Context
Wisdom is a unique psychological trait noted since antiquity, long discussed in humanities disciplines, recently operationalized by psychology and sociology researchers, but largely unexamined in psychiatry or biology.
Objective
To discuss recent neurobiological studies related to subcomponents of wisdom identified from several published definitions/descriptions of wisdom by clinical investigators in the field, ie, prosocial attitudes/behaviors, social decision making/pragmatic knowledge of life, emotional homeostasis, reflection/self-understanding, value relativism/tolerance, and acknowledgment of and dealing effectively with uncertainty.
Data Sources
Literature focusing primarily on neuroimaging/brain localization and secondarily on neurotransmitters, including their genetic determinants.
Study Selection
Studies involving functional neuroimaging or neurotransmitter functioning, examining human (rather than animal) subjects, and identified via a PubMed search using keywords from any of the 6 proposed subcomponents of wisdom were included.
Data Extraction
Studies were reviewed by both of us, and data considered to be potentially relevant to the neurobiology of wisdom were extracted.
Data Synthesis
Functional neuroimaging permits exploration of neural correlates of complex psychological attributes such as those proposed to comprise wisdom. The prefrontal cortex figures prominently in several wisdom subcomponents (eg, emotional regulation, decision making, value relativism), primarily via top-down regulation of limbic and striatal regions. The lateral prefrontal cortex facilitates calculated, reason-based decision making, whereas the medial prefrontal cortex is implicated in emotional valence and prosocial attitudes/behaviors. Reward neurocircuitry (ventral striatum, nucleus accumbens) also appears important for promoting prosocial attitudes/behaviors. Monoaminergic activity (especially dopaminergic and serotonergic), influenced by several genetic polymorphisms, is critical to certain subcomponents of wisdom such as emotional regulation (including impulse control), decision making, and prosocial behaviors.
Conclusions
We have proposed a speculative model of the neurobiology of wisdom involving frontostriatal and frontolimbic circuits and monoaminergic pathways. Wisdom may involve optimal balance between functions of phylogenetically more primitive brain regions (limbic system) and newer ones (prefrontal cortex). Limitations of the putative model are stressed. It is hoped that this review will stimulate further research in characterization, assessment, neurobiology, and interventions related to wisdom.
Of all the pursuits open to men, the search for wisdom is most perfect, more sublime, more profitable, and more full of joy.—Thomas Aquinas (~ 1260)1
Wisdom, a unique human attribute rich in history dating back to the dawn of civilization, is a newcomer to the world of empirical research. For centuries, wisdom was the sole province of religion and philosophy.2 A standard philosophical (in Greek, philos-sophia = lover of wisdom) definition of wisdom pertains to judicious application of knowledge,3 and most religions have considered it a virtue. Wisdom is thought to be a complex construct, with several subcomponents. While the relative emphasis on specific subcomponents has varied across cultures and periods, there have been more similarities than differences among different postulated concepts of wisdom. While classic Greek writings on wisdom focused on rationality, early Indian and Chinese thinkers stressed emotional balance.4,5 Yet, these conceptualizations of wisdom shared several common features, such as thoughtful decision making, compassion, altruism, and insight. Excellent accounts of the history of the concept of wisdom are available.5-8
In the 19th century, Gall, who popularized the pseudoscience of phrenology, included among its 27 mental functions “comparative sagacity,” at times called wisdom, and assigned it to prefrontal regions.9 It was, however, only a few decades ago that sociology, psychology, and gerontology began considering wisdom as a subject worthy of discussion, albeit a controversial one. Erikson10 suggested that the last stage of his 8-stage theory of psychosocial development, from about age 65 years to death, centered on conflict resolution between ego integrity and despair, with successful resolution culminating in wisdom. In the 1970s, Baltes et al,11 Clayton,12 and others13 initiated empirical research in this area. Although the initial western theories of wisdom focused on cognitive abilities, Ardelt14 and others drew attention to the importance of emotional self-regulation. The evolving modern conceptualization of wisdom bears remarkable similarities to some of the oldest concepts of wisdom postulated in the Bhagavad Gita, an Indian religious text written several centuries BC.4
Recent years have witnessed paradigm shifts in medicine, moving the spotlight from disease to health, from treatment to prevention, and from risk factors to protective factors. Similarly, several positive psychological constructs have attracted growing academic interest as psychiatry has begun to appreciate the need for studying protective psychological traits such as resilience, instead of a singular focus on psychopathology.15-18 A few mental health researchers have noted the value of examining wisdom.19,20 There are now several scales for assessing wisdom, with variable psychometric properties.14,21,22 Overall, literature on wisdom continues to expand. As Figure 1 shows, the number of articles on the construct of wisdom found in a PubMed database search using the keyword “wisdom” has increased 7-fold from the 1970s through 2008. The topic of wisdom is also being discussed in prominent lay media,23 clinical medicine,24-26 and learning theories.27
The following overview is based on our interpretation of the literature on wisdom. It clearly would be unwise of us to claim this interpretation as a definitive model. Our goal is to stimulate discourse and research in an important but neglected area of investigation. Subsequent empirical research may lead to substantial revision or even repudiation of the putative model we describe herein.
Although there is no consensual definition of wisdom, we believe that wisdom is a unique psychological construct, not just a collection of desirable traits with a convenient unifying label. Wisdom may be viewed as a trait comprising several subcomponents. We searched the published literature on wisdom to identify definitions and found 10 major definitions or descriptions.11,13,14,21,22,28,29 Despite some variations in terms, we (T.W.M. and D.V.J.) agreed that the following 6 subcomponents of wisdom were included in at least 3 of these definitions: (1) prosocial attitudes/behaviors, (2) social decision making/pragmatic knowledge of life, (3) emotional homeostasis, (4) reflection/self-understanding, (5) value relativism/tolerance, and (6) acknowledgment of and dealing effectively with uncertainty/ambiguity. Figure 2 presents these subcomponents along with the researchers who included them as a part of their descriptions of wisdom. A few authors have emphasized other domains of wisdom, such as religiosity, intuition, or epistemology, but these were not included in at least 3 of the earlier-mentioned definitions.
We view wisdom as a trait distributed in the general population along a continuum rather than as a rare attribute30 restricted to iconic individuals like Mother Teresa, Mahatma Gandhi, and Nelson Mandela.5 Although it is somewhat stable as a trait within an individual, it is also shaped, to a significant extent, by experience and learning. There are inevitable overlaps between wisdom and other constructs, such as resilience and social cognition, that share certain psychological attributes, including emotional regulation and social decision making. Nonetheless, the construct of wisdom is distinct, as it includes several domains not essential for these other constructs.
Wisdom is considered an important contributor to successful personal and social functioning.19,31 Understanding the neurobiology of wisdom may have considerable clinical significance. For example, knowledge of the underlying mechanisms could potentially lead to development of preventive, therapeutic, and rehabilitative interventions for enhancing wisdom, including those designed for persons with relevant neuropsychiatric disorders (eg, frontotemporal dementia). Yet, neurobiology researchers have stayed away from investigating wisdom, in part, because of difficulties in defining the phenotype. Indeed, we found no studies in a PubMed database search using the keyword “wisdom” in combination with the terms neurobiology, neuroimaging, and neurotransmitters.
We, therefore, decided to examine the literature on the neurobiology of each of the earlier-mentioned 6 subcomponents of wisdom, focusing on their putative neuroanatomical localization determined primarily by functional neuroimaging with a secondary focus on neurotransmitter functions (including their genetic determinants). Studies involving functional neuroimaging or neurotransmitter functioning, examining human (rather than animal) subjects, and identified via a PubMed search using keywords from any of the 6 proposed subcomponents of wisdom were included. These studies were reviewed by both of us (T.W.M. and D.V.J.), and data considered to be potentially relevant to the neurobiology of wisdom were extracted. Possible intermediate phenotypes (the more easily measured emotional and cognitive functions relevant to subcomponents of wisdom) may be localized to certain brain regions. As will be summarized later, the neurobiological substrates of different subcomponents of wisdom seem to include several common regions, such as the prefrontal cortex (PFC), especially dorsolateral PFC (DLPFC), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), medial PFC (MPFC), and anterior cingulate, and certain subcortical structures (mainly amygdala and striatum) and are strongly influenced by monoaminergic pathways.
Neurobiology of subcomponents of wisdom
Prosocial Attitudes and Behaviors
One of the most consistent subcomponents of wisdom, from both ancient and modern literature, is the promotion of common good and rising above self-interests, ie, exhibiting prosocial attitudes and behaviors, such as empathy, social cooperation, and altruism.6 Thus, sociopaths, who may exhibit exquisite social cognition and emotional regulation that actually facilitate their selfish motives, would not be considered wise.
Brain Localization via Neuroimaging.Empathy. Empathy facilitates other prosocial behaviors, including altruism.32,33 Mirror neurons, originally discovered in primates34 and later demonstrated in the human inferior frontal gyrus using neurophysiological methods and functional neuroimaging, may be primitive neurobiological substrates for empathy. In the PFC, mirror neurons fire in the same pattern while a person is performing an action and while watching someone else perform the same action, suggesting their role in appreciating nonverbal communication.35 Persons with greater unconscious somatic mimicry have higher ratings of self-reported altruism.36 When children observe and imitate facial expressions, mirror neurons are activated, and this activity correlates with empathy scores.37 Human empathy is obviously more complex than somatic mimicry. It requires consciously taking the perspective of another person, which is related to the “theory of mind,” developed by Perner and Lang38 as a model of how a person understands other people's mental states and emotions. Neuroimaging research in “theory of mind” tasks has consistently shown MPFC and posterior superior temporal sulcus activation.39-42 The MPFC appears involved in “mentalizing” or conceiving of the inner world of others, whereas posterior superior temporal sulcus activation occurs in response to visual stimuli relevant to internal mental states (eg, body gestures, facial expressions).
A functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study implicated MPFC in perception of shared emotional experiences.43 A meta-analysis of 80 studies concluded that MPFC had a prominent role in empathy.44 Individuals activate MPFC while making empathic social judgments,45 and ventromedial PFC (vmPFC) lesions predict empathic deficits.46 Also critical to empathy is an awareness of self- vs other differentiation to avoid mere emotional contagion. An fMRI study indicated that the superior temporal gyrus and inferior parietal lobe might be critical for self- vs other differentiation of emotions.47
Social Cooperation. Social cooperation (vs competition) likewise appears related to prosocial motives48 and has received attention in neuroimaging research, using a variety of tasks (eg, trust/reciprocity games including the “Prisoner's Dilemma”). Functional MRI studies have demonstrated that social cooperation activates MPFC and nucleus accumbens/ventral striatum, the latter being regions involved in central reward circuitry.49-51 Likewise, “altruistic punishment” (punishment of violators of social norms at cost to oneself) activates reward neurocircuitry.52 In contrast, social competition either decreases activity in areas activated by cooperation53 or activates other regions such as DLPFC.50 In an fMRI study, persons with high sociopathy ratings showed (relative to comparison subjects) decreased amygdala response while being uncooperative during a social cooperation task and less OFC activity while cooperating, suggesting a lack of aversive emotions while violating social norms and a lack of positive emotions while exhibiting social cooperation.54
Altruism. Altruism overlaps with cooperation, although altruism is notable for the potential harm or “decreased fitness” the altruistic person risks to help others.55 Harbaugh and colleagues56 demonstrated that the idea of voluntarily giving money compared with that of paying taxes (still perceived as a needed social good) caused increased activation in reward circuitry (caudate and nucleus accumbens). Similarly, Moll et al57 reported that both receiving monetary rewards and deciding to donate money activated ventral and dorsal striatum. This somewhat paradoxically suggests that the neural substrate of altruism may be akin to that of more instinctual self-pleasures.
Neurotransmitters and Genetics. Several genetic studies have reported that the heritability of prosocial behaviors, including altruism, is 50% to 60%.58-60 Moreover, research indicates involvement of monoamines and certain neuropeptides. This evidence is summarized in Table 1, with findings most notable for the roles of dopamine, serotonin, and the hypothalamic neuropeptides vasopressin and oxytocin in prosocial attitudes/behaviors.
Summary.Table 2 summarizes the role of various brain regions in promoting prosocial behavior (along with the other wisdom subcomponents described later). Prosocial behaviors are facilitated by empathy (rooted in mirror neurons and MPFC) and include social cooperation and altruism (tied to reward neurocircuitry and variations in monoaminergic/hypothalamic peptide functioning).
Social Decision Making/Pragmatic Knowledge of Life
The pragmatic knowledge and skills included in concepts of wisdom have not been directly studied biologically. Implicit in the descriptions of Baltes et al88 of “rich factual knowledge regarding human nature” and “knowledge regarding ways of dealing with life's problems” is the notion of dealing effectively with the constant complex social situations with which humans are confronted. Later we describe several studies related to social cognition and social (including moral) decision making, relevant to this dimension of wisdom. While this overlaps somewhat with the earlier-mentioned concept of prosocial behaviors, there appear to be neurobiological differences between experiencing shared emotions/goals and understanding others' emotions and behaviors. After recognizing and understanding others' emotions and motivations, as “theory of mind” facilitates, one may then use this information to make (or not make) “wise” social decisions.
Brain Localization via Neuroimaging.Decision Making. Ernst and Paulus89 reviewed the neurobiological circuits implicated in decision making, emphasizing differences in regions involved depending on which stage of decision making was being tested—forming a preference, executing an action, or evaluating an outcome. The first and last steps appeared to involve limbic and PFC regions, whereas executing an action was tied to striatal function.89 Montague and Berns90 stress that underlying each decision are both a representation of choices and a short-term (and sometimes distal) evaluation of the consequences of those choices. One aspect of wisdom is balancing choices based on immediate reward vs long-term consequences; as Osbeck and Robinson described, “contemplation of variable things is the function of practical wisom.”91(p68) This apparently involves a “top-down” interaction between the lateral PFC and emotion- and reward-based circuitry in limbic cortex and striatum, akin to regulation of impulsivity, discussed later. Consistent with this notion, McClure and colleagues92 demonstrated that choosing immediate rewards activated limbic and paralimbic cortices, whereas choosing delayed rewards activated DLPFC and parietal regions. However, another report presented contradictory results—increased DLPFC activity in persons prone to immediate reward-based decisions and increased OFC activity with delayed reward-based decisions.93 Whether differences in rewards (money92 vs sugary drinks93) were responsible for discrepant findings in these investigations is unclear. Supporting this latter study, OFC lesions have been reported to increase immediate reward bias and impulsivity.94 In an investigation comparing decision making of adolescents and adults, increased risk-prone decision making was associated with less activity in ventrolateral PFC, OFC, and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (ACC).95 Despite some conflicting results, both OFC and lateral PFC likely play roles in facilitating decisions favoring delayed gratification over immediate reward.
Moral Decision Making. Moral decision-making tasks have been investigated using fMRI. One prerequisite for moral decision making is moral sensitivity, ie, the ability to recognize a moral dilemma. Moral sensitivity is correlated with activity in MPFC, posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and posterior superior temporal sulcus.96 Implicit emotion-based moral attitudes have been linked to amygdala and vmPFC activity.97 Greene et al98 also showed increased MPFC, PCC, and angular gyrus activation in “personal” vs “impersonal” moral reasoning tasks. In contrast, impersonal moral decision-making tasks preferentially activated lateral frontoparietal regions. Consistent with these findings, another study of “simple ethical decisions” (ie, not involving ambiguity, bodily harm, or violence) showed activated temporal and lateral PFC regions.99 The role of PCC may, however, be more related to processing self-relevant emotional stimuli (as opposed to decision making per se), as another report showed increased PCC activity when subjects were presented with a moral dilemma and when they simply passively viewed the dilemma's outcome.100 Greene et al101 examined another angle of moral decision making by comparing neural activity in personal moral decisions vs utilitarian moral judgments, ie, those requiring possible violation of personal moral judgments and emotional self-interests for the sake of common social good (eg, actively sacrificing one person to save the lives of several others). These conflictual, utilitarian-based moral decisions activated ACC (involved in conflict detection) and DLPFC, likely recruited to use more “calculated and rational” thought processes to overcome automatic emotional responses. Consistent with this notion, persons with vmPFC damage were found to have an increased tendency for utilitarian moral decisions.102
Neurotransmitters and Genetics. Limited evidence indicates that dopamine and serotonin play roles in normal and abnormal social cognition; dopamine, in particular, influences reward bias in general decision making. Based largely on results from studies of autism and schizophrenia, 2 disorders with notably impaired social cognition,103 one review outlined how both serotonin and dopamine may play important roles in social cognition, including the ability to mentalize, associated with the “theory of mind.”104
Summary. A number of brain regions are involved in social (including moral) decision making, especially DLPFC, vmPFC, ACC, and amygdala.
Increasingly, wisdom researchers speak of integration of affective control and cognitive processes as being crucial to wisdom.14 Partly underlying emotional regulation is impulse control, which, as discussed earlier, is also relevant to decision making. While these 2 wisdom subcomponents share a substrate of impulse control, there are likely important differences in affective vs cognitive impulsivity. The purported role of emotional regulation in wisdom has centered on inhibiting prolonged negative emotions. Yet, disinhibition of positive emotions, such as happiness, love, and gratitude, which may involve insula and spindle cells, deserves additional research.105
Brain Localization via Neuroimaging.Impulse Control. Neuroimaging studies of impulse control have consistently implicated dorsal ACC and lateral PFC/inferior frontal gyrus.106 Dorsal ACC appears to recognize a conflict between one's instinctual emotional response and a more reasonable overall social goal, whereas lateral PFC may maintain the overarching, more reasonable social goal in working memory and inhibit an inappropriate response. Behavioral inhibition as a specific component of impulse control is often tested with “go/no-go” tasks, designed to assess inhibition of activated or prepotent responses, eg, being asked to tap after hearing 1 tap but do nothing after hearing 2 taps.107 Typically, inferior frontal gyrus activates in “no-go” responses (ie, those that require behavioral inhibition).108
Reappraisal of Emotions. Reappraisal of emotions is a higher-order cognitive task involved in emotional homeostasis. Reframing negative emotional experiences as less aversive may involve recruitment of PFC regions (lateral, medial, and orbitofrontal) to dampen amygdala activity.109-113 Although not as often studied, regulation of positive emotions also seems to involve “top-down” prefrontal inhibition, although perhaps involving different subcortical regions, such as ventral striatum.113
Lieberman et al114 and others115 have also described a form of “unintentional self-regulation” or labeling negative emotions with words (“putting one's feelings into words”). This action appears to increase ventrolateral PFC activity and decrease amygdala activity (similar to intentional cognitive reframing). A key overarching concept in emotional homeostasis is the ability of PFC to inhibit limbic reactivity.
Neurotransmitters and Genetics. As a personality trait, the heritability of impulsivity is approximately 45%.116,117 Dopamine, through its mesocortical pathway, modulates impulsivity, with many studies exploring this in the context of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. The relationship between genetic variants of molecules involved in dopamineric and serotonergic pathways and impulsivity is summarized in Table 1.
Monoamines have long been recognized as regulators of emotion, based largely on studies of psychiatric disorders. More recently, investigators have examined their roles in emotional regulation outside of the context of psychopathology per se, also summarized in Table 1.
Summary. Wisdom necessitates integration of cognitive and emotional functions. Emotional homeostasis has primarily been investigated in relation to downregulation of aversive emotions, often via PFC activation and associated dampening of amygdala activity. Controlling reactions to aversive stimuli is also related to optimal monoaminergic functioning, especially variations related to dopamine and serotonin and genes associated with monoaminergic activity. The role of disinhibiting positive emotions in wisdom warrants further study.
Reflection/Self-understanding
Self-reflection is an essential prerequisite for insight, which is commonly included in many researchers' concept of wisdom.
Brain Localization via Neuroimaging. Uddin and colleagues118 reviewed the concept of a “default mode” neural network, including dorsal and vmPFC, precuneus, and posterior-lateral cortices, that shows high metabolic activity at “baseline” or “rest.” This “rest” likely includes what has been termed task-unrelated imagery and thought, such as autobiographical reminiscence, self-referential thought, and inner speech. In neuroimaging studies, reflecting on one's own current experience consistently activates MPFC. Tasks that involve self-judgment likewise activate MPFC.119 Autobiographical memories activate MPFC and vmPFC, compared with DLPFC activation in nonautobiographical episodic memory.120 Although self-reflection in moderation likely fosters wisdom, there are other types of self-directed internal thought processes related to perseveration, obsessionality, or self-absorption that are antithetical to wisdom, and these may be moderated by lateral PFC.121
Summary. An interaction between medial and lateral PFC seems critical for appropriate self-reflection necessary for insight.
Value Relativism/Tolerance
Tolerance of other persons' or cultures' value systems is often considered an important subcomponent of wisdom.
Brain Localization via Neuroimaging. Neuroimaging studies of tolerance have frequently focused on prominent societal prejudices, especially those related to race/ethnicity. Some investigations have demonstrated that the regulation of “automatic” prejudicial responses follows a neurobiological pattern similar to that described for impulse control: dorsal ACC detects an undesirable attitude surfacing, prompting lateral PFC inhibition of undesirable attitudes, and leading to downstream amygdala deactivation.122,123 While sharing rudimentary neurobiology with impulse control, value relativism is conceptually more complex and its study would benefit from the development of novel measures/tasks. Notably, “theory of mind” studies suggest that lateral PFC lesions impair inhibition of focus on one's own experience, in turn impairing consideration of someone else's state of mind.121,124 Lieberman described how the ability to recognize others' views and values may be linked to lateral PFC inhibitory functions, stating
a failure of this process may play a role in ‘naïve realism’, when individuals assume that others see the world the same way as they do and have difficulty acknowledging alternative viewpoints.125(p263)
Summary. Dorsal ACC and lateral PFC play important roles in tolerance of varied value systems by detecting and inhibiting, respectively, expressions of prejudicial responses.
Acknowledgment of and Dealing Effectively With Uncertainty and Ambiguity
Recognition and emotional tolerance of ambiguity, an important subcomponent of wisdom, have not received adequate attention in biological studies, although they may be partially related to factors described earlier for social decision making and emotional homeostasis.
Brain Localization via Neuroimaging. Krain and colleagues126 reviewed neuroimaging studies of persons confronted with uncertain or ambiguous decisions and contrasted risk-based decision making (where outcomes have known probabilities and subjects choose between “safe” and “risky” decisions) vs decision making in the face of ambiguity (where the probability of specific outcomes is unknown or close to chance, and the choices do not differ in reward value). This meta-analysis concluded that decision making in the face of ambiguity most consistently activated DLPFC, dorsal ACC, insula, and parietal areas. In contrast, decisions involving risk activated OFC, MPFC, caudate, and rostral ACC. Subsequent investigations have supported this conclusion; persons who preferred ambiguous over risky decisions preferentially activated lateral PFC on fMRI.127 Similarly, an fMRI study comparing ambiguous with unambiguous decisions found an association between ambiguity and dorsal ACC and DLPFC activity.128 Overall, these results resonate with a concept proposed by Zelazo and Muller129 that there is a system for “hot” (ie, affectively charged) executive functions (consistent with regions activated in risk-based decision making) and “cold” (ie, analytical) executive functions (consistent with regions activated in decisions made amidst uncertainty).
Summary. Dorsal ACC and lateral PFC activity may be central to rational decision making in the face of ambiguity.
Wisdom is a long-recognized multidimensional and adaptive human attribute. By examining the more consistently identified subcomponents of wisdom, one can begin to hypothesize how such a complex human characteristic may be orchestrated within the human brain.
By using a definition of wisdom based on literature overview, as we have done, we risk relying on an averaged implicit theory of investigators rather than on a definitive model. However, a definitive model for a rather amorphous human trait may need empirical research demonstrating neurobiological basis for validated phenotypes. The ability to measure wisdom objectively is still quite limited, namely because the definition of wisdom is heterogeneous. To stimulate focused neurobiology research in wisdom, a provisional review-based definition serves a purpose, provided one recognizes this as only a first step in a long process.
This review also has other limitations. This was not a meta-analysis and may have overlooked some relevant articles. Furthermore, there may be disagreement regarding the definition and measurement of some of the proposed subcomponents of wisdom. The biological investigations reviewed did not explicitly propose to study wisdom. Many studies included performance-based laboratory tasks, whose validity for assessing specific domains of wisdom may be open to question (eg, how well an in vitro game assesses altruism in real life). A number of studies used fMRI; there are several common limitations to this research, including small sample sizes, difficulty interpreting connectivity or circuitry from isolated regional changes, variations in anatomical definitions of PFC subdivisions, and direct measurement of blood flow rather than neuronal function. Finally, published investigations on neurotransmitters and genetics in relationship to certain subcomponents of wisdom were scarce.
Nonetheless, several common themes from the reviewed biological studies can be summarized (Table 2). Although data on connectivity in the neuroimaging studies are limited, using the available evidence, we propose a working model, admittedly speculative, of how specific brain regions may interact to contribute to subcomponents of wisdom (Figure 3). The lateral PFC (especially DLPFC), often working in concert with dorsal ACC and at times with OFC and MPFC, appears to have an important inhibitory effect on several brain areas associated with emotionality and immediate reward dependence (eg, amygdala, ventral striatum), thereby facilitating the subcomponents described as pragmatic life knowledge and decision making, emotional homeostasis, value relativism, and processing ambiguity. This rational/analytical aspect of wisdom appears to be complemented by a more emotion-based subcomponent, including prosocial attitudes and behaviors that involve MPFC, PCC, OFC, superior temporal sulcus, and reward neurocircuitry. Likewise, MPFC seems to mediate self-reflection and self-awareness; a certain amount of lateral PFC inhibition of this process may, however, be required to stop this function short of maladaptive self-absorption. It is interesting to note the interplay and balance between phylogenetically older brain regions (eg, limbic cortex) and the more recently evolved PFC in the putative neurobiology of wisdom.
Our suggested model of the neurobiology of wisdom raises a conceptual issue. How can our holistic concept of wisdom as a distinct trait, which is consistent with the unified theory of mind,130 be compatible with the reductionism implicit in the relationship between individual brain regions and specific mental functions hypothesized in the neuroimaging research reviewed earlier? We believe that these 2 perspectives can coexist, just as Takahashi and Overton5 have sought to integrate the analytic and synthetic models of wisdom. Examining functional divisions in the brain via fMRI and similar methods is potentially valuable. For example, Jung and Haier131 recently reviewed neuroimaging studies relevant to human intelligence and reasoning. They concluded that there are several distinct brain regions that contribute to intelligence and reasoning and that the coordination among these regions appears to follow a pattern they termed parieto-frontal integration. As expected, there is partial overlap in the brain regions (eg, ACC, DLPFC) implicated in their review and ours. Nonetheless, there are several important characteristics in which wisdom differs from intelligence and reasoning in that it also includes domains such as practical application of knowledge, use of knowledge for the common social good, and integration of affect and knowledge.12,132 Brain regions putatively involved in wisdom that were not prominent in the review of intelligence and reasoning131 include limbic cortex, MPFC, and striatum. Although there is general agreement regarding important functional divisions within the brain, the nature of these divisions is almost assuredly oversimplified and will undergo continual revisions.5 The same would apply to specific subcomponents of wisdom.
The possible neurochemical and genetic contributions to wisdom (Table 1) are related to many of those identified in psychopathology. The fact that monoaminergic functioning is related to stress reactivity and emotional homeostasis is not surprising. The possible roles of oxytocin and vasopressin in prosocial behaviors demonstrate the importance of examining nontraditional neurotransmitters.
Empirical research on wisdom is in its infancy. There are several potentially valuable lines of research that may be suggested.
Defining a valid phenotype: Reliability and validity of theory-based definitions of wisdom should be demonstrated across different populations.
Objectively measuring wisdom: Relatively objective, reliable measures of real-world behaviors should be developed.
Investigating developmental course of wisdom: Wisdom may be studied from a developmental perspective to identify possible critical periods for wisdom development. Advances in genetics/genomics and connectivity analyses in functional neuroimaging as well as electrophysiology may help clarify the interplay between biological and environmental factors in the lifetime course of wisdom.
Examining relationship of wisdom to sociodemographic variables: Older age has been traditionally associated with wisdom but the limited available empirical research does not consistently support this notion.133-135 The neurobiological literature is sparse regarding age-related differences in most of the subcomponents of wisdom discussed. One notable exception is that aging has been associated with better emotional regulation.136 The possible enhancement of wisdom and its specific subcomponents with aging-related cumulative life experience warrants investigation. Cross-cultural comparisons of wisdom and sex differences also deserve research.
Studying neuropsychiatric disorders affecting wisdom: Research in naturally occurring disorders (eg, frontotemporal dementia137 or traumatic injuries [eg, case of Phineas Gage])138 that affect the implicated neurobiological substrates of wisdom would help inform the neurobiology of wisdom as well as clinical applications of the concept.
Using animal models: While wisdom may be uniquely human, certain intermediate phenotypes could be studied in appropriate animal models.
Assessing health care implications of wisdom: Research on possible impact of wisdom on longevity, quality of life, and receipt of improved health care would have major public health significance.
Developing interventions to enhance wisdom: Development and testing of interventions (psychosocial or biological) to enhance wisdom could be valuable for people with and without serious psychopathology. Similarly, whether wisdom moderates the outcomes of other interventions (eg, psychotherapy) would be useful to evaluate.
Wisdom warrants scientific study with the same rigorous methods that we demand in investigations on various forms of psychopathology. At the same time, progress in such research will require maintaining the wisdom to recognize the limits of available scientific methods.
Correspondence: Dilip V. Jeste, MD, Division of Geriatric Psychiatry, University of California, San Diego, 116A-1, VA San Diego Healthcare System, 3350 La Jolla Village Dr, San Diego, CA 92161 (djeste@ucsd.edu).
Submitted for Publication: June 17, 2008; final revision received October 2, 2008; accepted October 6, 2008.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: This work was supported, in part, by grant MH080002 from the National Institute of Mental Health, grant AG26757 from the National Institute on Aging, the US Health Resources and Services Administration (Geriatric Academic Career Award), and by the University of California, San Diego, Sam and Rose Stein Institute for Research on Aging and the Department of Veterans Affairs.
1.Aquinas
T Summa contra Gentiles. Notre Dame, IN University of Notre Dame Press1991;
2.Takahashi
M Toward a culturally inclusive understanding of wisdom: historical roots in the East and West.
Int J Aging Hum Dev 2000;51
(3)
217- 230
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3. Webster's Encyclopedic Dictionary of the English Language. New York, NY Lexicon Publications1990;
4.Jeste
DVVahia
I Comparison of the conceptualization of wisdom in ancient Indian literature with modern views: focus on the Bhagavad Gita.
Psychiatry 2008;71
(3)
197- 209
PubMedGoogle Scholar 5.Takahashi
MOverton
WF Cultural foundations of wisdom: an integrated developmental approach. Sternberg
RJJordan
J
A Handbook of Wisdom Psychological Perspectives. New York, NY Cambridge University Press2005;32- 60
Google Scholar 6.Birren
JESvensson
CM Wisdom in history. Sternberg
RJJordan
J
A Handbook of Wisdom Psychological Perspectives. New York, NY Cambridge University Press2005;3- 31
Google Scholar 7.Brugman
GM Wisdom and Aging. Amsterdam, Netherlands Elsevier2006;
8.Ardelt
M Wisdom as expert knowledge system: a critical review of a contemporary operationalization of an ancient concept.
Hum Dev 2004;47
(5)
257- 285
Google ScholarCrossref 9.Zola-Morgan
S Localization of brain function: the legacy of Franz Joseph Gall (1758-1828).
Annu Rev Neurosci 1995;18359- 383
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Clayton
V Wisdom and intelligence: the nature and function of knowledge in the later years.
Int J Aging Hum Dev 1982;15
(4)
315- 321
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Sternberg
RJ Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development. New York, NY Cambridge University Press1990;
16.Tucker
PMPfefferbaum
BNorth
CSKent
ABurgin
CEParker
DEHossain
AJeon-Slaughter
HTrautman
RP Physiologic reactivity despite emotional resilience several years after direct exposure to terrorism.
Am J Psychiatry 2007;164
(2)
230- 235
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Connor
KMDavidson
JR Development of a new resilience scale: the Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale (CD-RISC).
Depress Anxiety 2003;18
(2)
76- 82
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Charney
DS Psychobiological mechanisms of resilience and vulnerability: implications for successful adaptation to extreme stress.
Am J Psychiatry 2004;161
(2)
195- 216
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Vaillant
GE Does Wisdom Increase With Age? Aging Well: Surprising Guideposts to a Happier Life From the Landmark Harvard Study of Adult Development. Boston, MA Little, Brown and Co2002;249- 257
21.Brugman
GM Wisdom and aging. Birren
JESchaie
KW
Handbook of the Psychology of Aging. 6th ed. Burlington, MA Elsevier Academic Press2005;445- 469
Google Scholar 22.Jason
LAReichler
AKing
CMadsen
DCamacho
JMarchese
W The measurement of wisdom: a preliminary report.
J Community Psychol 2001;29
(5)
585- 598
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Hergott
LJ A piece of my mind. Playing the Moonlight Sonata from memory: celebrating the wonders of our difficult life.
JAMA 2002;288
(20)
2516- 2517
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Keeley
D Rigorous assessment of palliative care revisited: wisdom and compassion are needed when evidence is lacking.
BMJ 1999;319
(7223)
1447- 1448
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Daniele
F First International Conference between West and East—Leonardo and Lao-Tze: Western science meets Eastern wisdom. Experiences of scientists and intellectuals for the creation of a new paradigm of modern science.
Evid Based Complement Alternat Med 2008;5
(1)
51- 54
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Nemiroff
GH Reconstructing Education: Toward a Pedagogy of Critical Humanism. New York, NY Bergin and Garvey1992;
28.Glover
RJ Perspectives on aging: issues affecting the latter part of the life cycle.
Educ Gerontol 1998;24
(4)
325- 331
Google ScholarCrossref 29.Brown
SCGreene
JA The Wisdom Development Scale: translating the conceptual to the concrete.
J Coll Student Dev 2008;47
(1)
1- 19
Google ScholarCrossref 30.Baltes
PBStaudinger
UM Wisdom: a metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence.
Am Psychol 2000;55
(1)
122- 136
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 32.Eisenberg
N Empathy-related responding and prosocial behaviour.
Novartis Found Symp 2007;27871- 80
PubMedGoogle Scholar 34.di Pellegrino
GFadiga
LFogassi
LGallese
VRizzolatti
G Understanding motor events: a neurophysiological study.
Exp Brain Res 1992;91
(1)
176- 180
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Craighero
LMetta
GSandini
GFadiga
L The mirror-neurons system: data and models.
Prog Brain Res 2007;16439- 59
PubMedGoogle Scholar 37.Pfeifer
JHIacoboni
MMazziotta
JCDapretto
M Mirroring others' emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children.
Neuroimage 2008;39
(4)
2076- 2085
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Fletcher
PCHappe
FFrith
UBaker
SCDolan
RJFrackowiak
RSFrith
CD Other minds in the brain: a functional imaging study of “theory of mind” in story comprehension.
Cognition 1995;57
(2)
109- 128
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Gallagher
HLHappé
FBrunswick
NFletcher
PCFrith
CDFrith
CD Reading the mind in cartoons and stories: an fMRI study of ‘theory of mind’ in verbal and nonverbal tasks.
Neuropsychologia 2000;38
(1)
11- 21
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Brunet
ESarfati
YHardy-Bayle
MCDecety
JA PET investigation of the attribution of intentions with a nonverbal task.
Neuroimage 2000;11
(2)
157- 166
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Seitz
RJSchäfer
RScherfeld
DFriederichs
SPopp
KWittsack
HJAzari
NPFranz
M Valuating other people's emotional face expression: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography study.
Neuroscience 2008;152
(3)
713- 722
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Seitz
RJNickel
JAzari
NP Functional modularity of the medial prefrontal cortex: involvement in human empathy.
Neuropsychology 2006;20
(6)
743- 751
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Farrow
TFZheng
YWilkinson
IDSpence
SADeakin
JFTarrier
NGriffiths
PDWoodruff
PW Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness.
Neuroreport 2001;12
(11)
2433- 2438
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Shamay-Tsoory
SGTomer
RBerger
BDAharon-Peretz
J Characterization of empathy deficits following prefrontal brain damage: the role of the right ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
J Cogn Neurosci 2003;15
(3)
324- 337
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Lawrence
EJShaw
PGiampietro
VPSurguladze
SBrammer
MJDavid
AS The role of ‘shared representations’ in social perception and empathy: an fMRI study.
Neuroimage 2006;29
(4)
1173- 1184
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.De Dreu
CKWeingart
LRKwon
S Influence of social motives on integrative negotiation: a meta-analytic review and test of two theories.
J Pers Soc Psychol 2000;78
(5)
889- 905
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Decety
JJackson
PLSommerville
JAChaminade
TMeltzoff
AN The neural bases of cooperation and competition: an fMRI investigation.
Neuroimage 2004;23
(2)
744- 751
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Singer
TKiebel
SJWinston
JSDolan
RJFrith
CD Brain responses to the acquired moral status of faces.
Neuron 2004;41
(4)
653- 662
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.de Quervain
DJFischbacher
UTreyer
VSchellhammer
MSchnyder
UBuck
AFehr
E The neural basis of altruistic punishment.
Science 2004;305
(5688)
1254- 1258
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Rilling
JKSanfey
AGAronson
JANystrom
LECohen
JD Opposing BOLD responses to reciprocated and unreciprocated altruism in putative reward pathways.
Neuroreport 2004;15
(16)
2539- 2543
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Rilling
JKGlenn
ALJairam
MRPagnoni
GGoldsmith
DRElfenbein
HALilienfeld
SO Neural correlates of social cooperation and non-cooperation as a function of psychopathy.
Biol Psychiatry 2007;61
(11)
1260- 1271
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Lehmann
LKeller
L The evolution of cooperation and altruism—a general framework and a classification of models.
J Evol Biol 2006;19
(5)
1365- 1376
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Harbaugh
WTMayr
UBurghart
DR Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations.
Science 2007;316
(5831)
1622- 1625
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Moll
JKrueger
FZahn
RPardini
MOliveira-Souza
RGrafman
J Human fronto-mesolimbic networks guide decisions about charitable donation.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103
(42)
15623- 15628
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Hur
YMRushton
JP Genetic and environmental contributions to prosocial behaviour in 2- to 9-year-old South Korean twins.
Biol Lett 2007;3
(6)
664- 666
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Rushton
JP Genetic and environmental contributions to pro-social attitudes: a twin study of social responsibility.
Proc Biol Sci 2004;271
(1557)
2583- 2585
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Rushton
JPFulker
DWNeale
MCNias
DKEysenck
HJ Altruism and aggression: the heritability of individual differences.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1986;50
(6)
1192- 1198
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Bachner-Melman
RGritsenko
INemanov
LZohar
AHDina
CEbstein
RP Dopaminergic polymorphisms associated with self-report measures of human altruism: a fresh phenotype for the dopamine D4 receptor.
Mol Psychiatry 2005;10
(4)
333- 335
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Reeves
SJMehta
MAMontgomery
AJAmiras
DEgerton
AHoward
RJGrasby
PM Striatal dopamine (D2) receptor availability predicts socially desirable responding.
Neuroimage 2007;34
(4)
1782- 1789
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Young
SESmolen
AHewitt
JKHaberstick
BCStallings
MCCorley
RPCrowley
TJ Interaction between MAO-A genotype and maltreatment in the risk for conduct disorder: failure to confirm in adolescent patients.
Am J Psychiatry 2006;163
(6)
1019- 1025
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Wood
RMRilling
JKSanfey
AGBhagwager
ZRogers
RD Effects of tryptophan depletion on the performance of an iterated Prisoner's Dilemma game in healthy adults.
Neuropsychopharmacology 2006;31
(5)
1075- 1084
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Knafo
AIsrael
SDarvasi
ABachner-Melman
RUzefovsky
FCohen
LFeldman
ELerer
ELaiba
ERaz
YNemanov
LGritsenko
IDina
CAgam
GDean
BBornstein
GEbstein
RP Individual differences in allocation of funds in the dictator game associated with length of the arginine vasopressin 1a receptor RS3 promoter region and correlation between RS3 length and hippocampal mRNA.
Genes Brain Behav 2008;7
(3)
266- 275
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Jacob
SBrune
CWCarter
CSLeventhal
BLLord
CCook
EH
Jr Association of the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) in Caucasian children and adolescents with autism.
Neurosci Lett 2007;417
(1)
6- 9
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 67.Faraone
SVDoyle
AEMick
EBiederman
J Meta-analysis of the association between the 7-repeat allele of the dopamine D(4) receptor gene and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 2001;158
(7)
1052- 1057
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Cornish
KMManly
TSavage
RSwanson
JMorisano
DButler
NGrant
CCross
GBentley
LHollis
CP Association of the dopamine transporter (DAT1) 10/10-repeat genotype with ADHD symptoms and response inhibition in a general population sample.
Mol Psychiatry 2005;10
(7)
686- 698
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 69.Eisenberg
JMei-Tal
GSteinberg
ATartakovsky
EZohar
AGritsenko
INemanov
LEbstein
RP Haplotype relative risk study of catechol-O-methyltransferase (COMT) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): association of the high-enzyme activity Val allele with ADHD impulsive-hyperactive phenotype.
Am J Med Genet 1999;88
(5)
497- 502
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Drabant
EMHariri
ARMeyer-Lindenberg
AMunoz
KEMattay
VSKolachana
BSEgan
MFWeinberger
DR Catechol O-methyltransferase val158met genotype and neural mechanisms related to affective arousal and regulation.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63
(12)
1396- 1406
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Nomura
MNomura
Y Psychological, neuroimaging, and biochemical studies on functional association between impulsive behavior and the 5-HT2A receptor gene polymorphism in humans.
Ann N Y Acad Sci 2006;1086134- 143
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 72.Frankle
WGLombardo
INew
ASGoodman
MTalbot
PSHuang
YHwang
DRSlifstein
MCurry
SAbi-Dargham
ALaruelle
MSiever
LJ Brain serotonin transporter distribution in subjects with impulsive aggressivity: a positron emission study with [11C]McN 5652.
Am J Psychiatry 2005;162
(5)
915- 923
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 73.Meyer-Lindenberg
ABuckholtz
JWKolachana
BHariri
APezawas
LBlasi
GWabnitz
AHonea
RVerchinski
BCallicott
JHEgan
MMattay
VWeinberger
DR Neural mechanisms of genetic risk for impulsivity and violence in humans.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103
(16)
6269- 6274
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 74.Passamonti
LFera
FMagariello
ACerasa
AGioia
MCMuglia
MNicoletti
GGallo
OProvinciali
LQuattrone
A Monoamine oxidase-a genetic variations influence brain activity associated with inhibitory control: new insight into the neural correlates of impulsivity.
Biol Psychiatry 2006;59
(4)
334- 340
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 75.Hariri
ARMattay
VSTessitore
AKolachana
BFera
FGoldman
DEgan
MFWeinberger
DR Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala.
Science 2002;297
(5580)
400- 403
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 76.Heinz
ABraus
DFSmolka
MNWrase
JPuls
IHermann
DKlein
SGrüsser
SMFlor
HSchumann
GMann
KBüchel
C Amygdala-prefrontal coupling depends on a genetic variation of the serotonin transporter.
Nat Neurosci 2005;8
(1)
20- 21
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 77.Hariri
ARDrabant
EMMunoz
KEKolachana
BSMattay
VSEgan
MFWeinberger
DR A susceptibility gene for affective disorders and the response of the human amygdala.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62
(2)
146- 152
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 78.Canli
TOmura
KHaas
BWFallgatter
AConstable
RTLesch
KP Beyond affect: a role for genetic variation of the serotonin transporter in neural activation during a cognitive attention task.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2005;102
(34)
12224- 12229
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 79.Pezawas
LMeyer-Lindenberg
ADrabant
EMVerchinski
BAMunoz
KEKolachana
BSEgan
MFMattay
VSHariri
ARWeinberger
DR 5-HTTLPR polymorphism impacts human cingulate-amygdala interactions: a genetic susceptibility mechanism for depression.
Nat Neurosci 2005;8
(6)
828- 834
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.David
SPMurthy
NVRabiner
EAMunafó
MRJohnstone
ECJacob
RWalton
RTGrasby
PM A functional genetic variation of the serotonin (5-HT) transporter affects 5-HT1A receptor binding in humans.
J Neurosci 2005;25
(10)
2586- 2590
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 81.Caspi
ASugden
KMoffitt
TETaylor
ACraig
IWHarrington
HMcClay
JMill
JMartin
JBraithwaite
APoulton
R Influence of life stress on depression: moderation by a polymorphism in the 5HTT gene.
Science 2003;301
(5631)
386- 389
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 82.Canli
TQiu
MOmura
KCongdon
EHaas
BWAmin
ZHerrmann
MJConstable
RTLesch
KP Neural correlates of epigenesis.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 2006;103
(43)
16033- 16038
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 83.Gutknecht
LJacob
CStrobel
AKriegebaum
CMuller
JZeng
YMarkert
CEscher
AWendland
JReif
AMossner
RGross
CBrocke
BLesch
KP Tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation influences personality traits and disorders related to emotional dysregulation.
Int J Neuropsychopharmacol 2007;10
(3)
309- 320
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 84.Canli
TCongdon
EGutknecht
LConstable
RTLesch
KP Amygdala responsiveness is modulated by tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation.
J Neural Transm 2005;112
(11)
1479- 1485
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 85.Herrmann
MJHuter
TMuller
FMuhlberger
APauli
PReif
ARenner
TCanli
TFallgatter
AJLesch
KP Additive effects of serotonin transporter and tryptophan hydroxylase-2 gene variation on emotional processing.
Cereb Cortex 2007;17
(5)
1160- 1163
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 86.Neumeister
ACharney
DSBelfer
IGeraci
MHolmes
CSharabi
YAlim
TBonne
OLuckenbaugh
DAManji
HGoldman
DGoldstein
DS Sympathoneural and adrenomedullary functional effects of alpha2C-adrenoreceptor gene polymorphism in healthy humans.
Pharmacogenet Genomics 2005;15
(3)
143- 149
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 87.Morgan
CA
IIIWang
SRasmusson
AHazlett
GAnderson
GCharney
DS Relationship among plasma cortisol, catecholamines, neuropeptide Y, and human performance during exposure to uncontrollable stress.
Psychosom Med 2001;63
(3)
412- 422
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 88.Baltes
PBSmith
JStaudinger
UM Wisdom and successful aging. Sonderegger
T
Nebraska Symposium on Motivation. Lincoln University of Nebraska Press1992;123- 167
Google Scholar 89.Ernst
MPaulus
MP Neurobiology of decision making: a selective review from a neurocognitive and clinical perspective.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;58
(8)
597- 604
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 91.Osbeck
LMRobinson
DN Philosophical theories of wisdom. Sternberg
RJJordan
J
A Handbook of Wisdom Psychological Perspectives. New York, NY Cambridge University Press2005;3- 31
Google Scholar 92.McClure
SMLaibson
DILoewenstein
GCohen
JD Separate neural systems value immediate and delayed monetary rewards.
Science 2004;306
(5695)
503- 507
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 93.Boettiger
CAMitchell
JMTavares
VCRobertson
MJoslyn
GD'Esposito
MFields
HL Immediate reward bias in humans: fronto-parietal networks and a role for the catechol-O-methyltransferase 158(Val/Val) genotype.
J Neurosci 2007;27
(52)
14383- 14391
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 94.Berlin
HARolls
ETKischka
U Impulsivity, time perception, emotion and reinforcement sensitivity in patients with orbitofrontal cortex lesions.
Brain 2004;127
(pt 5)
1108- 1126
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 95.Eshel
NNelson
EEBlair
RJPine
DSErnst
M Neural substrates of choice selection in adults and adolescents: development of the ventrolateral prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices.
Neuropsychologia 2007;45
(6)
1270- 1279
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 96.Robertson
DSnarey
JOusley
OHarenski
KDuBois
BFGilkey
RKilts
C The neural processing of moral sensitivity to issues of justice and care.
Neuropsychologia 2007;45
(4)
755- 766
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 97.Luo
QNakic
MWheatley
TRichell
RMarton
ABlair
RJ The neural basis of implicit moral attitude—an IAT study using event-related fMRI.
Neuroimage 2006;30
(4)
1449- 1457
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 98.Greene
JDSommerville
RBNystrom
LEDarley
JMCohen
JD An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment.
Science 2001;293
(5537)
2105- 2108
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 99.Heekeren
HRWartenburger
ISchmidt
HSchwintowski
HPVillringer
A An fMRI study of simple ethical decision-making.
Neuroreport 2003;14
(9)
1215- 1219
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 100.Pujol
JReixach
JHarrison
BJTimoneda-Gallart
CVilanova
JCPerez-Alvarez
F Posterior cingulate activation during moral dilemma in adolescents.
Hum Brain Mapp 2008;29
(8)
910- 921
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 101.Greene
JDNystrom
LEEngell
ADDarley
JMCohen
JD The neural bases of cognitive conflict and control in moral judgment.
Neuron 2004;44
(2)
389- 400
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 102.Koenigs
MYoung
LAdolphs
RTranel
DCushman
FHauser
MDamasio
A Damage to the prefrontal cortex increases utilitarian moral judgements.
Nature 2007;446
(7138)
908- 911
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 103.Green
MFPenn
DLBentall
RCarpenter
WTGaebel
WGur
RCKring
AMPark
SSilverstein
SMHeinssen
R Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities.
Schizophr Bull 2008;34
(6)
1211- 1220
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 105.Vaillant
G Spiritual Evolution: A Scientific Defense of Faith. New York, NY Broadway2008;
106.Congdon
ECanli
T The endophenotype of impulsivity: reaching consilience through behavioral, genetic, and neuroimaging approaches.
Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev 2005;4
(4)
262- 281
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 107.Aron
ARFletcher
PCBullmore
ETSahakian
BJRobbins
TW Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans.
Nat Neurosci 2003;6
(2)
115- 116
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 108.Horn
NRDolan
MElliott
RDeakin
JFWoodruff
PW Response inhibition and impulsivity: an fMRI study.
Neuropsychologia 2003;41
(14)
1959- 1966
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 109.Phan
KLFitzgerald
DANathan
PJMoore
GJUhde
TWTancer
ME Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;57
(3)
210- 219
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 110.Goldin
PRMcRae
KRamel
WGross
JJ The neural bases of emotion regulation: reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion.
Biol Psychiatry 2008;63
(6)
577- 586
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 111.Ochsner
KNBunge
SAGross
JJGabrieli
JD Rethinking feelings: an FMRI study of the cognitive regulation of emotion.
J Cogn Neurosci 2002;14
(8)
1215- 1229
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 112.Cooney
REJoormann
JAtlas
LYEugène
FGotlib
IH Remembering the good times: neural correlates of affect regulation.
Neuroreport 2007;18
(17)
1771- 1774
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 114.Lieberman
MDEisenberger
NICrockett
MJTom
SMPfeifer
JHWay
BM Putting feelings into words: affect labeling disrupts amygdala activity in response to affective stimuli.
Psychol Sci 2007;18
(5)
421- 428
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 115.Hariri
ARBookheimer
SYMazziotta
JC Modulating emotional responses: effects of a neocortical network on the limbic system.
Neuroreport 2000;11
(1)
43- 48
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 116.Pedersen
NLPlomin
RMcClearn
GEFriberg
L Neuroticism, extraversion, and related traits in adult twins reared apart and reared together.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1988;55
(6)
950- 957
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 117.Seroczynski
ADBergeman
CSCoccaro
EF Etiology of the impulsivity/aggression relationship: genes or environment?
Psychiatry Res 1999;86
(1)
41- 57
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 118.Uddin
LQIacoboni
MLange
CKeenan
JP The self and social cognition: the role of cortical midline structures and mirror neurons.
Trends Cogn Sci 2007;11
(4)
153- 157
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 119.Fossati
PHevenor
SJGraham
SJGrady
CKeightley
MLCraik
FMayberg
H In search of the emotional self: an FMRI study using positive and negative emotional words.
Am J Psychiatry 2003;160
(11)
1938- 1945
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 120.Gilboa
A Autobiographical and episodic memory—one and the same? Evidence from prefrontal activation in neuroimaging studies.
Neuropsychologia 2004;42
(10)
1336- 1349
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 121.Samson
DApperly
IAKathirgamanathan
UHumphreys
GW Seeing it my way: a case of a selective deficit in inhibiting self-perspective.
Brain 2005;128
(pt 5)
1102- 1111
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 122.Amodio
DMHarmon-Jones
EDevine
PGCurtin
JJHartley
SLCovert
AE Neural signals for the detection of unintentional race bias.
Psychol Sci 2004;15
(2)
88- 93
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 123.Cunningham
WAJohnson
MKRaye
CLChris
GJGore
JCBanaji
MR Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces.
Psychol Sci 2004;15
(12)
806- 813
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 124.Samson
DApperly
IAHumphreys
GW Error analyses reveal contrasting deficits in “theory of mind”: neuropsychological evidence from a 3-option false belief task.
Neuropsychologia 2007;45
(11)
2561- 2569
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 126.Krain
ALWilson
AMArbuckle
RCastellanos
FXMilham
MP Distinct neural mechanisms of risk and ambiguity: a meta-analysis of decision-making.
Neuroimage 2006;32
(1)
477- 484
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 127.Huettel
SAStowe
CJGordon
EMWarner
BTPlatt
ML Neural signatures of economic preferences for risk and ambiguity.
Neuron 2006;49
(5)
765- 775
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 128.Simmons
AStein
MBMatthews
SCFeinstein
JSPaulus
MP Affective ambiguity for a group recruits ventromedial prefrontal cortex.
Neuroimage 2006;29
(2)
655- 661
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 129.Zelazo
PDMuller
U Executive function in typical and atypical development. Goswami
U
Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development. Oxford, England Blackwell2002;445- 489
Google Scholar 131.Jung
REHaier
RJ The Parieto-Frontal Integration Theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: converging neuroimaging evidence.
Behav Brain Sci 2007;30
(2)
135- 154
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 133.Baltes
PBStaudinger
UMaercker
ASmith
J People nominated as wise: a comparative study of wisdom related knowledge.
Psychol Aging 1995;10
(2)
155- 166
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 134.Staudinger
UMBaltes
PB Interactive minds: a facilitative setting for wisdom-related performance.
J Pers Soc Psychol 1996;71
(4)
746- 762
Google ScholarCrossref 136.Mather
MCartensen
LL Aging and motivated cognition: the positivity effect in attention and memory.
Trends Cogn Sci 2005;9
(10)
496- 502
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 137.Rankin
KPSantos-Modesitt
WKramer
JHPavlic
DBeckman
VMiller
BL Spontaneous social behaviors discriminate behavioral dementias from psychiatric disorders and other dementias.
J Clin Psychiatry 2008;69
(1)
60- 73
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref