  J Analysis of quantitative trait loci that influence animal behavior.  J Neurobiol 2003;54 (1) 46- 77PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
5.
Yalcin  BWillis-Owen  SAFullerton  JMeesaq  ADeacon  RMRawlins  JNCopley  RRMorris  APFlint  JMott  R Genetic dissection of a behavioral quantitative trait locus shows that Rgs2 modulates anxiety in mice.  Nat Genet 2004;36 (11) 1197- 1202PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
6.
Neubig  RRSiderovski  DP Regulators of G-protein signalling as new central nervous system drug targets.  Nat Rev Drug Discov 2002;1 (3) 187- 197PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
7.
Kagan  J Galen's Prophecy.  New York, NY BasicBooks1994;
8.
Flint  JCorley  RDeFries  JCFulker  DWGray  JAMiller  SCollins  AC A simple genetic basis for a complex psychological trait in laboratory mice.  Science 1995;269 (5229) 1432- 1435PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
9.
Schwartz  CEWright  CIShin  LMKagan  JRauch  SL Inhibited and uninhibited infants “grown up”: adult amygdalar response to novelty.  Science 2003;300 (5627) 1952- 1953PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
10.
Stein  MBSimmons  ANFeinstein  JSPaulus  MP Increased amygdala and insula activation during emotion processing in anxiety-prone subjects.  Am J Psychiatry 2007;164 (2) 318- 327PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
11.
Phan  KLFitzgerald  DANathan  PJTancer  ME Association between amygdala hyperactivity to harsh faces and severity of social anxiety in generalized social phobia.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;59 (5) 424- 429PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
12.
Stein  MBGoldin  PRSareen  JZorrilla  LTBrown  GG Increased amygdala activation to angry and contemptuous faces in generalized social phobia.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;59 (11) 1027- 1034PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
13.
Rosenbaum  JFBiederman  JHirshfeld-Becker  DRKagan  J A controlled study of behavioral inhibition in children of parents with panic disorder and depression.  Am J Psychiatry 2000;157 (12) 2002- 2010PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
14.
Smoller  JWRosenbaum  JBiederman  JKennedy  JDai  DRacette  SRLaird  NMKagan  JSnidman  NHirshfeld-Becker  DTsuang  MTSklar  PBSlaugenhaupt  SA Association of a genetic marker at the corticotropin-releasing hormone locus with behavioral inhibition.  Biol Psychiatry 2003;54 (12) 1376- 1381PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
15.
Costa  PTMcCrae  RR NEO-PI-R Professional Manual.  Odessa, FL Psychological Assessment Resources1992;
16.
Paulus  MPFeinstein  JSCastillo  GSimmons  ANStein  MB Dose-dependent decrease of activation in bilateral amygdala and insula by lorazepam during emotion processing.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62 (3) 282- 288PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
17.
Hariri  ARMattay  VSTessitore  AKolachana  BFera  FGoldman  DEgan  MFWeinberger  DR Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala.  Science 2002;297 (5580) 400- 403PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
18.
Cooney  REAtlas  LYJoormann  JEugene  FGotlib  IH Amygdala activation in the processing of neutral faces in social anxiety disorder: is neutral really neutral?  Psychiatry Res 2006;148 (1) 55- 59PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
19.
Semplicini  ALenzini  LSartori  MPapparella  ICalo  LAPagnin  EStrapazzon  GBenna  CCosta  RAvogaro  ACeolotto  GPessina  AC Reduced expression of regulator of G-protein signaling 2 (RGS2) in hypertensive patients increases calcium mobilization and ERK1/2 phosphorylation induced by angiotensin II.  J Hypertens 2006;24 (6) 1115- 1124PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
20.
Yang  BZZhao  HKranzler  HRGelernter  J Practical population group assignment with selected informative markers: characteristics and properties of Bayesian clustering via STRUCTURE.  Genet Epidemiol 2005;28 (4) 302- 312PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
21.
Cox  RW AFNI: software for analysis and visualization of functional magnetic resonance neuroimages.  Comput Biomed Res 1996;29 (3) 162- 173PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
22.
Cohen  MS Parametric analysis of fMRI data using linear systems methods.  Neuroimage 1997;6 (2) 93- 103PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
23.
Friston  KJFrith  CDTurner  RFrackowiak  RS Characterizing evoked hemodynamics with fMRI.  Neuroimage 1995;2 (2) 157- 165PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
24.
Lancaster  JLWoldorff  MGParsons  LMLiotti  MFreitas  CSRainey  LKochunov  PVNickerson  DMikiten  SAFox  PT Automated Talairach atlas labels for functional brain mapping.  Hum Brain Mapp 2000;10 (3) 120- 131PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
25.
Kagan  JReznick  SSnidman  N Biological bases of childhood shyness.  Science 1988;240 (4849) 167- 171PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
26.
Biederman  JHirshfeld-Becker  DRRosenbaum  JFHérot  CFriedman  DSnidman  NKagan  JFaraone  SV Further evidence of association between behavioral inhibition and social anxiety in children.  Am J Psychiatry 2001;158 (10) 1673- 1679PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
27.
Robinson  JLKagan  JReznick  JSCorley  R The heritability of inhibited and uninhibited behavior: a twin study.  Dev Psychol 1992;28 (6) 1030- 1037Google ScholarCrossref
28.
Jang  KLLivesley  WJVernon  PA Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: a twin study.  J Pers 1996;64 (3) 577- 591PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
29.
Caspi  AHarrington  HMilne  BAmell  JWTheodore  RFMoffitt  TE Children's behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26.  J Pers 2003;71 (4) 495- 513PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
30.
Bienvenu  OJNestadt  GSamuels  JFCosta  PTHoward  WTEaton  WW Phobic, panic, and major depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality.  J Nerv Ment Dis 2001;189 (3) 154- 161PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
31.
Phan  KLWager  TDTaylor  SFLiberzon  I Functional neuroimaging studies of human emotions.  CNS Spectr 2004;9 (4) 258- 266PubMedGoogle Scholar
32.
Killgore  WDYurgelun-Todd  DA Social anxiety predicts amygdala activation in adolescents viewing fearful faces.  Neuroreport 2005;16 (15) 1671- 1675PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
33.
Paulus  MPStein  MB An insular view of anxiety.  Biol Psychiatry 2006;60 (4) 383- 387PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
34.
Drabant  EMHariri  ARMeyer-Lindenberg  AMunoz  KEMattay  VSKolachana  BSEgan  MFWeinberger  DR Catechol O-methyltransferase val158met genotype and neural mechanisms related to affective arousal and regulation.  Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63 (12) 1396- 1406PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
35.
Pezawas  LMeyer-Lindenberg  ADrabant  EMVerchinski  BAMunoz  KEKolachana  BSEgan  MFMattay  VSHariri  ARWeinberger  DR 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.  Nat Neurosci 2005;8 (6) 828- 834PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
36.
Meyer-Lindenberg  AWeinberger  DR Intermediate phenotypes and genetic mechanisms of psychiatric disorders.  Nat Rev Neurosci 2006;7 (10) 818- 827PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
37.
Ingi  TAoki  Y Expression of RGS2, RGS4 and RGS7 in the developing postnatal brain.  Eur J Neurosci 2002;15 (5) 929- 936PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
38.
Ingi  TKrumins  AMChidiac  PBrothers  GMChung  SSnow  BEBarnes  CALanahan  AASiderovski  DPRoss  EMGilman  AG Dynamic regulation of RGS2 suggests a novel mechanism in G-protein signaling and neuronal plasticity.  J Neurosci 1998;18 (18) 7178- 7188PubMedGoogle Scholar
39.
Ghavami  AHunt  RAOlsen  MAZhang  JSmith  DLKalgaonkar  SRahman  ZYoung  KH Differential effects of regulator of G protein signaling (RGS) proteins on serotonin 5-HT1A, 5-HT2A, and dopamine D2 receptor-mediated signaling and adenylyl cyclase activity.  Cell Signal 2004;16 (6) 711- 721PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
40.
Han  JMark  MDLi  XXie  MWaka  SRettig  JHerlitze  S RGS2 determines short-term synaptic plasticity in hippocampal neurons by regulating G(i/o)-mediated inhibition of presynaptic Ca(2+) channels.  Neuron 2006;51 (5) 575- 586PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
41.
Oliveira-Dos-Santos  AJMatsumoto  GSnow  BEBai  DHouston  FPWhishaw  IQMariathasan  SSaski  TWakeham  AOhaski  PSRoder  JCBarnes  CASiderovski  DPPenninger  JM Regulation of T cell activation, anxiety, and male aggression by RGS2.  Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2000;97 (22) 12272- 12277PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
42.
Gross  VTank  JObst  M  et al.  Autonomic nervous system and blood pressure regulation in RGS2-deficient mice.  Am J Physiol Regul Integr Comp Physiol 2005;288 (5) R1134- R1142PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
43.
Riddle  ELRana  BKMurthy  KKPlehm  RBlumer  KJDiedrich  AJordan  JLuft  FC Polymorphisms and haplotypes of the regulator of G protein signaling-2 gene in normotensives and hypertensives.  Hypertension 2006;47 (3) 415- 420PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
44.
Gladstone  GParker  G Measuring a behaviorally inhibited temperament style: development and initial validation of new self-report measures.  Psychiatry Res 2005;135 (2) 133- 143PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
45.
Pérez-Edgar  KRoberson-Nay  RHardin  MGPoeth  KGuyer  AENelson  EEMcClure  EBHenderson  HAFox  NAPine  DSErnst  M Attention alters neural responses to evocative faces in behaviorally inhibited adolescents.  Neuroimage 2007;35 (4) 1538- 1546PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
46.
Hayward  CKillen  JKraemer  HTaylor  C Linking self-reported childhood behavioral inhibition to adolescent social phobia.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998;37 (12) 1308- 1316PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
47.
Gladstone  GLParker  GBMitchell  PBWilhelm  KAMalhi  GS Relationship between self-reported childhood behavioral inhibition and lifetime anxiety disorders in a clinical sample.  Depress Anxiety 2005;22 (3) 103- 113PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
48.
Hirshfeld-Becker  DRBiederman  JHenin  AFaraone  SVDavis  SHarrington  KRosenbaum  JF Behavioral inhibition in preschool children at risk is a specific predictor of middle childhood social anxiety: a five-year follow-up.  J Dev Behav Pediatr 2007;28 (3) 225- 233PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
49.
Schwartz  CESnidman  NKagan  J Adolescent social anxiety as an outcome of inhibited temperament in childhood.  J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1999;38 (8) 1008- 1015PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
50.
Bienvenu  OJSamuels  JFCosta  PTReti  IMEaton  WWNestadt  G Anxiety and depressive disorders and the five-factor model of personality: a higher- and lower-order personality trait investigation in a community sample.  Depress Anxiety 2004;20 (2) 92- 97PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
51.
Lorberbaum  JPKose  SJohnson  MRArana  GWSullivan  LKHamner  MBBallenger  JCLydiard  RBBrodrick  PSBohning  DEGeorge  MS Neural correlates of speech anticipatory anxiety in generalized social phobia.  Neuroreport 2004;15 (18) 2701- 2705PubMedGoogle Scholar
52.
Fullerton  JCubin  MTiwari  HWang  CBomhra  ADavidson  SMiller  SFairburn  CGoodwin  GNeale  MCFiddy  SMott  RAllison  DBFlint  J Linkage analysis of extremely discordant and concordant sibling pairs identifies quantitative-trait loci that influence variation in the human personality trait neuroticism.  Am J Hum Genet 2003;72 (4) 879- 890PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
53.
Shifman  SBhomra  ASmiley  SWray  NRJames  MRMartin  NGHettema  JMAn  SSNeale  MCvan den Oord  EFKendler  KSChen  XBoomsma  DIMiddeldorp  CMHottenga  JJSlagboom  PEFlint  J A whole genome association study of neuroticism using DNA pooling [published ahead of print July 21, 2007].  Mol Psychiatry 2008;13 (3) 302- 31210.1038/sj.mp.4002048PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
54.
Bienvenu  OJBrown  CSamuels  JFLiang  KYCosta  PTEaton  WWNestadt  G Normal personality traits and comorbidity among phobic, panic and major depressive disorders.  Psychiatry Res 2001;102 (1) 73- 85PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
55.
Hummelen  BWilberg  TPedersen  GKarterud  S The relationship between avoidant personality disorder and social phobia.  Compr Psychiatry 2007;48 (4) 348- 356PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
56.
Hettema  JMPrescott  CAKendler  KS Genetic and environmental sources of covariation between generalized anxiety disorder and neuroticism.  Am J Psychiatry 2004;161 (9) 1581- 1587PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
57.
Kendler  KSGardner  COGatz  MPedersen  NL The sources of co-morbidity between major depression and generalized anxiety disorder in a Swedish national twin sample.  Psychol Med 2007;37 (3) 453- 462PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
58.
Stein  MBSchork  NJGelernter  J A polymorphism of the beta1-adrenergic receptor is associated with low extraversion.  Biol Psychiatry 2004;56 (4) 217- 224PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
59.
Leygraf  AHohoff  CFreitag  CWillis-Owen  SAKrakowitzky  PFritze  JFranke  PBandelow  BFimmers  RFlint  JDeckert  J Rgs 2 gene polymorphisms as modulators of anxiety in humans?  J Neural Transm 2006;113 (12) 1921- 1925PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref
×