Context
Human personality is characterized by substantial heritability but few functional gene variants have been identified. Although rodent data suggest that the neuronal isoform of nitric oxide synthase (NOS-I) modifies diverse behaviors including aggression, this has not been translated to human studies.
Objectives
To investigate the functionality of an NOS1 promoter repeat length variation (NOS1 Ex1f variable number tandem repeat [VNTR]) and to test whether it is associated with phenotypes relevant to impulsivity.
Design
Molecular biological studies assessed the cellular consequences of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR; association studies were conducted to investigate the impact of this genetic variant on impulsivity; imaging genetics was applied to determine whether the polymorphism is functional on a neurobiological level.
Setting
Three psychiatric university clinics in Germany.
Participants
More than 3200 subjects were included in the association study: 1954 controls, 403 patients with personality disorder, 383 patients with adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), 151 with familial ADHD, 189 suicide attempters, and 182 criminal offenders.
Main Outcome Measures
For the association studies, the major outcome criteria were phenotypes relevant to impulsivity, namely, the dimensional phenotype conscientiousness and the categorical phenotypes adult ADHD, aggression, and cluster B personality disorder.
Results
A novel functional promoter polymorphism in NOS1 was associated with traits related to impulsivity, including hyperactive and aggressive behaviors. Specifically, the short repeat variant was more frequent in adult ADHD, cluster B personality disorder, and autoaggressive and heteroaggressive behavior. This short variant came along with decreased transcriptional activity of the NOS1 exon 1f promoter and alterations in the neuronal transcriptome including RGS4 and GRIN1. On a systems level, it was associated with hypoactivation of the anterior cingulate cortex, which is involved in the processing of emotion and reward in behavioral control.
Conclusion
These findings implicate deficits in neuronal signaling via nitric oxide in moderation of prefrontal circuits underlying impulsivity-related behavior in humans.
In recent years, considerable evidence has been accrued for a genetic influence in the control of normal human brain function and behavioral disorders. By genome-wide linkage analysis and whole-genome and case-control association studies, a plethora of novel candidate genes have been proposed for all major psychiatric disorders. However, in almost all cases, haplotype blocks of synonymous or intronic single nucleotide polymorphisms in or near the identified genes were associated, while functional variants continue to be elusive. The search for functional gene variants explaining neurobiological functions thus remains a daunting task. For only a few polymorphisms linked to personality traits and resulting behavioral phenotypes, such as the repeat length variations of the serotonin transporter gene, 5-HTTLPR,1,2 and the MAO-A gene, MAO-A variable number tandem repeat (uVNTR),3,4 molecular functionality has convincingly been demonstrated. Both 5-HTTLPR and MAO-AuVNTR alter the expression of the respective gene, suggesting that this mechanism is likely more common than coding region single nucleotide polymorphisms in the control of human behavior.
Neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS-I) has been linked to rodent behavior by numerous pharmacologic studies. Likewise, knockdown of the Nos1 gene in mice results in behavioral changes involving decreased anxiety, increased impulsivity, and increased aggressiveness.5 The human gene encoding NOS-I, NOS1 (OMIM 163731), however, has hitherto not been linked to personality dimensions or behavioral traits. NOS1 is located at 12q24.3 and displays one of the most complex genes in the human genome.6 An approximately 125-kilobase (kb) region contains 28 protein-coding exons, whereas another approximately 125-kb region, termed variable region, includes at least 11 distinct first exons transcribed into messenger RNA but thereafter removed by splicing (Figure 1A).
Transcription of those alternative first exons, designated exons 1a to 1l, is driven by alternative promoters, resulting in a tissue-specific expression pattern.7 The promoter region of exon 1f, predominantly expressed in the basal ganglia, hippocampus, and cortex, harbors binding motifs for estrogen receptors, cyclic adenosine monophosphate response element-binding (CREB) protein 1, CCAAT/enhancer-binding transport protein (EBP) β-1, and nuclear factor of activated T cells (NFAT) (Figure 1B). Our group previously reported a highly polymorphic dinucleotide repeat with 180 to 210 cytosine-adenosine dinucleotide (CA) units, termed NOS1 Ex1f VNTR, present 33 base pairs (bp) downstream of the TATA box (Figure 1A and B).8 The frequency of the repeat length variants is not distributed evenly, because they cluster mainly at the alleles 182/184, 192, and 200/202/204 (Figure 1C). To facilitate genetic studies, the polymorphism was dichotomized in short (180-196 repeats) and long (198-210 repeats) alleles. Short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR alleles were associated with disease severity and prefrontal functioning in schizophrenia.8 Furthermore, short-allele carriers showed an increase in the risk of developing Alzheimer disease, with evidence of NOS1 × ApoE4 interaction.9
Although NOS1 exon 1f is highly conserved, the promoter repeat appears to be exclusive to humans (eFigure), although there is considerable linkage disequilibrium around this region (eFigure 1C). We thus hypothesized that NOS1 Ex1f VNTR might possess functional relevance on a molecular basis and that it is associated with dimensional behavioral traits, also reflected by personality disorders (PDs) and related domains. In analogy to the behavioral phenotype of Nos1 knockdown mice,5,10 a role for NOS1 Ex1f VNTR in impulsivity-related phenotypes (eg, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD] and cluster B PDs) was assumed.
Seven different samples were ascertained for this study, which are described in greater detail in the
supplemental “Methods” section11-13. The forensic sample was ascertained in the southwest German region of Saarland,14 while all other cohorts were recruited in Bavaria, a part of southern Germany. All subjects were white. Only participants who gave written informed consent after oral and written explanation about the aims of the investigation were enrolled. The study was approved by the ethics committees of the Universities of Würzburg, Homburg, and Munich.
This group included a total of 640 subjects; 284 were healthy blood donors originating from the Würzburg area who were not screened for psychiatric disorders; however, all participants were free of medication. An additional 356 individuals were recruited and screened for absence of psychiatric disorders.
The control B group consisted of 1314 unrelated healthy volunteers randomly selected from the general population of Munich.13 Subjects with neuropsychiatric disorders or with first-degree relatives with such disorders were excluded by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID-I and SCID-II) and the Family History Assessment Module.11
The PD group included 403 inpatients and outpatients of the Department of Psychiatry, University of Würzburg. Inclusion criteria were PD according to DSM-IV (antisocial, histrionic, borderline, narcissistic, avoidant, dependent, and obsessive-compulsive PDs) and age between 18 and 60 years. Exclusion criteria were medical conditions and lifetime diagnosis of schizophrenia or other psychotic disorders. Further details on the sample have previously been reported.15,16
The adult ADHD (aADHD) group consisted of 383 inpatients and outpatients of the Department of Psychiatry, University of Würzburg, referred for diagnostic assessment and treatment of aADHD and examined with the SCID-I. Inclusion criteria were aADHD according to DSM-IV, onset before the age of 7 years, lifelong persistence, current diagnosis, and age at recruitment between 18 and 65 years. Exclusion criteria were the appearance of symptoms restricted to the duration of any other Axis I disorder; current diagnosis of active alcohol or other drug abuse or dependence; lifetime diagnosis of bipolar I disorder, schizophrenia, or any other psychotic disorder; and mental retardation (IQ, <80). Details on the sample may be obtained elsewhere.12
We recruited 189 suicide attempters among patients consecutively referred to the Department of Psychiatry, University of Munich. The sample description has previously been published.13 Current and lifetime diagnoses of mental disorders were assessed close to discharge by applying the SCID-I. Patients with psychiatric disorders due to a general medical condition or with dementia were excluded.
This sample has formerly been described in detail.12 Briefly, 182 adult male volunteers, referred for forensic examination to the Institute of Forensic Psychiatry, Saarland University, Homburg, entered the study. Subjects with a severe Axis I diagnosis and mentally retarded subjects (IQ, <70) were excluded. Each subject was assigned to the violent or nonviolent group according to personal police records and recently committed crimes on the basis of careful evaluation of all available documents. Violence, as an outcome variable of predisposition toward offensive aggression, was defined as an abiding “overt and intentional physically aggressive behavior against another person” as suggested by Volavka17(p308) and adopted by an expert consensus.18 By this means, only subjects with habitual aggressive and violent behavior were included in the violent group, whereas offenders with a history of only nonviolent crimes were assigned to the nonviolent group. History of PD and substance abuse was assessed by standardized instruments.14 Childhood environment was assessed by the adverse childhood environment index, resulting in a mean score of 0 (optimal) to 2 (most adverse childhood environment).
The familial ADHD (fADHD) group consisted of 151 inpatients and outpatients aged 8 to 18 years referred to the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, University of Würzburg. The sample included 118 boys and 33 girls from 102 families. Both parents were ascertained in all families. Included children were characterized by standardized full interview (Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children–Present and Lifetime Version) with a parent and met DSM-IV criteria for ADHD. Exclusion criteria were birth weight less than 2000 g, IQ lower than 85, neurologic or severe somatic disorder, drug abuse, and autistic disorder.
The NOS1 Ex1f VNTR was determined as described previously.8 One of the primers was labeled with a fluorescent dye (cy-5; TIB MolBiol, Berlin, Germany), enabling detection of the polymerase chain reaction (PCR) product. Electrophoretic separation of the PCR products was performed with a DNA sequencer (CEQ8000; Beckman-Coulter, Krefeld, Germany). Primers and PCR conditions are available on request.
MICROARRAY EXPRESSION STUDIES
Approximately 1-cm3 pieces of cerebellar tissue were obtained from the Stanley Medical Research Institute, corresponding to brains for which BA46 microarray data have been obtained.19 The DNA was extracted and NOS1 Ex1f VNTR was determined as described in the previous section. Thereafter, genotype was correlated with microarray data by classifying genotype as 0/1/2 values (long-long/short-long/short-short), in which a fold change reported for a gene would be an average fold change for an increase of 1 in the genotype value. Correction for multiple testing was done thereafter, adjusting α for 20 000 tests. Of the remaining genes, those upregulated or downregulated more than 10% were considered meaningful.
A fragment of approximately 1.2 kb encompassing NOS1 Ex1f and its flanking regions was amplified from genomic DNA of humans homozygous for either 182, 192, or 204 CA repeats. The PCR products were digested with HaeIII and resolved electrophoretically on agarose gel. HaeIII-HaeIII amplicon subbands of 558, 568, and 580 bp respective to the different repeat lengths were purified and blunt-end cloned into a pGL3 basic vector (Promega, Mannheim, Germany). XL-10 competent cells (Stratagene, Amsterdam, the Netherlands) were transformed with 1 μL of ligation product and plated. Positive clones were selected and grown before plasmid purification with a kit (Maxiprep; Qiagen, Hilden, Germany). HeLa cells were grown, seeded, and transfected with 400 ng of pGL3-promotor plasmid and 70 ng of phRG-TK-DNA (Renilla luciferase as transfection control) per well. Transiently transfected cells were allowed to grow for 24 hours before direct collection or induction with 10μM forskolin or 10nM 17β-estradiol (Sigma-Aldrich Company, Taufkirchen, Germany) for 12 hours. Both firefly and Renilla luciferase activities were measured with a kit (Dual-Luciferase Assay Kit; Promega) on a 96-well device (Microlumate; Berthold, Bad Wildbad, Germany). Promoter activities were expressed as relative light units, normalized for protein content and Renilla activity.
ELECTROPHYSIOLOGIC EXAMINATIONS
One hundred sixty-seven healthy controls, genotyped for NOS1 Ex1f VNTR, were investigated by means of a continuous performance test (CPT) as described previously.8 Subjects were matched regarding mean age (F2,163 = 0.18, P = .84) and sex distribution (χ2 = 1.13, P = .57). For the CPT, 400 letters were presented sequentially in a pseudo-randomized order; participants were instructed to press a button whenever the primer condition “O” was directly followed by an “X” (“go” condition). “no-go” was defined as “O” followed by any letter other than an “X.” Parallel to this task, the electroencephalogram was recorded from 21 scalp electrodes. The data were segmented into different conditions of the CPT, and go and no-go epochs were further analyzed. Segments were averaged to 1 go and 1 no-go event-related potential per subject. For topographic analysis, the global field power peaks were determined within a P300 time frame. At individual global field power peaks, amplitude, latency, and anterior-posterior location of the positive centroid were calculated. From these data, the no-go anteriorization, defined as the distance between the individual go and no-go centroid, was determined. For the statistical analysis, analysis of variance was used to determine the effect of genotype on the go and the no-go centroid. Post hoc analyses were conducted by the t test for independent samples.
Case-Control Comparisons for Categorical Phenotypes (PD, Suicide Attempt, aADHD)
Association of NOS1 genotype with aADHD were tested by means of χ2 tests comparing NOS1 genotype frequencies in the aADHD sample with those in the fully psychiatrically screened control sample B. The association remained significant when tested against control sample A and the combined control sample. To control for comorbidity with cluster B PD, a second analysis was performed comparing only those aADHD participants who did not exhibit a cluster B PD (n = 194). Tests for association between NOS1 Ex1f VNTR and PD, or suicide attempt, were examined by means of χ2 tests with df = 2.
Personality Measures in Sample B
Possible effects of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR genotype on personality traits assessed by the NEO Personality Inventory–Revised (NEO-PI-R)20 were examined in control sample B (complete data available for 1099 subjects). Association tests were performed by means of analysis of variance. Taking into account possible sex effects and interactions between sex and genotype, NOS1 genotype and the participants' sex were entered as between-subjects factors. The analysis of variance model comprised sex and NOS1 genotype as well as their interactions. Effects were considered significant if their type 1 error probability was less than α = .05 (uncorrected). Numerator degrees of freedom in both samples were 1 for sex and 2 for NOS1 and NOS1 × sex; denominator degrees of freedom were 1087. Power calculations using G*Power 321 indicated that the healthy control sample was large enough to detect effects explaining more than 1% variance in personality traits at a significance threshold of α = .05 and with a power of 1−β = 0.80.
Subjects were compared with regard to the presence or absence of pervasive violent behavior as previously described.14 A logistic regression model with violent behavior as the dependent variable and NOS1 Ex1f VNTR as the main effect of interest was performed, controlling for 4 possible confounding variables: adverse childhood experience, history of substance abuse, PD, and age.
A family-based association test, valid for families with an arbitrary number of affected children, was performed by using a permutation test implemented in FAMHAP software.22
EFFECT OF NOS1 Ex1f VNTR ON REPORTER GENE EXPRESSION
Because the functional relevance of this polymorphism has not been investigated, we determined expression of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR short (182 repeats), intermediate (192 repeats), and long (204 repeats) alleles fused to a luciferase reporter vector. The sequence included a CREB binding site, the TATA box, and the beginning of exon 1f (Figure 1B). The full-length insert with long repeats of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR resulted in maximal gene expression, whereas intermediate and, to a greater extent, short repeats decreased expression of the reporter gene (Figure 1D). Although the construct contains a CREB binding site, forskolin treatment failed to increase gene expression; relative promoter activity was also not affected by estrogen.
EFFECT OF NOS1 Ex1f VNTR ON THE NEURONAL TRANSCRIPTOME
To assess whether NOS1 Ex1f VNTR also influences the regulation of other genes, we examined the impact of length variation on global gene transcription by expression profiling of human postmortem brain (Brodmann area [BA] 46) collected from 105 individuals.19 Microarray data including a total of 19 500 genes were stratified by the presence of 0, 1, or 2 short promoter alleles. We applied strict correction for multiple testing, ie, used an α adjustment of 2.5 × 10−6, and considered genes that were upregulated or downregulated more than 10% as functionally meaningful. Twenty-five genes were identified as significantly altered (Table 1). The complete list of genes with significant changes in expression is given in eTable 1.
ASSOCIATION OF NOS1 Ex1f VNTR WITH IMPULSIVITY-RELATED PHENOTYPES
On the basis of our findings indicating that NOS1 Ex1f VNTR alleles affect the brain transcriptome and decrease promoter activity, and previous data in Nos1 knockdown mice arguing for increased impulsivity and resulting aggression,5 we predicted an influence of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR on human impulsivity (ie, instant acting without consideration of consequences, including aggressive, emotional, and hyperactive behaviors) and related disorders. The primary aim was to link NOS1 both to dimensional variations of human behavior, most likely underlying psychopathological disease states, and to disease categories. Because genotyping of large and well-characterized cohorts is critical in this respect, we determined frequencies of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR variants in several distinct samples (Table 2), including healthy controls with data on personality questionnaires, a PD sample, a cohort of patients with aADHD, suicide attempters, and criminal offenders. The total number of genotyped individuals was 3111 (3466 including the trio analysis).
Both examined control populations (which were ascertained independently) displayed an almost identical distribution of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR (χ2, P = .33), whereas a consistent significant excess of short alleles was detected in adequately powered patient populations (Table 2; for the comparison of NOS genotype frequencies in PD, suicide attempters, and aADHD, the respective sample sizes permitted detection of relatively small effects with a power of 1 − β > 0.95). On a categorical level, short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR alleles were not associated with anxious-avoidant cluster C PD (P = .33), but were associated with the presence of emotional-dramatic cluster B PD (P = .01), especially histrionic (P = .009) PD, and with the presence of aADHD (P = .001). Because in the latter sample a high comorbidity with cluster B PD has been reported,12 we investigated in a separate, family-based sample of children with ADHD (n = 355)23 whether the short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR allele contributes to genetic risk toward ADHD per se. However, short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR alleles were not preferentially transmitted to affected offspring (P = .39). We then controlled for absence or presence of cluster B PD in aADHD; after exclusion of all aADHD participants with cluster B PD, the association of NOS1 with aADHD remained significant (P = .04), arguing for the notion that the polymorphism conveys risk for persistence of ADHD into adulthood independently of comorbid PD. In the forensic sample, childhood adversity was controlled for because it was shown to be a risk factor for later-life violence.14 Short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR alleles were independently associated with later-life violent crime (P = .04; eTable 2). Finally, suicide attempters also carried short alleles more frequently (P = .02).
Taken together, these findings provide evidence of an association between short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR alleles and disorders characterized by impulsive, hyperactive, and aggressive behaviors. To uncover possible underlying dimensional variation in personality, the revised NEO-PI-R20 was applied in controls. In healthy controls (n = 1099), an NOS1 Ex1f VNTR × sex interaction on the personality domain conscientiousness was demonstrated (eTable 3). There was a nominal trend to lower scores in male short-allele carriers, whereas this effect was statistically significant for women (P = .001; Figure 2).
EFFECT OF NOS1 Ex1f VNTR ON PREFRONTAL BRAIN FUNCTIONING
Diminished activity of the prefrontal cortex is suggested to play a role in increased impulsivity. This prompted us to investigate a subset of control subjects (n = 167) by using a CPT with parallel recording of event-related potential and subsequent topographical analysis. The no-go centroid, a measure of medial prefrontal activity during response inhibition, was localized significantly more posteriorly in carriers of the short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR allele, in a gene–dose-dependent fashion (F2,167 = 3.29, P < .05; Figure 3), as was nominally also the case for schizophrenic patients.8 This indicates that activation of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), which is the major neural source for the specific topography of the no-go centroid,24 is diminished in short-allele carriers, suggesting impaired medial prefrontal functioning in these subjects.
Rodent studies using pharmacologic methods and genetically modified animals5,10 suggest that NOS-I has a role in regulating impulsive-aggressive behaviors. We thus investigated whether genetic variation in NOS1 influences analogous behaviors in humans as well. This was indeed the case because a functional promoter polymorphism (NOS1 Ex1f VNTR) was associated with personality dimensions related to impulsivity as well as psychiatric disorders that share impulsive behavior as a common phenotype (namely, aADHD, cluster B PD, and aggression). Together with MAO-AuVNTR3 and 5HTTLPR,1NOS1 Ex1f VNTR thus belongs to a class of promoter repeat polymorphisms that alter gene expression and have been shown to be associated with variation in human behavior.
To demonstrate functionality of the polymorphism, we established that short variants of the repeat result in decreased reporter gene expression. It thus can be assumed that short promoter repeats also decrease the expression of the alternative NOS1 exon 1f. The consequences, however, remain unclear. In enteric neurons, it was shown that reduced NOS1 exon 1c expression was compensated for by upregulation of NOS1 exon 1f,25 suggesting a reciprocal regulation of both alternative first exons. It can therefore be speculated that, on reduced NOS1 exon 1f transcription, a counter-regulatory increase of NOS1 exon 1c occurs. Further consequences of diminished NOS1 exon 1f expression might include altered messenger RNA stability, differential messenger RNA maturation, or changes in the intracellular trafficking of messenger RNA. Although the detailed molecular action of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR is not known yet, it has a role in intracellular mechanisms as reflected by changes in the transcriptome of human brain tissue, indicated by microarray data from BA46 (Table 1). In a recent meta-analysis incorporating 18 studies,26 this brain region belongs to the neurocircuitry of impulsivity, as assessed by functional magnetic resonance imaging using no-go tasks, a method comparable to our event-related potential study (Figure 3), suggesting that these expressional changes might indeed have a role in the regulation of impulsive behavior.
Interestingly, several of the genes identified to be dysregulated are directly related to nitric oxide (NO) function. Some of these genes code for enzymes involved in reactive oxygen detoxification mechanisms and form a common pathway: superoxide dismutase 2, glutathione peroxidase 3, and mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK8). The last of these is activated by stressful stimuli,27 negatively regulated by NO,28 and in turn suppresses superoxide dismutase 1 expression.29 Thus, NOS1 Ex1f VNTR seems to be associated with an imbalance of genes related to NO function. A second set of genes corresponds to cytoskeletal proteins (β4- and β5-tubulin, MAP1LC3B, and NEFL).
Most interestingly, however, a number of genes were previously implicated in psychiatric disorders: CHI3L1, involved in Akt signaling, has been proposed as a schizophrenia susceptibility gene,30 probably by counteracting pro-inflammatory cytokines. α-Synuclein is a key player in the pathogenesis of neurodegenerative disorders.31 Probably by nitrosylation,32 NO increases the number of α-synuclein aggregates, thereby probably promoting disease progression. With respect to impulsive behaviors, it appears noteworthy that α-synuclein has also been suggested as a candidate gene for alcoholism.33 Several upregulated genes were already shown to have a role both in glutamatergic signaling and schizophrenia: RGS4 can be considered one of the most promising schizophrenia candidate genes, as underscored by a recent meta-analysis,34 and affects N-methyl D-aspartate 1 (NMDA) signaling.35 Also converging on the glutamatergic synapse, NEFL increases the cell surface expression of the NMDA receptor and consecutive NMDA-mediated toxicity.36 Finally, and most notably, GRIN1, the gene encoding the upstream NOS-I activator NMDA receptor 1, is dysregulated as a function of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR. Our previous finding that this polymorphism affects disease severity in schizophrenia8 thus might at least in part be explained by its effects on glutamatergic genes. Taken together, these data indicate a deficit in specific molecular pathways in short-allele carriers, all of which represent important molecular cascades in psychiatric disease: oxidative stress, neuronal cytoskeleton, and glutamatergic signaling.
Our case-control association studies in several independent, large samples argue for a role of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR in disorders with the shared feature of impulsive behaviors,37 a core feature and diagnostic criterion of aADHD and cluster B PD38 alike. Furthermore, increased impulsivity is thought to underlie or facilitate at least some forms of suicidal39 and aggressive40 behavior. The aforementioned categorical phenotypes are not independent and should therefore be viewed from a quantitative standpoint, as aADHD has a high comorbidity with cluster B PD12; both conditions go along with an increased risk for autoaggressive and heteroaggressive behavior. Thus, NOS1 Ex1f VNTR might be a genetic factor underlying those conditions, partially explaining their interrelatedness with emphasis on impulsivity as a common factor. Given that those disorders feature complex genetics, this possible explanation for their interrelatedness strongly argues for a dimensional view of these phenotypes. This argument, however, should also be reflected by parallel alteration of personality dimensions, as assessed, eg, by personality questionnaires; indeed, this was the case because NOS1 Ex1f VNTR influenced conscientiousness.
Low conscientiousness levels indicate high impulsivity, ie, they correlate negatively with impulsivity.41,42 Despite the fact that impulsivity is a facet of NEO-PI-R neuroticism, the biological trait of impulsivity might rather be mirrored by the conscientiousness domain.43 Underscoring this notion, both cluster B PD and aADHD feature significantly decreased conscientiousness scores.12,44 A complex effect of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR on conscientiousness became evident as we observed a gene × sex interaction. Female carriers of the short NOS1 Ex1f VNTR allele tend to act less diligent and persistent, with an overall lower motivation in goal-directed behavior. The biological rationale behind the sex specificity might be a cis-acting estrogen binding site upstream of the NOS1 Ex1f VNTR–containing region, outside the reporter gene construct, which itself was not sensitive to estrogen. Because age correlates positively with conscientiousness,45 and because early adversity also affects this personality dimension46 as well as later-life violence,14 these variables should be assessed for their interaction with NOS1 Ex1f VNTR genotype in future studies.
Taken together, NOS1 Ex1f VNTR is associated with a personality dimension related to cognitive control and inhibition, relevant to the categorical phenotypes associated with NOS1 (ie, aADHD, cluster B PD, and aggression), and thereby contributes to the genetic risk of psychiatric disorders that have dysfunctional impulsivity47 as a common feature. Molecular mechanisms might include dysregulation of glutamatergic neurotransmission but also interactions of NO with the serotonergic system. Serotonin levels, which are regulated by serotonin transporter, have been suggested to be crucial in the regulation of impulsivity48; accordingly, selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors are the treatment of choice for disorders of impulse control. Interestingly, NOS-I reciprocally interacts with serotonin transporter, causing a reduction of serotonin uptake.49 High NOS1 expression thus might have the same functional consequences as selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor treatment. Alternative explanations for the association findings are, however, also possible. For example, deficits in cognitive functioning underlie most of the conditions examined in this study: NOS1 Ex1f VNTR may not influence impulsivity per se but may influence cognitive abilities, a hypothesis that is well worth further investigation. However, in the control sample (n = 1216), NOS1 Ex1f VNTR was not associated with overall IQ (F1,1208 = 1303, P = .27), even if this does not necessarily rule out the possibility that NOS-I influences more specific cognitive functions.
Although impulsivity is considered neurobiologically heterogeneous,48 behavioral inhibition is regarded to represent an important endophenotype that can be assessed by go/no-go tasks.50 Because the prefrontal cortex is implicated in behavioral inhibition and impulsivity, and because our previous data suggested impaired prefrontal functioning due to NOS1 Ex1f VNTR in schizophrenic patients, the approach of imaging genetics by means of event-related potentials that has been developed and applied in our laboratory51 was used to probe the hypothesis that genetic variation of NOS1 Ex1f VNTR affects ACC functioning. Indeed, carriers of risk alleles displayed decreased activation of the ACC, as evidenced by more posterior localization of the no-go centroid. This might well be a neural circuitry–dependent mechanism in how NOS1 Ex1f VNTR genotype exerts its influence on cognitive control and psychiatric disease states. The ACC is thought to be involved not only in error detection but also in the recognition of effort-reward correlations, thus comprising an outcome monitoring system.52 Genetically driven hypoexcitability of the ACC might thus lead to blunted response toward long-term behavioral consequences, thereby resulting in more immediate and uncontrolled actions underlying disinhibited, impulsive, and impaired goal-directed behavior as reflected by the personality domain conscientiousness. Further support for involvement of NOS1 in prefrontal functioning comes from a recent study53 demonstrating an association of a 3′UTR NOS1 single nucleotide polymorphism with frontotemporal dementia; nevertheless, the underlying molecular mechanisms remain to be established.
In conclusion, we have demonstrated that a repeat polymorphism in the alternative promoter of exon 1f of neuronal NOS1 (1) affects promoter activity, (2) influences the transcriptome of the human brain, (3) is associated with psychiatric disorders with the shared feature of impulsivity, (4) is associated with personality traits related to cognitive control, and (5) affects electrocortical measures of prefrontal functions involved in cognitive control. Translational phenotyping of low NOS1 expression in mice and humans thus converges to the notion that deficits in neuronal signaling via gaseous NO are associated with impulsive behaviors and impaired prefrontal brain functioning.
Correspondence: Andreas Reif, MD, Unit for Molecular Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics and Psychotherapy, University of Würzburg, Füchsleinstr 15, D-97080 Würzburg, Germany (reif_a@klinik.uni-wuerzburg.de).
Submitted for Publication: March 18, 2008; final revision received July 4, 2008; accepted July 18, 2008.
Author Contributions: Dr Reif had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This study was supported by the German Research Foundation (grants RE1632/1-1 and 1-3 [Dr Reif], KFO 125 [Drs Reif, Jacob, Fallgatter, and Lesch], SFB 581 [Dr Lesch], and SFB TRR 58 [Drs Reif, Fallgatter, Ehlis, and Lesch]), Federal Ministry of Education and Research (IZKF Würzburg, 01KS9603 [Dr Lesch] and IZKF N-27-N [Drs Reif and Herterich]), and the European Commission (NEWMOOD LSHM-CT-2003-503474 [Dr Lesch]).
Additional Contributions: Postmortem brain tissue was donated by the Stanley Medical Research Institute's brain collection courtesy of Michael B. Knable, PhD (Stanley Research Institute), E. Fuller Torrey, Maree J. Webster, and Robert H. Yolken. We are indebted to M. Elashoff for statistical evaluation of microarray data. Theresia Töpner, Nicole Steigerwald, Gabriela Ortega, Melanie Harder, Ramona Täglich, and Andrea Eujen, MD, provided excellent technical assistance. We gratefully acknowledge Tomasz Jarczok, MD, Jasmin Wegener, MD, Monika Heine, MD, and Andrea Borreatti-Hümmer, MD (all from the Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics, and Psychotherapy, University of Würzburg), for help in ascertaining patients and diagnostic assessment; Astrid Dempfle and T. Trang Nguyen (Institute of Medical Biometry and Epidemiology, Philipps-University Marburg) for statistical advice; and Jürgen Deckert, MD (Department of Psychiatry, Psychosomatics, and Psychotherapy, University of Würzburg), and Cornelius Gross, PhD (EMBL Monterondo, Rome, Italy), for critical and helpful discussion of the manuscript.
This article was corrected online for typographical errors on 1/5/2008.
1.Lesch
KP, Bengel
D, Heils
A, Sabol
SZ, Greenberg
BD, Petri
S, Benjamin
J, Müller
CR, Hamer
DH, Murphy
DL. Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. .
Science. 1996;274(5292):1527-1531.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Caspi
A, Sugden
K, Moffitt
TE, Taylor
A, Craig
IW, Harrington
H, McClay
J, Mill
J, Martin
J, Braithwaite
A, Poulton
R. Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. .
Science. 2003;301(5631):386-389.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Deckert
J, Catalano
M, Syagailo
YV, Bosi
M, Okladnova
O, Di Bella
D, Nöthen
MM, Maffei
P, Franke
P, Fritze
J, Maier
W, Propping
P, Beckmann
H, Bellodi
L, Lesch
KP. Excess of high activity monoamine oxidase A gene promoter alleles in female patients with panic disorder. .
Hum Mol Genet. 1999;8(4):621-624.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Caspi
A, McClay
J, Moffitt
TE, Mill
J, Martin
J, Craig
IW, Taylor
A, Poulton
R. Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. .
Science. 2002;297(5582):851-854.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Nelson
RJ, Demas
GE, Huang
PL, Fishman
MC, Dawson
VL, Dawson
TM, Snyder
SH. Behavioral abnormalities in male mice lacking neuronal nitric oxide synthase. .
Nature. 1995;378(6555):383-386.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Zhang
T, Haws
P, Wu
Q. Multiple variable first exons: a mechanism for cell- and tissue-specific gene regulation. .
Genome Res. 2004;14(1):79-89.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Bros
M, Boissel
JP, Godtel-Armbrust
U, Forstermann
U. Transcription of human neuronal nitric oxide synthase mRNAs derived from different first exons is partly controlled by exon 1–specific promoter sequences. .
Genomics. 2006;87(4):463-473.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Reif
A, Herterich
S, Strobel
A, Ehlis
AC, Saur
D, Jacob
CP, Wienker
T, Töpner
T, Fritzen
S, Walter
U, Schmitt
A, Fallgatter
AJ, Lesch
KP. A neuronal nitric oxide synthase (NOS-I) haplotype associated with schizophrenia modifies prefrontal cortex function. .
Mol Psychiatry. 2006;11(3):286-300.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Galimberti
D, Scarpini
E, Venturelli
E, Strobel
A, Herterich
S, Fenoglio
C, Guidi
I, Scalabrini
D, Cortini
F, Bresolin
N, Lesch
KP, Reif
A. Association of a NOS1 promoter repeat with Alzheimer's disease. .
Neurobiol Aging. 2008;29(9):1359-1365.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Wultsch
T, Chourbaji
S, Fritzen
S, Kittel
S, Grünblatt
E, Gerlach
M, Gutknecht
L, Chizat
F, Golfier
G, Schmitt
A, Gass
P, Lesch
KP, Reif
A. Behavioural and expressional phenotyping of nitric oxide synthase-I knockdown animals. .
J Neural Transm Suppl. 2007;72(72):69-85.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 11.Rice
JP, Reich
T, Bucholz
KK, Neuman
RJ, Fishman
R, Rochberg
N, Hesselbrock
VM, Nurnberger
JI
Jr, Schuckit
MA, Begleiter
H. Comparison of direct interview and family history diagnoses of alcohol dependence. .
Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 1995;19(4):1018-1023.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Jacob
CP, Romanos
J, Dempfle
A, Heine
M, Windemuth-Kieselbach
C, Kruse
A, Reif
A, Walitza
S, Romanos
M, Strobel
A, Brocke
B, Schäfer
H, Schmidtke
A, Böning
J, Lesch
K-P. Co-morbidity of adult attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder with focus on personality traits and related disorders in a tertiary referral center. .
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2007;257(6):309-317.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Rujescu
D, Giegling
I, Mandelli
L, Schneider
B, Hartmann
AM, Schnabel
A, Maurer
K, Möller
HJ, Serretti
A. NOS-I and -III gene variants are differentially associated with facets of suicidal behavior and aggression-related traits. .
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B(1):42-48.
Google ScholarCrossref 14.Reif
A, Rosler
M, Freitag
CM, Schneider
M, Eujen
A, Kissling
C, Wenzler
D, Jacob
CP, Retz-Junginger
P, Thome
J, Lesch
K-P, Retz
W. Nature and nurture predispose to violent behavior: serotonergic genes and adverse childhood environment. .
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2007;32(11):2375-2383.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Jacob
CP, Strobel
A, Hohenberger
K, Ringel
T, Gutknecht
L, Reif
A, Brocke
B, Lesch
KP. Association between allelic variation of serotonin transporter function and neuroticism in anxious cluster C personality disorders. .
Am J Psychiatry. 2004;161(3):569-572.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Jacob
CP, Müller
J, Schmidt
M, Hohenberger
K, Gutknecht
L, Reif
A, Schmidtke
A, Mössner
R, Lesch
KP. Cluster B personality disorders are associated with allelic variation of monoamine oxidase A activity. .
Neuropsychopharmacology. 2005;30(9):1711-1718.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Filley
CM, Price
BH, Nell
V, Antoinette
T, Morgan
AS, Bresnahan
JF, Pincus
JH, Gelbort
MM, Weissberg
M, Kelly
JP. Toward an understanding of violence: neurobehavioral aspects of unwarranted physical aggression: Aspen Neurobehavioral Conference consensus statement. .
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol. 2001;14(1):1-14.
PubMedGoogle Scholar 19.Higgs
BW, Elashoff
M, Richman
S, Barci
B. An online database for brain disease research. .
BMC Genomics. 2006;7:7010.1186/1471-2164-7-70
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Costa
PT, McCrae
RR. Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five-Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI) Professional Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources; 1998.
21.Faul
F, Erdfelder
E, Lang
AG, Buchner
A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. .
Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175-191.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 23.Walitza
S, Renner
TJ, Dempfle
A, Konrad
K, Wewetzer
Ch, Halbach
A, Herpertz-Dahlmann
B, Remschmidt
H, Smidt
J, Linder
M, Flierl
L, Knölker
U, Friedel
S, Schäfer
H, Gross
C, Hebebrand
J, Warnke
A, Lesch
KP. Transmission disequilibrium of polymorphic variants in the tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. .
Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(12):1126-1132.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Fallgatter
AJ, Bartsch
AJ, Herrmann
MJ. Electrophysiological measurements of anterior cingulate function. .
J Neural Transm. 2002;109(5-6):977-988.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Saur
D, Vanderwinden
JM, Seidler
B, Schmid
RM, De Laet
MH, Allescher
HD. Single-nucleotide promoter polymorphism alters transcription of neuronal nitric oxide synthase exon 1c in infantile hypertrophic pyloric stenosis. .
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2004;101(6):1662-1667.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Buchsbaum
BR, Greer
S, Chang
WL, Berman
KF. Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies of the Wisconsin card-sorting task and component processes. .
Hum Brain Mapp. 2005;25(1):35-45.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Park
HS, Mo
JS, Choi
EJ. Nitric oxide inhibits an interaction between JNK1 and c-Jun through nitrosylation. .
Biochem Biophys Res Commun. 2006;351(1):281-286.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Afonso
V, Santos
G, Collin
P, Khatib
AM, Mitrovic
DR, Lomri
N, Leitman
DC, Lomri
A. Tumor necrosis factor-α down-regulates human Cu/Zn superoxide dismutase 1 promoter via JNK/AP-1 signaling pathway. .
Free Radic Biol Med. 2006;41(5):709-721.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Zhao
X, Tang
R, Gao
B, Shi
Y, Zhou
J, Guo
S, Zhang
J, Wang
Y, Tang
W, Meng
J, Li
S, Wang
H, Ma
G, Lin
C, Xiao
Y, Feng
G, Lin
Z, Zhu
S, Xing
Y, Sang
H, St Clair
D, He
L. Functional variants in the promoter region of Chitinase 3-like 1 (CHI3L1) and susceptibility to schizophrenia. .
Am J Hum Genet. 2007;80(1):12-18.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Lee
VM, Trojanowski
JQ. Mechanisms of Parkinson's disease linked to pathological α-synuclein: new targets for drug discovery. .
Neuron. 2006;52(1):33-38.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Bönsch
D, Lederer
T, Reulbach
U, Hothorn
T, Kornhuber
J, Bleich
S. Joint analysis of the NACP-REP1 marker within the α synuclein gene concludes association with alcohol dependence. .
Hum Mol Genet. 2005;14(7):967-971.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Talkowski
ME, Seltman
H, Bassett
AS, Brzustowicz
LM, Chen
X, Chowdari
KV, Collier
DA, Cordeiro
Q, Corvin
AP, Deshpande
SN, Egan
MF, Gill
M, Kendler
KS, Kirov
G, Heston
LL, Levitt
P, Lewis
DA, Li
T, Mirnics
K, Morris
DW, Norton
N, O'Donovan
MC, Owen
MJ, Richard
C, Semwal
P, Sobell
JL, St Clair
D, Straub
RE, Thelma
BK, Vallada
H, Weinberger
DR, Williams
NM, Wood
J, Zhang
F, Devlin
B, Nimgaonkar
VL. Evaluation of a susceptibility gene for schizophrenia: genotype based meta-analysis of RGS4 polymorphisms from thirteen independent samples. .
Biol Psychiatry. 2006;60(2):152-162.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Harrison
PJ, Weinberger
DR. Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence [published corrections appear in
Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(4):420 and
Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(8):804]. .
Mol Psychiatry. 2005;10(1):40-68.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Ratnam
J, Teichberg
VI. Neurofilament-light increases the cell surface expression of the
N-methyl-
D-aspartate receptor and prevents its ubiquitination. .
J Neurochem. 2005;92(4):878-885.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Moeller
FG, Barratt
ES, Dougherty
DM, Schmitz
JM, Swann
AC. Psychiatric aspects of impulsivity. .
Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(11):1783-1793.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Fossati
A, Barratt
ES, Borroni
S, Villa
D, Grazioli
F, Maffei
C. Impulsivity, aggressiveness, and
DSM-IV personality disorders. .
Psychiatry Res. 2007;149(1-3):157-167.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Zouk
H, Tousignant
M, Seguin
M, Lesage
A, Turecki
G. Characterization of impulsivity in suicide completers: clinical, behavioral and psychosocial dimensions. .
J Affect Disord. 2006;92(2-3):195-204.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Barratt
ES, Stanford
MS, Kent
TA, Felthous
A. Neuropsychological and cognitive psychophysiological substrates of impulsive aggression. .
Biol Psychiatry. 1997;41(10):1045-1061.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Zuckerman
M. P-impulsive sensation seeking and its behavioral, psychophysiological and biochemical correlates. .
Neuropsychobiology. 1993;28(1-2):30-36.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Manuck
SB, Flory
JD, McCaffery
JM, Matthews
KA, Mann
JJ, Muldoon
MF. Aggression, impulsivity, and central nervous system serotonergic responsivity in a nonpatient sample. .
Neuropsychopharmacology. 1998;19(4):287-299.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Congdon
E, Canli
T. The endophenotype of impulsivity: reaching consilience through behavioral, genetic, and neuroimaging approaches. .
Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2005;4(4):262-281.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Saulsman
LM, Page
AC. The five-factor model and personality disorder empirical literature: a meta-analytic review. .
Clin Psychol Rev. 2004;23(8):1055-1085.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45. McCrae
RR, Costa
PT
Jr, Pedroso de Lima
M, Simões
A, Ostendorf
F, Angleitner
A, Marusić
I, Bratko
D, Caprara
GV, Barbaranelli
C, Chae
JH, Piedmont
RL. Age differences in personality across the adult life span: parallels in five cultures. .
Dev Psychol. 1999;35(2):466-477.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Torgersen
AM, Janson
H. Why do identical twins differ in personality: shared environment reconsidered. .
Twin Res. 2002;5(1):44-52.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Dickman
SJ. Functional and dysfunctional impulsivity: personality and cognitive correlates. .
J Pers Soc Psychol. 1990;58(1):95-102.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 49.Chanrion
B, Mannoury la Cour
C, Bertaso
F, Lerner-Natoli
M, Freissmuth
M, Millan
MJ, Bockaert
J, Marin
P. Physical interaction between the serotonin transporter and neuronal nitric oxide synthase underlies reciprocal modulation of their activity. .
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2007;104(19):8119-8124.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Congdon
E, Lesch
KP, Canli
T. Analysis of DRD4 and DAT polymorphisms and behavioral inhibition in healthy adults: implications for impulsivity. .
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008;147B(1):27-32.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Fallgatter
AJ, Jatzke
S, Bartsch
AJ, Hamelbeck
B, Lesch
KP. Serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism influences topography of inhibitory motor control. .
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. 1999;2(2):115-120.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Botvinick
MM, Cohen
JD, Carter
CS. Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: an update. .
Trends Cogn Sci. 2004;8(12):539-546.
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Venturelli
E, Villa
C, Scarpini
E, Fenoglio
C, Guidi
I, Lovati
C, Marcone
A, Cortini
F, Scalabrini
D, Clerici
F, Bresolin
N, Mariani
C, Cappa
S, Galimberti
D. Neuronal nitric oxide synthase C276T polymorphism increases the risk for frontotemporal lobar degeneration. .
Eur J Neurol. 2008;15(1):77-81.
PubMedGoogle Scholar