Context
Several data sources suggest a link between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (BD); however, family studies have not revealed coaggregation of these disorders.
Objectives
To systematically review family studies of probands with schizophrenia and BD and to determine whether these disorders coaggregate in families.
Data Sources
MEDLINE and PsycINFO databases.
Study Selection
All family studies published from January 1, 1980, to December 31, 2006, reporting morbid risk or raw counts of schizophrenia or BD in first-degree relatives (FDRs) of a proband group with DSM-III or later; International Classification of Diseases, Ninth or Tenth Revision; or research diagnostic criteria schizophrenia or BD were included. Of the original 2326 studies identified through the database search, 38 studies were used to investigate rates of BD in FDRs of probands with schizophrenia, while 39 studies were used to examine rates of schizophrenia in FDRs of BD probands.
Data Extraction
Data were analyzed with a novel random-effects bootstrapping technique that allows for the inclusion of studies lacking a patient or control group, which made up a substantial portion of the available data. Data were also blindly weighted for methodological quality.
Data Synthesis
The FDRs of probands with schizophrenia showed significantly (P = .01) increased rates of BD relative to control FDRs (odds ratio [OR] = 2.08). The FDRs of probands with BD showed marginally (P = .06) increased rates of schizophrenia relative to control FDRs (OR = 2.10); this analysis was significant (P = .02) when studies that did not report morbid risk estimates were excluded (in this case, OR = 3.49).
Conclusions
This meta-analysis provides direct evidence for familial coaggregation of schizophrenia and BD, a finding that argues against the view that these disorders are entirely discrete diagnostic entities. Rather, a continuum model is supported.
An enduring question in the genetics of major mental illness is the extent of overlap between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder (BD). Genetic linkage studies have identified susceptibility loci that both disorders share,1-4 and the only twin study that has assigned diagnoses nonhierarchically (such that individuals meeting criteria for multiple disorders would be assigned all diagnoses) found significant genetic overlap among schizophrenia, BD, and schizoaffective disorder (SD).5 Furthermore, family studies have shown familial coaggregation of SD and unipolar depression in both schizophrenia6-9 and BD.8,10,11 However, evidence for direct familial coaggregation of schizophrenia and BD is scarce, and the replicability of linkage studies that show shared susceptibility genes between these disorders has been poor.12 An extensive literature search (see “Methods” section) failed to reveal any studies that demonstrated increased risk of BD in the families of probands with schizophrenia, as did an earlier meta-analysis of 3 family studies.13 While at least 2 studies of BD probands have revealed increased risk of schizophrenia,10,14 many more have not.7,8,11
Given the shared genetic loading for SD and unipolar illness in both schizophrenia and BD, a lack of familial aggregation between these 2 disorders is somewhat puzzling. A potential explanation for negative findings is lack of power, because statistical tests for binary events with low expected frequencies suffer from low power. If the true risk of BD in the general population is 1% and the odds ratio (OR) of risk of BD in first-degree relatives (FDRs) of probands with schizophrenia over control probands is small (1.5 in this example), a family study would require assessment of roughly 7800 FDRs to achieve 50% power to detect an effect (assuming symmetrically distributed error variance); to our knowledge, no family studies of either schizophrenia or BD have approached this size. This power problem is due both to the low prevalence of these disorders in the population and, in this example, to the low OR. Thus, if the true risk of BD were instead 10%, one would need fewer than 900 FDRs to achieve 50% power with an OR of 1.5; whereas if the risk were 1% but the OR was 5, one would need fewer than 300 FDRs. Therefore, while the available literature rules out rates of BD in FDRs of patients with schizophrenia that are in excess of 3 or 4 times the normal rate, existing studies have not had sufficient power to detect effects much smaller than this (naturally, the same argument applies to rates of schizophrenia in the families of patients with BD).
One way to increase power is to combine the results of multiple studies using meta-analysis. However, many family studies lack an appropriate control group and so cannot be used in traditional meta-analyses that calculate a measure of association (eg, Φ, a correlation coefficient for contingency tables) for each study and assess significance across studies, because these measures require a control group. Consequently, we developed a novel meta-analytic procedure that allows inclusion of all studies reporting rates of schizophrenia or BD in family members of patient or control probands (and not necessarily both) to determine whether there is evidence of familial coaggregation of schizophrenia and BD.
We attempted to identify all family studies that (1) were published in English between January 1, 1980, and December 31, 2006; (2) included schizophrenia or BD proband groups; (3) reported lifetime counts or morbid risks of schizophrenia or BD in probands' FDRs; and (4) used 1 of the following diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia or BD: the research diagnostic criteria of Spitzer et al15; DSM-III, DSM-III-R, or DSM-IV-TR; or International Classification of Diseases, Ninth (ICD-9) or Tenth (ICD-10) Revision. We identified candidate studies through a search of the MEDLINE and PsycINFO databases using the keywords listed in the eTable (available at http://www.archgenpsychiatry.com), resulting in 2326 hits.
When 2 or more studies investigated the same proband group, we compared the quality of data from each study and used only the best available data (see below), such that data were only included in the analysis once. In a few cases, we used 1 study's data on a proband group's familial rates of schizophrenia and another study's data on the same proband group's familial rates of BD. None of the probands included in the meta-analysis had a diagnosis of SD, as it is known to coaggregate with both schizophrenia and BD. When multiple studies reported on the same probands, the study we included was based on the following criteria (in decreasing order of importance): (1) recency of diagnostic criteria used; (2) diagnostic methods used (eg, direct interviews preferred to questionnaires); (3) number of data sources used for diagnosis; (4) size of proband and relative samples; (5) availability of morbid risk counts in preference to lifetime counts; and (6) morbid risk methods, preferring methods given lower penalty scores in the weighting procedure described below.
A total of 66 studies were retained for meta-analysis; of these, 12 reported rates of BD in relatives of schizophrenic probands,6,7,16-25 13 reported rates of schizophrenia in relatives of BD probands,8,10,14,26-35 and 6 reported both rates of BD in relatives of schizophrenic probands and rates of schizophrenia in relatives of BD probands.36-41 This resulted in a total of 18 studies reporting rates of BD in at least (exact numbers were not available for all studies) 8896 relatives of 2084 schizophrenic probands and 19 studies reporting rates of schizophrenia in at least 7066 relatives of 1312 BD probands. These numbers of relatives are minimums, because some studies report only the number of relatives at risk after estimation of morbid risk—commonly known as Bezugsziffer—and not the total number of relatives assessed.
Therefore, 31 of the 66 studies included the data of primary interest, while the remaining 35 studies reported rates of either BD or schizophrenia in relatives of probands with the same disorder. Of all of the 66 studies, any that included data on rates of schizophrenia or BD in relatives of control probands were used in our primary analyses. Thus, from the full set of 66 studies, in addition to the data already described, 38 studies6,7,16-18,20-25,33,36-61 reported rates of schizophrenia in 24 106 relatives of 5366 probands with schizophrenia, 32 studies8,10,26-32,34-41,62-76 reported rates of BD in 12 558 relatives of 2576 probands with BD, 20 studies6-8,10,16-18,21,23-25,31,40,43,48,50,51,53,54,59 reported rates of schizophrenia in 8356 relatives of 1571 control probands, and 18 studies6-8,10,16-18,21,23-25,31,40,63-65,69,76 reported rates of BD in 8640 relatives of 1777 control probands.
Unfortunately, 8 of the studies that reported familial rates of BD in probands with schizophrenia and 15 of the studies that reported rates of schizophrenia in the families of BD probands did not report data on a control group; if we used a procedure that could not accommodate this missing data, there would only be 10 and 4 studies, respectively, remaining in these analyses. Thus, to include an adequate number of studies in our analyses, we had to use a procedure that would allow us to treat the absence of a control group as missing data—an approach that is valid, provided that data missingness is not systematically related to the outcome of interest (ie, studies lacking a control group do not have systematically higher or lower diagnostic rates than other studies).
An intuitive approach is to simply add up the raw counts across all of the available studies into a single contingency table and carry out a χ2 test. However, this is a fixed-effects approach that is not appropriate in cases in which there is meaningful between-study variance. Because the studies included in our analysis vary on a large number of potentially relevant dimensions (eg, diagnostic criteria, methods of proband ascertainment, and diagnostic methods in family members), it is reasonable to expect there to be meaningful sources of variance between studies, in which case fixed-effects models inflate type I error rates beyond the nominal value of α, sometimes quite substantially.77 Consequently, we developed a meta-analytic technique that allows missing studies to be included in a random-effects model and used bootstrapping procedures to assess the statistical significance of the results. To our knowledge, this is the only valid random-effects approach that allows for the inclusion of studies with missing data. All analyses were carried out using Matlab (The MathWorks Inc, Natick, Massachusetts).
First, the table P, with proband diagnoses (patient or control) in rows and relative diagnoses (including no diagnosis) in columns, is defined as
where Pij is the value in the ith row and the jth column of P, there are Ki studies in the analysis that report complete data for the ith row, wk is the study quality weight for the kth study (see “Study Weighting” section), dijk is the number of observations in the ith row and the jth column for the kth study, and ni.k is the number of observations in the ith row of the kth study (the marginal value for the ith row). Thus, Pij gives the weighted proportion of relatives of probands with the ith diagnosis who are expected to receive the diagnosis in the jth column across the Ki studies, with the weight given to each study determined by the product of the study quality weight, wk, and the square root of the sample size (which is inversely proportional to the variance of each study).
Once the table P is calculated, the correlation coefficient Φ and the OR can be computed; however, because only 1 contingency table (with all values bounded by 0 and 1) is calculated, it is not possible to obtain P values or confidence intervals (CIs) using parametric statistics such as χ2. However, bootstrapping can be used to obtain the empirical distribution of Φ and the OR, and bootstrapped CIs and P values can be calculated. For each of the analyses described below, we used 100 000 bootstrap resamplings to calculate a bias-corrected bootstrap estimate.78 The full procedure for calculating the bias-corrected bootstrap for the table P is described in the eAppendix.
In the course of analyzing the data, it became clear that the OR produces a very large bias estimate in this procedure owing to the extreme skewedness of its distribution. That is, because the OR is bounded by 0 and infinity but has an expected value of 1 under the null hypothesis, the distribution of the statistic has a strong positive skew. The bias estimate correctly detects this is a strong positive bias, and so the bootstrapped values of the statistic are substantially reduced, resulting in a loss of power relative to an unbiased statistic. Because Φ is approximately normally distributed when its value is close to 0, its estimated bias is very small. Consequently, for each of our analyses, we report bootstrapped P values and CIs only for Φ and use the OR simply as a descriptive statistic.
We performed separate analyses to determine whether FDRs of patients with schizophrenia exhibit increased rates of BD and whether FDRs of patients with BD exhibit increased rates of schizophrenia. In addition, we also report on analyses of the rate of schizophrenia in FDRs of patients with schizophrenia and the rate of BD in FDRs of patients with BD. While it is already well known that both of these disorders show strong familial aggregation, a report of the average Φ and OR across studies could be valuable to some readers, and so we report them herein.
Another issue with combining studies in this literature is that some studies report morbid risk estimates (in which the number of relatives assessed for a disorder is reduced by some proportion to account for the fact that some are expected to develop a disorder later in life, after the study is carried out), while others report raw counts of the number of ill and healthy relatives. Including both types of data in a single analysis results in substantial variance between the 2 types of study, because raw counts will produce systematically lower estimates of risk than morbid risk estimates. Consequently, we used the entire set of studies reporting both raw counts and morbid risk of schizophrenia and BD and calculated the mean reduction in the number of relatives at risk after morbid risk estimation (ie, Bezugsziffer divided by the raw number of relatives assessed) for each disorder (for schizophrenia, mean = 0.717 [standard deviation = 0.149]; for BD, mean = 0.634 [standard deviation = 0.166]). We then reduced the number of relatives used for calculating P (ie, ni.k) from studies that reported only raw counts by this proportion, so that risk estimates from both study types would be on par. We additionally reduced the weight given to studies using raw counts (see “Study Weighting” section) to reflect the fact that the morbid risk adjustment we used reflects an estimate, rather than actual morbid risk estimation in the study in question. Finally, to evaluate the extent to which inclusion of studies reporting only raw counts influenced the results of our analyses, we carried out an additional set of analyses that excluded these studies altogether.
To give greater weight to studies of higher methodological quality, we calculated a penalty score, Qk, for each included study. Various methodological qualities were assigned a penalty value, all of which were summed to generate the penalty score for that study. We assigned penalty values blind to study outcomes. Weights were then calculated as wk = 1/(2 + Qk). The mean of the weights was 0.276 (standard deviation = 0.074) and the range was 0.150 to 0.447. In addition to giving greater weight to higher quality studies, this procedure is used to minimize any between-study variance due to higher or lower diagnostic rates being systematically reported by studies of poorer methodological quality. To ensure that weighting has not unduly influenced the results, we report results for both weighted and unweighted data (in which all values of w are set to 1) for each of our analyses. To the extent that the weighted and unweighted analyses produce comparable findings, one can be confident that the weighting procedure is not driving the results.
Higher penalty scores were given for the following methodological features: (1) diagnostic criteria differing from those of the DSM-IV; (2) direct clinical interviews not used for proband diagnosis; (3) relative diagnoses assigned without a blinding procedure; (4) relative diagnoses based solely on medical or other records; (5) use of family history method (as opposed to direct interviews of relatives), with lower penalties for using more family members as informants; (6) no assessment of interrater reliability or use of consensus diagnosis for relatives’ diagnoses; (7) no morbid risk estimation; (8) use of morbid risk procedures that do not directly use age-of-onset information in the data (eg, the Weinberg method); (9) use of the modified Stromgren procedure, as it has been shown to be heavily biased79; (10) inclusion of probands with other comorbid psychiatric disorders; (11) use of a special population (eg, adolescent-onset patients, adoptees, and twins); (12) use of only drug responders or nonresponders, or only male or female probands; (13) use of an unrepresentative control group (students and nurses); (14) relaxed diagnostic criteria (eg, inclusion of individuals with schizophreniform disorder); (15) Bezugsziffer had to be estimated based on the reported age-of-onset distribution and number of relatives assessed, because it was not reported directly; and (16) only probands with type I or type II BD were included, not both, because of the possibility that one of these types could show more or less coaggregation with schizophrenia than BD as a whole (too few studies reported both separately to adequately assess this possibility). In cases in which a penalty applied only to some proportion of the relatives assessed (eg, when some number of assessed relatives were interviewed directly and the rest were assessed via family history methods), the penalty score was multiplied by the proportion of the total number of relatives to which the weight applied. The precise penalties used, and the number of studies receiving them, are given in the Table.
RATES OF BD IN FDRs OF PROBANDS WITH SCHIZOPHRENIA
The risk of BD in FDRs of probands with schizophrenia was 0.99% compared with 0.48% in control probands (OR = 2.08; Φ = 0.030). This difference was significant (P = .01; 95% CI for Φ = 0.008-0.050). The distribution and 95% CI of Φ in the 100 000 bootstrap resamplings of the data are shown in Figure 1. Results were largely unchanged in the unweighted analysis (P = .01), with estimated risk of BD in FDRs at 1.02% for probands with schizophrenia and 0.50% for control probands (OR = 2.02; Φ = 0.029; 95% CI = 0.007-0.051).
Results from the analysis excluding studies that did not use morbid risk estimates were very similar, with risk of BD in the FDRs of probands with schizophrenia estimated at 1.01% as opposed to 0.50% in control probands (OR = 2.06; Φ = 0.030). As with the more inclusive analysis, these results were significant (P = .02; 95% CI for Φ = 0.005-0.052). Again, little change was seen in the unweighted analysis, with risks at 1.04% for FDRs of probands with schizophrenia and 0.54% for control probands (P = .03; OR = 1.95; Φ = 0.029; 95% CI = 0.003-0.052).
RATES OF SCHIZOPHRENIA IN THE FDRs OF PROBANDS WITH BD
The risk of schizophrenia in the FDRs of probands with BD was 1.77% compared with 0.85% in control probands (OR = 2.10; Φ = 0.041). This difference was marginally significant (P = .06; 95% CI for Φ = −0.001 to 0.077). The distribution and 95% CI of Φ for these data are shown in Figure 2. Results in the unweighted analysis were similar, with risk of schizophrenia in the FDRs of BD probands at 1.74% and at 1.00% for control probands. This difference was not significant (P = .13; OR = 1.77; Φ = 0.032; 95% CI = −0.010 to 0.072).
When studies that did not report morbid risk data were excluded from the analysis, the estimated risk of schizophrenia was 2.04% in the FDRs of BD probands and 0.59% in the FDRs of control probands (P = .02; OR = 3.49; Φ = 0.063; 95% CI = 0.013-0.108). This result was also significant in the unweighted analysis, with risk in the FDRs of BD probands at 2.10% and 0.60% in control probands (P = .01; OR = 3.57; Φ = 0.065; 95% CI = 0.014-0.110).
RATES OF INDEX DISORDER IN FDRs OF PATIENT PROBANDS
As expected, both schizophrenia and BD show substantially increased risk of the relevant index disorder in FDRs of patient probands over control probands in the weighted analysis (both P < .001). The risk of schizophrenia was estimated at 6.68% for FDRs of probands with schizophrenia (the estimate for controls is the same as reported above, 0.85%; OR = 8.38; Φ = 0.153; 95% CI = 0.119-0.186). The risk of BD was estimated at 10.54% for FDRs of probands with BD (0.48% in control probands) (OR = 24.47; Φ = 0.221; 95% CI = 0.158-0.264).
The data presented herein provide the first direct evidence of familial coaggregation of schizophrenia and BD, with FDRs of probands with either schizophrenia or BD having roughly double the expected risk of the other disorder. While the results for aggregation of schizophrenia in the families of probands with BD were somewhat equivocal (marginally significant in the primary analysis and not significant in the unweighted analysis), the fact that significant aggregation was observed when only studies using morbid risk methods were included, combined with the significant aggregation of BD in the FDR of probands with schizophrenia, makes it very likely that aggregation of schizophrenia in the families of BD probands is a real phenomenon.
Several alternative explanations for our findings deserve consideration. First, it could be argued that our findings simply reflect a small, but significant, rate of misdiagnosis of FDR. We find this interpretation unlikely, as the rates of misdiagnosis required to account for our results are quite high. If the true rate of BD in the FDRs of patients with schizophrenia were the same as the rate for FDRs of control probands, our weighted analysis indicates that 12.1% of cases of schizophrenia in FDRs would have to be misdiagnosed as BD to account for our data; conversely, 4.6% of cases of BD would have to be misdiagnosed as schizophrenia. While this does not unequivocally demonstrate that misdiagnosis does not account for a portion of the observed effects, the rates of misdiagnosis required to account for our findings seem much too large. Similarly, given that SD aggregates in the families of individuals with both schizophrenia and BD,8,10 it is also possible that misdiagnosis of schizoaffective family members as having either schizophrenia or BD could drive our results. However, based on observed rates of SD in the families of patients with schizophrenia and BD,8 15% of FDRs of patients with SD and 58% of FDRs of patients with BD would have to be misdiagnosed to account for our data.
Another potential explanation for our findings is that elevated rates of disorder are reported in the FDRs of patient probands owing to a surveillance or reporting bias in family history studies. This is unlikely for a number of reasons. First, in most of the studies in our analysis, relative diagnoses were assigned blind to proband diagnosis (and the influence of unblinded studies was reduced with the weighting procedure); so, in these studies there is no reason to expect the overreporting of illness to differentially affect diagnostic rates in patient and control FDRs. Indeed, the rates of disorder in control probands are at the low end of the lifetime prevalence generally found for these disorders (0.5%-1.5% for schizophrenia and 0.4%-1.6% for BD80), suggesting that the occurrence of disorder is not being overreported. Additionally, even if there were some degree of bias acting on FDRs of patient probands but not control probands, it seems unlikely that such bias could be of a sufficient magnitude to account for our data—that is, diagnoses of the nonindex disorder would have to be twice as likely (given ORs > 2.0) for FDRs of patient than control probands, a level of bias that seems unreasonably high.
Finally, a concern with many meta-analyses is publication bias, in which studies reporting null or small effects may be underrepresented in the literature because of difficulty in publishing null results, thereby resulting in overestimation of the true effect size. However, there is no reason to suspect that publication bias has influenced our results, because only 2 studies in our analysis reported significant aggregation of schizophrenia in the families of probands with BD, and none reported significant aggregation of BD in the families of probands with schizophrenia. Thus, nearly all of the included studies had null results. Furthermore, many of the studies in our analyses were not specifically interested in detecting coaggregation of these disorders, so no reporting bias would be expected in these studies.
The findings of the present study are inconsistent with categorical models of psychiatric disorder that treat schizophrenia and BD as separate, unrelated illnesses. While there are obviously important clinical distinctions between these disorders, the data are consistent either with a continuum from psychotic to affective illness on which diagnostic categories are imposed or with a set of distinct disease entities with overlapping symptomatology that are not distinguished effectively in current psychiatric nosology.81,82 Either model fits well with findings that FDRs have a very high risk of developing the same disorder as index probands, an intermediate risk of developing schizoaffective disorder regardless of whether the index proband has schizophrenia or BD,8,10 and a low, but still increased, risk of developing BD for FDRs of probands with schizophrenia and vice versa. This pattern of results would be expected if all 3 disorders share genetic liability, but FDRs who develop one of these disorders are more likely to develop a disorder at the same point on the psychotic-affective continuum as the index case. Alternatively, it could be that some individuals with a familial disorder whose symptomatology overlaps with schizophrenia and BD (such as SD) meet current diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia or BD,81 with familial aggregation leading to an apparent continuum. Unfortunately, we did not systematically obtain data pertaining to SD, so we do not have meta-analytic estimates of coaggregation among SD, schizophrenia, and BD, which could help differentiate these 2 possibilities.
An important implication of these results is that genetic linkage studies could increase their power to identify susceptibility genes by treating all of the disorders along this spectrum as a single phenotype. Given that there is shared genetic risk of developing schizophrenia, BD, and SD, a plausible model is that some number of genes confer risk of psychotic-affective disorder in general, while another small set of genes—or epigenetic or environmental factors—determine where on the continuum the disorder will fall if it develops. If this is the case, treating 1 of the disorders as a different phenotype than the disorder of interest would substantially reduce the power to detect susceptibility genes that confer risk to disorders across the psychotic-affective continuum.
Our results, while novel, fit well within a set of findings in the literature that suggest a link between schizophrenia and BD. In addition to the previously discussed findings of shared susceptibility loci and high rates of comorbidity when using nonhierarchical diagnoses, it is worth noting that these disorders share a number of epidemiological83 and perinatal84 risk factors. Indeed, in recent years a number of authors have argued for a degree of continuity between these disorders, in opposition to the traditional Kraepelinian dichotomy.12,81,82,85-87 In light of the findings presented herein that schizophrenia and BD coaggregate, it is unlikely that these 2 disorders represent truly discrete and unrelated disease entities. While there are obviously important distinctions between schizophrenia and BD, some degree of relationship between them clearly exists—at a minimum there is a shared genetic risk factor between at least a subset of individuals with either disorder (suggesting a shared etiology), while at most they may be different behavioral expressions of a common, or at least substantively overlapping, pathology.
Correspondence: Jared X. Van Snellenberg, MA, MPhil, Department of Psychology, Columbia University, 406 Schermerhorn Hall, 1190 Amsterdam Ave, Mail Code 5501, New York, NY 10027 (jared@psych.columbia.edu).
Submitted for Publication: August 14, 2008; final revision received December 15, 2008; accepted January 2, 2009.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Author Contributions: Mr Van Snellenberg takes full responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis; Mr Snellenberg and Ms Candia had full access to the data throughout the study.
Additional Contributions: Niall Bolger, PhD, provided comments on the bootstrapping procedures used for the analyses; Joshua Weinberg, BS, assisted in developing Matlab scripts for data analysis; and Lucas Sjulson, MD, PhD, provided valuable comments on an earlier version of the manuscript.
1.Badner
JAGershon
ES Meta-analysis of whole-genome linkage scans of bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.
Mol Psychiatry 2002;7
(4)
405- 411
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Berrettini
W Evidence for shared susceptibility in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.
Am J Med Genet C Semin Med Genet 2003;123C
(1)
59- 64
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Berrettini
WH Susceptibility loci for bipolar disorder: overlap with inherited vulnerability to schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry 2000;47
(3)
245- 251
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Craddock
NO'Donovan
MCOwen
MJ The genetics of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder: dissecting psychosis.
J Med Genet 2005;42
(3)
193- 204
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Cardno
AGRijsdijk
FSham
PCMurray
RMMcGuffin
P A twin study of genetic relationship between psychotic symptoms.
Am J Psychiatry 2002;159
(4)
539- 545
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Maier
WLichtermann
DFranke
PHeun
RFalkai
PRietschel
M The dichotomy of schizophrenia and affective disorders in extended pedigrees.
Schizophr Res 2002;57
(2-3)
259- 266
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Gershon
ESDeLisi
LEHamovit
JNurnberger
JI
JrMaxwell
MESchreiber
JDauphinais
DDingman
CW
IIGuroff
JJ A controlled family study of chronic psychoses: schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988;45
(4)
328- 336
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Maier
WLichtermann
DMinges
JHallmayer
JHeun
RBenkert
OLevinson
DF Continuity and discontinuity of affective disorder and schizophrenia: results of a controlled family study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50
(11)
871- 883
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Kendler
KSMcGuire
MGruenberg
AMO’Hare
ASpellman
MWalsh
D The Roscommon family study, I: methods, diagnosis of probands, and risk of schizophrenia in relatives.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50
(7)
527- 540
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 10.Vallès
VVan Os
JGuillamat
RGutiérrez
BCampillo
MGento
PFañanás
L Increased morbid risk for schizophrenia in families of in-patients with bipolar illness.
Schizophr Res 2000;42
(2)
83- 90
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Gershon
ESHamovit
JGuroff
JJDibble
ELeckman
JFSceery
WTargum
SDNurnberger
JI
JrGoldin
LRBunney
WE
Jr A family study of schizoaffective, bipolar I, bipolar II, unipolar, and normal control probands.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982;39
(10)
1157- 1167
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Crow
TJ How and why genetic linkage has not solved the problem of psychosis: review and hypothesis.
Am J Psychiatry 2007;164
(1)
13- 21
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Kendler
KSGardner
CO The risk for psychiatric disorders in relatives of schizophrenic and control probands: a comparison of three independent studies.
Psychol Med 1997;27
(2)
411- 419
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Rende
RHodgins
SPalmour
RFaucher
BAllaire
J-F Familial overlap between bipolar disorder and psychotic symptoms in a Canadian cohort.
Can J Psychiatry 2005;50
(4)
189- 194
PubMedGoogle Scholar 15.Spitzer
RLEndicott
JRobins
E Research diagnostic criteria: rationale and reliability.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1978;35
(6)
773- 782
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Adamson
TA Psychiatric illness in first degree relatives of Nigerian schizophrenic and surgical control patients.
Trop Geogr Med 1993;45
(2)
73- 76
PubMedGoogle Scholar 17.Baron
MGruen
RRainer
JDKane
JAsnis
LLord
S A family study of schizophrenic and normal control probands: implications for the spectrum concept of schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry 1985;142
(4)
447- 455
PubMedGoogle Scholar 18.Coryell
WZimmerman
M The heritability of schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder: a family study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1988;45
(4)
323- 327
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Kendler
KSMcGuire
MGruenberg
AMO’Hare
ASpellman
MWalsh
D The Roscommon family study, IV: affective illness, anxiety disorders, and alcoholism in relatives.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1993;50
(12)
952- 960
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Loranger
AWOldham
JMTulis
EH Familial transmission of DSM-III borderline personality disorder.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982;39
(7)
795- 799
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Poyurovsky
MKriss
VWeisman
GFaragian
SSchneidman
MFuchs
CWeizman
AWeizman
R Familial aggregation of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders and obsessive-compulsive associated disorders in schizophrenia probands with and without OCD.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 2005;133B
(1)
31- 36
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Silverman
JMPinkham
LHorvath
TBCoccaro
EFKlar
HSchear
SApter
SDavidson
MMohs
RCSiever
LJ Affective and impulsive personality disorder traits in the relatives of patients with borderline personality disorder.
Am J Psychiatry 1991;148
(10)
1378- 1385
PubMedGoogle Scholar 23.Tsuang
MTKendler
KKGruenberg
AM DSM-III schizophrenia: is there evidence for familial transmission?
Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl 1985;31977- 83
Google ScholarCrossref 24.Varma
SLSharma
I Psychiatric morbidity in the first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients.
Br J Psychiatry 1993;162672- 678
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Varma
SLZain
AMSingh
S Psychiatric morbidity in the first-degree relatives of schizophrenic patients.
Am J Med Genet 1997;74
(1)
7- 11
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Andreasen
NCRice
JEndicott
JCoryell
WGrove
WMReich
T Familial rates of affective disorder: a report from the National Institute of Mental Health collaborative study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1987;44
(5)
461- 469
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Baron
MGruen
RAsnis
LKane
J Schizoaffective illness, schizophrenia and affective disorders: morbidity risk and genetic transmission.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1982;65
(4)
253- 262
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Dean
CWilliams
RJBrockington
IF Is puerperal psychosis the same as bipolar manic-depressive disorder? a family study.
Psychol Med 1989;19
(3)
637- 647
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 29.Dwyer
JTDelong
R A family history study of twenty probands with childhood manic-depressive illness.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1987;26
(2)
176- 180
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 31.Kutcher
SMarton
P Affective disorders in first-degree relatives of adolescent onset bipolars, unipolars, and normal controls.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1991;30
(1)
75- 78
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 33.Pope
HG
JrYurgelun-Todd
D A family interview study of schizophrenia, schizoaffective disorder, and major mood disorder: updated findings.
Eur Psychiatry 1993;81- 5
Google Scholar 34.Price
LHNelson
JCCharney
DSQuinlan
DM The clinical utility of family history for the diagnosis of melancholia.
J Nerv Ment Dis 1984;172
(1)
5- 11
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Gureje
OBamidele
RAderibigbe
YA Heritability of schizophrenia: a controlled family history investigation in Nigeria.
Br J Psychiatry 1994;164
(4)
481- 486
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Pulver
AEBrown
CHWolyniec
PSMcGrath
JATam
D Psychiatric morbidity in the relatives of patients with DSM-III schizophreniform disorder: comparisons with the relatives of schizophrenic and bipolar disorder patients.
J Psychiatr Res 1991;25
(1-2)
19- 29
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 38.Scharfetter
CNüsperli
M The group of schizophrenias, schizoaffective psychoses, and affective disorders.
Schizophr Bull 1980;6
(4)
586- 591
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Sham
PCJones
PRussell
AGilvarry
KBebbington
PLewis
SToone
BMurray
R Age at onset, sex, and familial psychiatric morbidity in schizophrenia: Camberwell Collaborative Psychosis Study.
Br J Psychiatry 1994;165
(4)
466- 473
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Somnath
CPJanardhan Reddy
YCJain
S Is there a familial overlap between schizophrenia and bipolar disorder?
J Affect Disord 2002;72
(3)
243- 247
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Strober
MMorrell
WBurroughs
JLampert
CDanforth
HFreeman
R A family study of bipolar I disorder in adolescence: early onset of symptoms linked to increased familial loading and lithium resistance.
J Affect Disord 1988;15
(3)
255- 268
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Alda
MDvořáková
MZvolský
PPapežová
HPošmurová
M Genetic aspects in chronic schizophrenia: morbidity risks and contributory factors.
Schizophr Res 1989;2
(4-5)
339- 344
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 43.Asarnow
RFNuechterlein
KHFogelson
DSubotnik
KLPayne
DARussell
ATAsamen
JKuppinger
HKendler
KS Schizophrenia and schizophrenia-spectrum personality disorder in the first-degree relatives of children with schizophrenia: The UCLA family study.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2001;58
(6)
581- 588
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 44.Bellodi
LBussoleni
CScorza-Smeraldi
RGrassi
GZacchetti
LSmeraldi
E Family study of schizophrenia: exploratory analysis for relevant factors.
Schizophr Bull 1986;12
(1)
120- 128
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Chang
C-JChen
WJLiu
SKCheng
JJYang
WCChang
HJLane
HYLin
SKYang
TWHwu
HG Morbidity risk of psychiatric disorders among the first degree relatives of schizophrenia patients in Taiwan.
Schizophr Bull 2002;28
(3)
379- 392
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Clementz
BAGrove
WMIacono
WGSweeney
JA Smooth-pursuit eye movement dysfunction and liability for schizophrenia: implications for genetic modeling.
J Abnorm Psychol 1992;101
(1)
117- 129
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Das
SKMalhotra
SBasu
D Family study of acute and transient psychotic disorders: comparison with schizophrenia.
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol 1999;34
(6)
328- 332
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 48.Frangos
EAthanassenas
GTsitourides
SKatsanou
NAlexandrakou
P Prevalence of DSM III schizophrenia among the first-degree relatives of schizophrenic probands.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1985;72
(4)
382- 386
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 50.Joober
RRouleau
GALal
SBloom
DLalonde
PLabelle
ABenkelfat
C Increased prevalence of schizophrenia spectrum disorders in relatives of neuorleptic-nonresponsive schizophrenic patients.
Schizophr Res 2005;77
(1)
35- 41
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Kendler
KSGruenberg
AMKinney
DK Independent diagnoses of adoptees and relatives as defined by DMS-III in the provincial and national samples of the Danish adoption study of schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1994;51
(6)
456- 468
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 52.Kendler
KSKarkowski-Shuman
LWalsh
D Age at onset in schizophrenia and risk of illness in relatives: results from the Roscommon Family Study.
Br J Psychiatry 1996;169
(2)
213- 218
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Lim
LCCSim
LP The prevalence of schizophrenia in relatives of schizophrenic patients.
Singapore Med J 1992;33
(6)
645- 647
PubMedGoogle Scholar 54.Maj
MStarace
FPirozzi
R A family study of DSM-III-R schizoaffective disorder, depressive type, compared with schizophrenia and psychotic and nonpsychotic major depression.
Am J Psychiatry 1991;148
(5)
612- 616
PubMedGoogle Scholar 55.Onstad
SSkre
IEdvardsen
JTorgersen
SKringlen
E Mental disorders in first-degree relatives of schizophrenics.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1991;83
(6)
463- 467
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Pope
HG
JrJonas
JMCohen
BMLipinski
JF Failure to find evidence of schizophrenia in first-degree relatives of schizophrenic probands.
Am J Psychiatry 1982;139
(6)
826- 828
PubMedGoogle Scholar 57.Shimizu
AKurachi
MYamaguchi
NTorii
HIsaki
K Morbidity risk of schizophrenia to parents and siblings of schizophrenic patients.
Jpn J Psychiatry Neurol 1987;41
(1)
65- 70
PubMedGoogle Scholar 58.Stöber
GFranzek
ELesch
KPBeckmann
H Periodic catatonia: a schizophrenic subtype with major gene effect and anticipation.
Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 1995;245
(3)
135- 141
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 59.Taylor
MABerenbaum
SAJampala
VCCloninger
CR Are schizophrenia and affective disorder related? preliminary data from a family study.
Am J Psychiatry 1993;150
(2)
278- 285
PubMedGoogle Scholar 60.Ungvári
G Validity of the ICD-9 schizophrenia classification: a blind family history study.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983;68
(4)
287- 296
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 61.Waddington
JLYoussef
HA Familial-genetic and reproductive epidemiology of schizophrenia in rural Ireland: age at onset, familial morbid risk and parental fertility.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1996;93
(1)
62- 68
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Biederman
JFaraone
SMick
EWozniak
JChen
LOuellette
CMarrs
AMoore
PGarcia
JMennin
DLelon
E Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and juvenile mania: an overlooked comorbidity?
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35
(8)
997- 1008
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Faraone
SVBiederman
JMonuteaux
MC Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder with bipolar disorder in girls: further evidence for a familial subtype?
J Affect Disord 2001;64
(1)
19- 26
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 64.Fieve
RRGo
RDunner
DLElston
R Search for biological/genetic markers in a long-term epidemiological and morbid risk study of affective disorders.
J Psychiatr Res 1984;18
(4)
425- 445
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Geller
BTillman
RBolhofner
KZimerman
BStrauss
NAKaufmann
P Controlled, blindly rated, direct-interview family study of a prepubertal and early-adolescent bipolar I disorder phenotype.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2006;63
(10)
1130- 1138
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 66.Grigoroiu-Serbanescu
MMartinez
MNöthen
MMGrinberg
MSima
DPropping
PMarinescu
EHrestic
M Different familial transmission patterns in bipolar I disorder with onset before and after age 25.
Am J Med Genet 2001;105
(8)
765- 773
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 68.Lish
JDGyulai
LResnick
SMKirtland
AAmsterdam
JDWhybrow
PCPrice
RA A family history study of rapid-cycling bipolar disorder.
Psychiatry Res 1993;48
(1)
37- 46
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 69.Lewinsohn
PMKlein
DNSeeley
JR Bipolar disorder during adolescence and young adulthood in a community sample.
Bipolar Disord 2000;2
(3, pt 2)
281- 293
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 70.Neuman
RJGeller
BRice
JPTodd
RD Increased prevalence and earlier onset of mood disorders among relatives of prepubertal versus adult probands.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997;36
(4)
466- 473
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 71.Passmore
MJGarnham
JDuffy
AMacDougall
MMunro
ASlaney
CTeehan
AAlda
M Phenotypic spectra of bipolar disorder in responders to lithium versus lamotrigine.
Bipolar Disord 2003;5
(2)
110- 114
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 72.Pauls
DLMorton
LAEgeland
JA Risks of affective illness among first-degree relatives of bipolar I old-order Amish probands.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992;49
(9)
703- 708
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 73.Sadovnick
ADRemick
RALam
RZis
APYee
IMHuggins
MJBaird
PA Mood disorder service genetic database: morbidity risks for mood disorders in 3,942 first-degree relatives of 671 index cases with single depressions, recurrent depression, bipolar I, or bipolar II.
Am J Med Genet 1994;54
(2)
132- 140
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 74.Schürhoff
FBellivier
FJouvent
RMouren-Siméoni
MCBouvard
MAllilaire
JFLeboyer
M Early and late onset bipolar disorders: two different forms of manic-depressive illness?
J Affect Disord 2000;58
(3)
215- 221
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 76.Wozniak
JBiederman
JMundy
EMennin
DFaraone
S A pilot family study of childhood-onset mania.
J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1995;34
(12)
1577- 1583
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 77.Hedges
LVVevea
JL Fixed- and random-effects models in meta-analysis.
Psychol Methods 1998;3
(4)
486- 504
Google ScholarCrossref 78.Efron
BTibshirani
RJ An Introduction to the Bootstrap. New York, NY Chapman & Hall/CRC1993;
79.Risch
N Estimating morbidity risks with variable age of onset: review of methods and a maximum likelihood approach.
Biometrics 1983;39
(4)
929- 939
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 80.American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fourth Edition, Text Revision Washington, DC American Psychiatric Association2000;
81.Craddock
NOwen
MJ Rethinking psychosis: the disadvantages of a dichotomous classification now outweigh the advantages.
World Psychiatry 2007;6
(2)
84- 91
PubMedGoogle Scholar 82.Marneros
AAkiskal
HS The Overlap of Affective and Schizophrenic Spectra. Cambridge, England Cambridge University Press2007;
83.Torrey
EF Epidemiological comparison of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder.
Schizophr Res 1999;39
(2)
101- 106, discussion 159-160
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 84.Buka
SLFan
AP Association of prenatal and perinatal complications with subsequent bipolar disorder and schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res 1999;39
(2)
113- 119, discussion 160-161
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 86.Berrettini
WH Are schizophrenic and bipolar disorder related? a review of family and molecular studies.
Biol Psychiatry 2000;48
(6)
531- 538
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 87.Möller
HJ Bipolar disorder and schizophrenia: distinct illnesses or a continuum?
J Clin Psychiatry 2003;64
((suppl 6))
23- 28
PubMedGoogle Scholar