Context
The imperative to establish so-called endophenotypes—quantifiable measures of risk for neurological dysfunction—is a growing focus of research in schizophrenia. Electrophysiological markers of sensory processing, observable in human event-related potentials, hold great promise in this regard, lying closer to underlying physiology than descriptive clinical diagnostic tests.
Objective
Early visual processing deficits, as measured by clear amplitude reductions in the occipital P1 component of the visual event-related potential, have been repeatedly demonstrated in patients with schizophrenia. However, before P1 amplitude may be considered as an endophenotypic marker for schizophrenia, it is necessary to establish its sensitivity to genetic liability.
Design, Setting, and Participants
Event-related potential responses to simple visual isolated-check stimuli were examined in 25 clinically unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia and 15 DSM-IV–diagnosed schizophrenia probands and compared with responses from 26 healthy, age-matched control subjects. Using high-density electrical scalp recordings, between-groups analysis assessed the integrity of the visual P1 component across the 3 groups. The study was conducted at St Vincent's Psychiatric Hospital in Fairview, Dublin, Ireland.
Results
Substantially reduced P1 amplitude was demonstrated in both relatives and probands compared with controls with topographical mapping and inverse source analysis localizing this deficit largely to midline regions in early visual sensory cortices and regions of the dorsal visual stream. Additional later differences between these groups, where the relatives actually show larger amplitude responses, may point toward compensatory mechanisms at play in relatives.
Conclusions
Our findings demonstrate a deficit in early visual processing in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives of patients with schizophrenia, providing evidence that this deficit may serve as a genetic marker for this disorder. The efficacy of using P1 amplitude as an endophenotype is underscored by the observation of a large effect size (d = 0.9) over scalp sites where the deficit was maximal.
Although genetic influences in schizophrenia have been widely reported,1 the task of localizing the specific genes responsible has proven difficult.2 In the hunt for the underlying risk factors for schizophrenia, establishing so-called endophenotypes has emerged as a major focus.3 Endophenotypes serve as intermediate quantitative constructs, bridging the gap between genetic and environmental factors at one end and descriptive symptom-based diagnostics at the other. An effective endophenotype should index an individual's liability to develop or manifest a certain disease in much the same way that serum cholesterol predicts the risk of cardiovascular disease. Being quantifiable by definition, it should offer higher predictive power and lie closer to underlying genetic liability and gene action than diagnostic phenotypes. One of the most promising avenues for establishing such endophenotypes in schizophrenia lies in recordings of the event-related potential (ERP) where deficits in patients have been consistently demonstrated in both visual4,5 and auditory6-8 processing. This promise has been borne out recently with a number of reports uncovering ERP deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives similar to those seen in patients.9-15
In a series of recent ERP studies from this group, highly robust visual sensory processing deficits have been repeatedly observed in patients with schizophrenia,16-21 a finding now replicated by other groups.22 In these studies, generation of one of the most robust components of the visual evoked potential (VEP), the so-called P1 component, was consistently impaired in patients. This VEP component indexes early sensory processing in extrastriate visual areas and is recorded over both midline and lateral occipital scalp with peak latency typically varying between 75 and 110 milliseconds, dependent on stimulus features.23 A number of factors point to the P1 deficit as a particularly promising candidate as an endophenotype. First, this effect is especially strong with amplitude of the P1 in patients less than half the strength seen in healthy controls. This component is also very straightforward to measure, is readily identifiable across individuals, and takes no more than a couple of minutes to obtain a sufficiently clean and stable recording. Because it is a largely automatic response, it does not require any complex task, other than fixation of the eyes, and so is not susceptible to motivational issues or the ongoing clinical state of the patient in the way that later cognitive components such as the P300 can be.24,25
Clinically unaffected first-degree biological relatives have an elevated risk for schizophrenia in addition to showing mild impairments in neurobiological functioning (eg, executive functioning, verbal and visual memory, auditory attention, and verbal ability) similar to that found in patients.26-29 These impairments are thought to reflect the genetic predisposition for schizophrenia rather than the disease process itself. Our objective here was to investigate whether the visual P1 deficit is also observed in unaffected first-degree relatives and to establish whether this component has potential use as an endophenotypic marker for schizophrenia. The study was carried out in Ireland, a country with a high degree of common ancestry and shared genetic liabilities. The traceability of Irish families is conducive to future genetic linkage studies within families.
Informed consent was obtained from 25 (15 female) clinically unaffected first-degree biological relatives of known patients with schizophrenia (probands). Relatives were aged 18 to 64 years (mean ± SD, 32.3 ± 13.6 years) and were recruited from the St Vincent's Hospital catchment area in Fairview, Dublin, Ireland. They were contacted via telephone and mailings following consent from their probands, who were either being seen in the outpatient clinics or were inpatients in the hospital. First-degree relatives were recruited from 18 unique families and consisted of the parents (5), the siblings (15), or the children (5) of affected individuals meeting DSM-IV criteria for schizophrenia. For each patient with schizophrenia, a maximum of 3 first-degree relatives were recruited. The mean ± SD score on the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS)30 for the first-degree relatives was 0.64 ± 1.87. Also included were 15 (4 female) original probands, aged 17 to 53 years (mean ± SD, 32.6 ± 12.7 years) with mean ± SD scores on the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale31 and SANS of 40.80 ± 11.96 and 28.87 ± 22.09, respectively. Five of the 15 probands were drug-naive and experiencing their first episodes of psychosis. Nine of the patients were receiving medication with a mean chlorpromazine equivalent dose of 259.44 mg/d (range, 50-800 mg/d). The types of antipsychotics included atypicals (6), typicals (2), and a combination of both (1). One proband had ceased taking her medications 3 months prior to testing and was medication-free at the time of testing.
Control subjects were recruited from the St Vincent's Hospital staff community and through local recruitment efforts in the hospital catchment area. This group comprised 26 (13 female) paid volunteers aged 21 to 64 years (mean ± SD, 38.7 ± 12.6 years). The mean age of relatives and controls did not differ significantly (t50 = 1.73, P = .09), and the mean age of probands did not differ significantly from either group (P>.10). Nineteen of the 25 relatives, 21 of the 26 controls, and 12 of the 15 patients were right-handed as assessed by the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory.32 All subjects reported normal or corrected-to-normal vision. None of the relatives or controls were receiving any psychotropic medication at the time of testing.33,34 Relatives and controls were free of any psychiatric illness or symptoms by self-report using criteria from the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-III-R–Non-Patient (SCID-NP),35 and all reported no history of alcohol or substance abuse.
(Note that there was a marginal trend for the control subjects to be older than relatives. It is worth pointing out that to the extent that the VEP changes with age, it tends toward reduced amplitude and increased latencies, although these changes are very small and only seen over the course of many decades.36 Thus, if age were playing any role here, the prediction would go in exactly the opposite direction to that proposed. That is, control subjects would be expected to show reduced P1 amplitudes relative to the first-degree group.)
In each experimental block, subjects were presented with approximately 100 isolated-check images, gray on a white background (4° × 4° visual angle) at 64% contrast, and 40 line drawings of 2 kinds of animals (2.4° wide × 1.8° high) on a white background. Each block contained a different animal pair from a possible 22. The isolated-check stimuli and 1 pair of animal stimuli are shown in Figure 1. The 64% contrast condition was chosen to stimulate both the magnocellular (M) and parvocellular (P) systems. On average, subjects completed 13.5 blocks (range, 10-15), each lasting 3 minutes. Stimuli were presented centrally on a cathode ray tube computer monitor in random order with the monitor located 160 cm directly in front of the seated subjects.
The timing of the presentations was such that each image appeared for 60 milliseconds with a variable interstimulus interval between 740 and 1540 milliseconds (randomly in steps of 200 milliseconds) during which there was a blank white screen. The target animal was displayed at the start of the task and subjects were asked to respond each time this animal was presented by pressing a button with their right thumbs. They were told only to respond to target animals and to try to withhold responses to any other animal presented. The target and nontarget animals were presented with equal probability, ensuring that a subject could not rely on the exogenous alerting nature of any noncheckerboard stimulus to respond. Furthermore, the task of discrimination was made difficult by pairing similar-looking animals, eg, dolphin and whale. The primary motivation for using this task rather than simply having subjects passively observe the standard stimuli was to ensure that subjects had fixated centrally on the screen. Only ERPs to the standard checkerboard stimuli were included in the analysis.
Data acquisition and statistical analysis
Continuous electroencephalography was acquired through the ActiveTwo BioSemi electrode system (BioSemi, Amsterdam, the Netherlands) from 72 scalp electrodes, digitized at 512 Hz with an open pass-band from DC to 150 Hz. For analysis and display purposes, data were subsequently filtered with a 0-phase-shift 45 Hz low-pass filter (24 dB/octave) after acquisition. No high-pass filter was applied. With the BioSemi system, every electrode or combination of electrodes can be assigned as the reference, and this is done purely in software after acquisition. BioSemi replaces the “ground” electrodes used in conventional systems with 2 separate electrodes: Common Mode Sense active electrode and Driven Right Leg passive electrode. These 2 electrodes form a feedback loop, which drives the average potential of the subject (the Common Mode voltage) as close as possible to the ADC reference voltage in the AD-box (the ADC reference can be considered as the amplifier “zero”). A detailed description of the referencing and grounding conventions used by the BioSemi active electrode system appears online (http://www.biosemi.com/faq/cms&drl.htm). All data were rereferenced to the nasion after acquisition for analysis.
Data were analyzed using BESA version 5.08 (Brain Electric Source Analysis, Gräfelfing, Germany). All electrode channels were subjected to an artifact criterion of ± 120μV from −200 to 500 milliseconds to reject trials with excessive electromyogram (EMG) or other noise transients. The vertical and horizontal electro-oculograms were also visually inspected for blinks and large eye movements. Epochs were calculated for a time window from 200 milliseconds prestimulus to 500 milliseconds poststimulus and baseline-corrected relative to the interval −80 to 20 milliseconds. Accepted trials were then averaged for the isolated-check stimuli only. The mean ± SD epoch acceptance rate for the relatives was 59.6 ± 10.8%; for patients, 59.5 ± 10.7%; and for the control group, 63.4 ± 16.8%.
Our primary analysis was motivated by a specific hypothesis, based on previous research by our group, regarding reduction in early visual sensory processing (ie, the P1 processing period) in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives. A measure of P1 amplitude was defined as the area under the curve (vs the 0-μV baseline) in the interval 87 to 97 milliseconds, spanning the P1 component, chosen based on grand average waveforms (Figure 2). These area measures were then submitted to a repeated-measures multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) using SPSS software (SPSS Inc, Chicago, Il) with a between-subjects factor of group (relatives vs controls vs probands) and within-subjects factors of region (left, midline, or right) and electrode (O1/PO7/PO3/, Oz/POz/Pz, O2/PO4/PO8), covering the left lateral occipital, midline dorsal, and right lateral occipital visual scalp regions, respectively. All tests were 2-tailed with a preset α level of P<.05.
Following our primary analysis of P1 amplitude, it was of interest to further investigate spatiotemporal properties of any potential differences between groups using the statistical cluster plot method. This procedure has been used effectively in post hoc analyses as a means to fully explore complex data sets and generate pointed follow-up hypotheses.37,38 Pointwise 2-tailed t tests (here between controls and relatives) are calculated at each time point for all electrodes, and a color map is subsequently generated marking time points on each electrode for which the t value exceeds that corresponding to a .05 P value. Here we plot positive and negative t values in separate color scales (green and gold) to distinguish differences in opposite directions. All nonsignificant points are represented as white.
To localize the neural sources giving rise to the observed effects, we employed a source analysis technique based on a distributed linear inverse solution (LAURA).39 LAURA uses a realistic head model with a solution space of 4024 nodes where voxels are restricted to the gray matter of the Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) average brain divided into a regular grid with 6-mm spacing. It is capable of dealing with multiple simultaneously active sources of a priori unknown locations and makes no assumptions regarding the number or location of active sources. This linearly distributed inverse solution selects the source configuration that best mimics the biophysical behavior of electric vector fields and produces a unique estimator of the current source density vector inside the brain.40 Source analysis was performed here on group-averaged ERP data separately for controls and relatives.
The mean ± SD target hit rate for controls was 95.4 ± 7.6; for relatives, 91.4 ± 10.7; and for probands, 91.0 ± 11.5. The mean rates did not differ significantly between the relatives, patients, and controls (all P>.10). These rates indicate that subjects were actively observing the stimuli.
Figure 2 provides an overall picture of ERP morphology across the scalp for all 3 groups, showing responses from 6 representative electrodes over frontal, central, and posterior scalp regions. Group differences are evident, particularly in posterior regions. The distribution of P1 amplitudes at electrode site PO4 for each of the 3 groups is shown in the form of a scatterplot in Figure 3. The standard deviations for the controls, relatives, and probands were 6.08, 5.58, and 7.81 respectively.
A MANOVA (3 groups by 3 regions by 3 electrodes) was used to compare P1 amplitudes between all 3 groups over the left lateral occipital, midline dorsal, and right lateral occipital visual areas. This revealed a significant and highly robust main effect of group (F2,63 = 5.36, P = .007). Pairwise comparisons of group indicated substantially reduced P1 amplitude in the first-degree relative group compared with controls (mean difference, 3.72 μV; P = .02), confirming the main hypothesis of this study. Similarly, P1 amplitude in the proband group was also significantly reduced compared with controls (mean difference, 4.30 μV; P = .02), replicating our findings in a number of previous studies.5,16,17,20 No difference in P1 amplitude was found between probands and relatives (P = 1.00). As the finding of reduced P1 in probands is simply a replication of our previous work, further analysis was restricted to the comparison of first-degree relatives with controls using a protected 2 × 3 × 3 ANOVA in line with the principle aim of the study. The observed effects for these 2 groups are illustrated in Figure 4, which focuses on a shorter latency interval around the effect of interest at the right hemisphere parieto-occipital scalp site where P1 amplitude was maximal for both groups.
The protected 2 × 3 × 3 ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of group (F1,49 = 8.88, P = .004), again indicating substantially reduced P1 amplitude in the first-degree relative group compared with controls. There were also significant main effects of region (F2,49 = 28.42, P<.001) and electrode (F2,49 = 35.46, P<.001) and a region × electrode interaction (F2,49 = 10.92, P<.001), reflecting the topographic specificity of the P1. Protected follow-up comparisons between regions showed a significant difference when left and right hemisphere regions 1 and 3 (both P<.001) were compared with midline region 2. This arises from the bilateral foci in the topography of P1 amplitude (Figure 5). Comparison of regions 1 and 3 did not prove significant (P = .27).
(It should be pointed out that given typical conversion rates for parents [6%], siblings [9%], and children [13%]41 and given the age make-up of the first-degree relative group, one would predict that as many as 2 to 3 of our subjects could go on to develop schizophrenia. This proportion of subjects would clearly not be sufficient to drive the effects seen here.)
A significant region × electrode × group interaction was also found (F4,196 = 3.44, P = .03), suggesting some topographic specificity of the effect and warranting further examination. In follow-up 1-way ANOVAs, the group difference was found to be significant in all regions (all P<.02). Looking at individual electrodes, the group difference was significant at all locations except at midparietal scalp-site Pz, where it approached significance (P = .07). Despite this generality of significant effects, the difference topography (Figure 5) shows that the decrement in P1 amplitude is strongest over the midline dorsal parieto-occipital scalp. An effect size of d = 0.90 was calculated for the difference in P1 amplitude measured over the interval 91 to 97 milliseconds at the electrode site (PO4) where the effect was maximal, which constitutes a large effect size according to the criterion of Cohen.42
As shown in Figure 6, the statistical cluster plot contains a posterior cluster in the time range of the P1, reflecting the group difference reported in the MANOVA. In addition to this, a cluster shortly following the P1-related cluster in the same region reflects a difference in the opposite direction, ie, greater amplitude in relatives compared with controls (gold clusters). This second phase of activity was examined more closely in the LAURA source analysis, which revealed greater activation in extrastriate regions after 125 milliseconds.
Figure 7 displays the LAURA source analysis for 3 time points spanning the time frame of the P1 component (80, 90, and 100 milliseconds) and 2 further time points across the later sensory processing epoch (150 and 200 milliseconds). Source analysis revealed the expected generator pattern for the P1 with generators in midline visual structures, dorsal stream parietal regions, and ventral stream lateral occipital structures.17 In the plots for the 90-millisecond time point, the fact that the generators are substantially more active for the control subjects is evident. Note that at 150 milliseconds, a second round of activity in these regions is greater for relatives than control subjects, suggesting an additional round of re-entrant activity that is absent from the control data.
Impaired P1 generation has been repeatedly shown in patients with schizophrenia receiving medication,16-18,20,43,44 a result that has been replicated here. However, until now, it had not been assessed in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives. To our knowledge, this study is the first to show that clinically unaffected first-degree relatives also have significant impairments in early visual processing as indexed by the P1. The observed decrement in P1 generation is found in the absence of any age, sex, or medication effects, strongly suggesting that it is associated with genetic risk for schizophrenia. In addition, the decrement in P1 amplitude for this group is not at all subtle but rather is found to have a large effect size. As such, the P1 deficit appears to be a very promising candidate as an endophenotypic marker for the disorder.
The deficit is seen most strongly over more midline parieto-occipital scalp compared with more lateral occipito-temporal regions, a finding that is highly consistent with our previous studies in chronic patients.16,17,20 The early timing of this deficit, its general scalp topography over midline and dorsal occipital scalp, and inverse source-estimation all support the possibility of an early visual processing deficit that is more prominent in midline and dorsal regions of the visual system, as has been suggested by previous psychophysical18,43,45-48 and neurophysiological research.16,17,20
These data also add weight to the thesis that early visual processing deficits in schizophrenia may be biased to the M system more than the P system,16,18,49 insofar as the deficit appears mainly over midline and dorsal stream structures, which are known to receive their main cellular input from the M system.50 Of course, in the present study, we used a relatively high spatial frequency and high luminance contrast (64%) stimulus that will have activated both the M and P systems. In previous work in patients, however, stimuli that specifically activated one or the other cellular system have been employed and much of this work points to greater M system dysfunction. For example, in studies of patients with chronic schizophrenia, Butler and colleagues18,19 showed greater deficits in steady-state VEPs (ssVEPs) elicited by M-biased stimuli than by P-biased stimuli. Similarly, Foxe and colleagues17 found that early ventral stream processing appeared largely intact as evidenced by normal illusory contour processing in the lateral occipital complex despite large-scale deficits in dorsal P1 generation, suggesting relative preservation of P-mediated processing. There is also a body of work that has looked at patient performance on visual tasks that selectively bias the M and P pathways, and again these studies point to greater M pathway dysfunction.51-53 For example, patients with schizophrenia showed a larger deficit in letter discrimination compared with controls for letter stimuli composed of fast-moving dots than for stimuli composed of slow-moving dots. This deficit in high-velocity stimulus detection in patients was interpreted as indicative of a specific dysfunction in the M system, which is known to be essential for processing such stimuli. Although the issue of P vs M functioning has been well studied in patients, an obvious next step in studying first-degree relatives will be to use M and P biasing stimuli54 in conjunction with VEP recordings.
A major advantage of the P1 as a potential diagnostic tool is the relative ease with which a sufficiently clean and stable recording can be obtained and the fact that it is readily identifiable across individuals. In particular, because it is a largely automatic or exogenous response that doesn't necessarily require performance of an active task to obtain, it is not nearly as susceptible to motivational issues or the ongoing clinical state of the patient as later cognitive components such as the P300 can be.4,5 Nonetheless, while the P1 is largely automatic in that it is present even when subjects only passively view the stimulus, this is not to say that it is not cognitively penetrable. Most notably, modulations of P1 amplitude occur as a result of deployments of visual spatial attention and are thought to reflect facilitation of early sensory processing.55-58 The notion that the P1 deficit observed in the present study is a result of impaired spatial attention is rendered extremely unlikely, however, in light of the equivalent performance across groups, each demonstrating high target detection rates. This indicates that the groups did not differ in relation to fixation on, and attention to, the centrally presented stimuli. Moreover, it has been shown that, in contrast to peripheral stimuli, the P1 elicited by centrally presented stimuli, as was the case here, is not modulated by directing attention to or away from that location; ie, the mechanisms of sensory gain invoked by spatial attention do not appear to apply to foveal space.59 Similar results were observed in a recent intersensory attention study.60 Subjects attended to a completely different sensory modality (auditory stimuli) on which they performed a very taxing task and were required to actively ignore concurrent visual distracter stimuli, and yet still no effect of attention was seen on the visual P1 component. Although the present results cannot be explained on the basis of differential attentional deployment across groups, the deficit in P1 in both patients and relatives may very well be related to more general deficits in attention that are found in patients.61 For instance, it is plausible that dorsal P1 represents activation of the exogenous attentional orienting system, a critical function of the dorsal visual stream, and that this might explain some of the deficits in attentional orienting that have been reported in patients.62,63 However, recent work has also suggested that attentional functions in schizophrenia may not be particularly more impaired than any other cognitive function.64 For now, a relationship between P1 deficits and any attentional dysfunction remains to be explicitly tested in both patients and relatives.
Post hoc analyses also uncovered a second phase of differential activity (120-150 milliseconds) between groups in the time frame immediately following the initial phase of the P1 (80-100 milliseconds). In this second phase, it was the relatives who showed higher amplitude. Source analysis suggested that this second phase represented re-entrant feedback processing in striate and neighboring extrastriate regions. This re-entrant activity appears to be almost absent in probands. One possible explanation for this re-entrant activity in relatives is that it represents compensatory mechanisms to increase activation in early visual areas following an initial weak response. Clearly, this effect was not part of our original hypotheses, and although robust, it will need to be confirmed in future work.
Ultimately, a major goal in schizophrenia research is to derive measures to detect schizophrenia before the actual onset of prodromal or full-blown psychotic symptoms. To this end, there is evidence to show that the mild subclinical neuropsychological problems that are often reported in nonpsychotic first-degree relatives are reversible by low-dose antipsychotic medications.65 The development of an easily measured, objective electrophysiological phenotype would be of great use in these efforts, and the findings of this study suggest that the visual P1 deficit is a very promising candidate.
The finding of substantially reduced P1 amplitude in clinically unaffected first-degree relatives of patients gives considerable weight to the possibility of using the P1 deficit as an endophenotypic marker or diagnostic tool for schizophrenia. The deficit is more pronounced over the midline parieto-occipital scalp, suggesting that the impairment lies mostly in midline and dorsal visual stream structures, a contention that is supported by inverse source analyses. Further longitudinal studies will be required to determine the integrity of the P1 across all age groups and chronicity. Ultimately, it is the early detection of schizophrenia in high-risk individuals that will enable us to treat and better manage this debilitating disorder.
Correspondence: John J. Foxe, PhD, Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, 140 Old Orangeburg Rd, Orangeburg, NY 10962 (foxe@nki.rfmh.org).
Submitted for Publication: November 1, 2005; final revision received January 26, 2006; accepted March 3, 2006.
Author Contributions: Dr Foxe takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis and attests that all authors had full access to all the data in the study.
Financial Disclosure: None reported.
Funding/Support: Merck Research Labs provided generous support for the Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory at St Vincent's Hospital in Fairview, Dublin, Ireland. This work was also supported in part by grants MH65350 (J.J.F.) and MH49334 (D.C.J.) from the National Institute of Mental Health. Additional salary support (S.Y.) came from a grant from the Irish Higher Education Authority (HEA-Ireland) to our colleague, Dr Richard Reilly, at University College Dublin.
Acknowledgment: We are indebted to Sophie Molholm, PhD, who provided valuable editorial comments on an earlier draft of this manuscript.
1.Harrison
PJWeinberger
DR Schizophrenia genes, gene expression, and neuropathology: on the matter of their convergence.
Mol Psychiatry 2005;1040- 68
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 2.Almasy
LBlangero
J Endophenotypes as quantitative risk factors for psychiatric disease: rationale and study design.
Am J Med Genet 2001;10542- 44
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 3.Gottesman
IIGould
TD The endophenotype concept in psychiatry: etymology and strategic intentions.
Am J Psychiatry 2003;160636- 645
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 4.Johnson
SCLowery
NKohler
CTuretsky
BI Global-local visual processing in schizophrenia: evidence for an early visual processing deficit.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;58937- 946
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 5.Schechter
IButler
PDSilipo
GZemon
VJavitt
DC Magnocellular and parvocellular contributions to backward masking dysfunction in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res 2003;6491- 101
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 6.Mathalon
DHFord
JMPfefferbaum
A Trait and state aspects of P300 amplitude reduction in schizophrenia: a retrospective longitudinal study.
Biol Psychiatry 2000;47434- 449
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 7.Javitt
DCShelley
AMSilipo
GLieberman
JA Deficits in auditory and visual context-dependent processing in schizophrenia: defining the pattern.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;571131- 1137
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 8.Shelley
AMSilipo
GJavitt
DC Diminished responsiveness of ERPs in schizophrenic subjects to changes in auditory stimulation parameters: implications for theories of cortical dysfunction.
Schizophr Res 1999;3765- 79
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 9.Clementz
BAGeyer
MABraff
DL Poor P50 suppression among schizophrenia patients and their first-degree biological relatives.
Am J Psychiatry 1998;1551691- 1694
PubMedGoogle Scholar 10.Winterer
GCoppola
REgan
MFGoldberg
TEWeinberger
DR Functional and effective frontotemporal connectivity and genetic risk for schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry 2003;541181- 1192
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 11.Winterer
GEgan
MFRaedler
TSanchez
CJones
DWCoppola
RWeinberger
DR P300 and genetic risk for schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2003;601158- 1167
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 12.Myles-Worsley
M P50 sensory gating in multiplex schizophrenia families from a Pacific Island isolate.
Am J Psychiatry 2002;1592007- 2012
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 13.Myles-Worsley
MOrd
LBlailes
FNgiralmau
HFreedman
R P50 sensory gating in adolescents from a Pacific Island isolate with elevated risk for schizophrenia.
Biol Psychiatry 2004;55663- 667
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 14.Cadenhead
KSLight
GAShafer
KMBraff
DL P50 suppression in individuals at risk for schizophrenia: the convergence of clinical, familial, and vulnerability marker risk assessment.
Biol Psychiatry 2005;571504- 1509
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 15.Bramon
EMcDonald
CCroft
RJLandau
SFilbey
FGruzelier
JHSham
PCFrangou
SMurray
RM Is the P300 wave an endophenotype for schizophrenia? a meta-analysis and a family study.
Neuroimage 2005;27960- 968
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 16.Foxe
JJDoniger
GMJavitt
DC Early visual processing deficits in schizophrenia: impaired P1 generation revealed by high-density electrical mapping.
Neuroreport 2001;123815- 3820
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 17.Foxe
JJMurray
MMJavitt
DC Filling-in in schizophrenia: a high-density electrical mapping and source-analysis investigation of illusory contour processing.
Cereb Cortex 2005;151914- 1927
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 18.Butler
PDSchechter
IZemon
VSchwartz
SGGreenstein
VCGordon
JSchroeder
CEJavitt
DC Dysfunction of early-stage visual processing in schizophrenia.
Am J Psychiatry 2001;1581126- 1133
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 19.Butler
PDZemon
VSchechter
ISaperstein
AMHoptman
MJLim
KORevheim
NSilipo
GJavitt
DC Early-stage visual processing and cortical amplification deficits in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62495- 504
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 20.Doniger
GMFoxe
JJMurray
MMHiggins
BAJavitt
DC Impaired visual object recognition and dorsal/ventral stream interaction in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002;591011- 1020
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 21.Schechter
IButler
PDZemon
VMRevheim
NSaperstein
AMJalbrzikowski
MPasternak
RSilipo
GJavitt
DC Impairments in generation of early-stage transient visual evoked potentials to magno- and parvocellular-selective stimuli in schizophrenia.
Clin Neurophysiol 2005;1162204- 2215
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 22.Spencer
KMNestor
PGNiznikiewicz
MASalisbury
DFShenton
MEMcCarley
RW Abnormal neural synchrony in schizophrenia.
J Neurosci 2003;237407- 7411
PubMedGoogle Scholar 23.Foxe
JJSimpson
GV Flow of activation from V1 to frontal cortex in humans: a framework for defining “early” visual processing.
Exp Brain Res 2002;142139- 150
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 24.Liu
ZTam
WCXue
ZYao
SWu
D Positive and negative symptom profile schizophrenia and abnormalities in the P300 component of the event-related potential: a longitudinal controlled study.
Psychiatry Res 2004;132131- 139
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 25.Wood
SMPotts
GFHall
JFUlanday
JBNetsiri
C Event-related potentials to auditory and visual selective attention in schizophrenia.
Int J Psychophysiol 2006;6067- 75
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 26.Faraone
SVSeidman
LJKremen
WSToomey
RPepple
JRTsuang
MT Neuropsychologic functioning among the nonpsychotic relatives of schizophrenic patients: the effect of genetic loading.
Biol Psychiatry 2000;48120- 126
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 27.Moldin
SOGottesman
II At issue: genes, experience, and chance in schizophrenia: positioning for the 21st century.
Schizophr Bull 1997;23547- 561
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 28.Tsuang
MTStone
WSTarbox
SIFaraone
SV Validating schizotaxia and its place in studies of preventing schizophrenia by psychopharmacological intervention.
Essent Psychopharmacol 2005;691- 103
PubMedGoogle Scholar 29.Snitz
BEMacdonald
AW
IIICarter
CS Cognitive deficits in unaffected first-degree relatives of schizophrenia patients: a meta-analytic review of putative endophenotypes.
Schizophr Bull 2006;32179- 194
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 30.Andreasen
NC Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City Dept of Psychiatry, University of Iowa
33.Butler
PDHarkavy-Friedman
JMAmador
XFGorman
JM Backward masking in schizophrenia: relationship to medication status, neuropsychological functioning, and dopamine metabolism.
Biol Psychiatry 1996;40295- 298
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 34.Spohn
HELacoursiere
RBThompson
KCoyne
L Phenothiazine effects on psychological and psychophysiological dysfunction in chronic schizophrenics.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1977;34633- 644
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 35.Spitzer
RLWilliams
JBGibbon
MFirst
MB The Structured Clinical Interview for
DSM-III-R (SCID), I: history, rationale, and description.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1992;49624- 629
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 36.Page
JWCrognale
MA Differential aging of chromatic and achromatic visual pathways: behavior and electrophysiology.
Vision Res 2005;451481- 1489
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 37.Molholm
SRitter
WMurray
MMJavitt
DCSchroeder
CEFoxe
JJ Multisensory auditory-visual interactions during early sensory processing in humans: a high-density electrical mapping study.
Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 2002;14115- 128
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 39.Grave de Peralta Menedez
RGonzalez
ASLantz
GMichel
CMLandis
T Noninvasive localization of electromagnetic epileptic activity, I: method descriptions and simulations.
Brain Topogr 2001;14131- 137
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 40.Sehatpour
PMolholm
SJavitt
DCFoxe
JJ Spatiotemporal dynamics of human object recognition processing: an integrated high-density electrical mapping and functional imaging study of “closure” processes.
Neuroimage 2006;29605- 618
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 41.Gottesman
IIErlenmeyer-Kimling
L Family and twin strategies as a head start in defining prodromes and endophenotypes for hypothetical early-interventions in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res 2001;5193- 102
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 42.Cohen
J Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. 2nd ed. Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers1988;8- 13
43.Basinska
A Altered electrophysiological pattern of target detection in schizophrenia in the Continuous Attention Test.
Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars) 1998;58207- 220
PubMedGoogle Scholar 44.Romani
AZerbi
FMariotti
GCallieco
RCosi
V Computed tomography and pattern reversal visual evoked potentials in chronic schizophrenic patients.
Acta Psychiatr Scand 1986;73566- 573
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 45.Chen
YPalafox
GPNakayama
KLevy
DLMatthysse
SHolzman
PS Motion perception in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999;56149- 154
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 46.Chen
YLevy
DLNakayama
KMatthysse
SPalafox
GHolzman
PS Dependence of impaired eye tracking on deficient velocity discrimination in schizophrenia.
Arch Gen Psychiatry 1999;56155- 161
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 47.Green
MFNuechterlein
KHBreitmeyer
BMintz
J Backward masking in unmedicated schizophrenic patients in psychotic remission: possible reflection of aberrant cortical oscillation.
Am J Psychiatry 1999;1561367- 1373
PubMedGoogle Scholar 48.Schwartz
BDEvans
WJ Neurophysiological mechanisms of attention deficits in schizophrenia.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1999;12207- 220
PubMedGoogle Scholar 49.Schwartz
BDTomlin
HREvans
WJRoss
KV Neurophysiologic mechanisms of attention: a selective review of early information processing in schizophrenics.
Front Biosci 2001;6D120- D134
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 51.Schwartz
BDMaron
BAEvans
WJWinstead
DK Smooth pursuit tracking deficits of patients with schizophrenia at specific within-sine wave bins.
Neuropsychiatry Neuropsychol Behav Neurol 1999;12221- 229
PubMedGoogle Scholar 52.Cadenhead
KSSerper
YBraff
DL Transient versus sustained visual channels in the visual backward masking deficits of schizophrenia patients.
Biol Psychiatry 1998;43132- 138
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 53.Keri
SKelemen
OJanka
ZBenedek
G Visual-perceptual dysfunctions are possible endophenotypes of schizophrenia: evidence from the psychophysical investigation of magnocellular and parvocellular pathways.
Neuropsychology 2005;19649- 656
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 54.Bedwell
JSBrown
JMMiller
LS The magnocellular visual system and schizophrenia: what can the color red tell us?
Schizophr Res 2003;63273- 284
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 55.Rugg
MDMilner
ADLines
CRPhalp
R Modulation of visual event-related potentials by spatial and non-spatial visual selective attention.
Neuropsychologia 1987;2585- 96
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 56.Heinze
HJLuck
SJMangun
GRHillyard
SA Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays, I: evidence for early selection.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990;75511- 527
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 57.Luck
SJHeinze
HJMangun
GRHillyard
SA Visual event-related potentials index focused attention within bilateral stimulus arrays, II: functional dissociation of P1 and N1 components.
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1990;75528- 542
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 58.Martinez
AAnllo-Vento
LSereno
MIFrank
LRBuxton
RBDubowitz
DJWong
ECHinrichs
HHeinze
HJHillyard
SA Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention.
Nat Neurosci 1999;2364- 369
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 60.Foxe
JJSimpson
GV Biasing the brain's attentional set, II: effects of selective intersensory attentional deployments on subsequent sensory processing.
Exp Brain Res 2005;166393- 401
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 62.Laurens
KRKiehl
KANgan
ETLiddle
PF Attention orienting dysfunction during salient novel stimulus processing in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res 2005;75159- 171
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 63.Wang
KFan
JDong
YWang
CQLee
TMPosner
MI Selective impairment of attentional networks of orienting and executive control in schizophrenia.
Schizophr Res 2005;78235- 241
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref 65.Tsuang
MTStone
WSTarbox
SIFaraone
SV Treatment of nonpsychotic relatives of patients with schizophrenia: six case studies.
Am J Med Genet 2002;114943- 948
PubMedGoogle ScholarCrossref